Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T18:31:31.421Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cannabis use and cognitive biases in people with first-episode psychosis and their siblings

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 November 2024

L. Roldan*
Affiliation:
Department of Personality, Assessment and Clinical Psychology, School of Psychology, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Spain
T. Sánchez-Gutiérrez
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Córdoba, Spain
I. Fernández-Arias
Affiliation:
Department of Personality, Assessment and Clinical Psychology, School of Psychology, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Spain
E. Rodríguez-Toscano
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, Cognitive processes and Speech therapy, School of Psychology, Universidad Complutense de Madrid (UCM); Madrid, Spain
G. López
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry and Mental Health, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón, IiSGM, CIBERSAM, School of Medicine, Universidad Complutense, Madrid, Spain
J. Merchán-Naranjo
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry and Mental Health, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón, IiSGM, CIBERSAM, School of Medicine, Universidad Complutense, Madrid, Spain
A. Calvo
Affiliation:
Department of Personality, Assessment and Clinical Psychology, School of Psychology, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Spain
M. Rapado-Castro
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry and Mental Health, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón, IiSGM, CIBERSAM, School of Medicine, Universidad Complutense, Madrid, Spain Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre, Department of Psychiatry, The University of Melbourne and Melbourne Health, 161 Barry Street, Carlton South, Victoria 3053, Australia
M. Parellada
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry and Mental Health, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón, IiSGM, CIBERSAM, School of Medicine, Universidad Complutense, Madrid, Spain
C. Moreno
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry and Mental Health, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón, IiSGM, CIBERSAM, School of Medicine, Universidad Complutense, Madrid, Spain
L. Ferraro
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedicine, Neuroscience and Advanced Diagnostics (BiND), Psychiatry Section, University of Palermo, Palermo, Italy
D. La Barbera
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedicine, Neuroscience and Advanced Diagnostics (BiND), Psychiatry Section, University of Palermo, Palermo, Italy
C. La Cascia
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedicine, Neuroscience and Advanced Diagnostics (BiND), Psychiatry Section, University of Palermo, Palermo, Italy
G. Tripoli
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedicine, Neuroscience and Advanced Diagnostics (BiND), Psychiatry Section, University of Palermo, Palermo, Italy
M. Di Forti
Affiliation:
Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, England ESRC Centre for Society and Mental Health, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
R.M. Murray
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
D. Quattrone
Affiliation:
Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, England
C. Morgan
Affiliation:
ESRC Centre for Society and Mental Health, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK Department of Health Services and Population Research, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, England
C. Gayer-Anderson
Affiliation:
Department of Health Services and Population Research, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, England
P. B. Jones
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge; 24CAMEO Early Intervention Service, Cambridgeshire & Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge, CB21 5EF, UK
H.E. Jongsma
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge; 24CAMEO Early Intervention Service, Cambridgeshire & Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge, CB21 5EF, UK
J.B. Kirkbride
Affiliation:
Psylife Group, Division of Psychiatry, University College London, 6th Floor, Maple House, 149 Tottenham Court Road, London W1T 7NF, UK
J. van Os
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, Maastricht University Medical Center, Maastricht, Netherlands Department Psychiatry, Brain Centre Rudolf Magnus, Utrecht University Medical Centre, Utrecht, The Netherlands
P. García-Portilla
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine -Psychiatry, Universidad de Oviedo, ISPA, INEUROPA, CIBERSAM, Oviedo, Spain
S. Al-Halabí
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Oviedo, Spain
J. Bobes
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine -Psychiatry, Universidad de Oviedo, ISPA, INEUROPA, CIBERSAM, Oviedo, Spain
L. de Haan
Affiliation:
Amsterdam UMC, University of Amsterdam, Academic Psychiatric Centre, Early Psychosis Department, Arkin, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
M. Bernardo
Affiliation:
Barcelona Clinic Schizophrenia Unit, Hospital Clinic, Departament de Medicina, Institut de Neurociències (UBNeuro), Universitat de Barcelona (UB), Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi I Sunyer (IDIBAPS), CIBERSAM, ISCIII, Barcelona, Spain
J.L. Santos
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Servicio de Psiquiatría Hospital “Virgen de la Luz,” Cuenca, Spain
J. Sanjuán
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Hospital Clínico Universitario de Valencia, INCLIVA, CIBERSAM, School of Medicine, Universidad de Valencia, Valencia, Spain
M. Arrojo
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Psychiatric Genetic Group, Instituto de Investigación Sanitaria de Santiago de Compostela, Complejo Hospitalario Universitario de Santiago de Compostela, Santiago de Compostela, Spain
A. Szoke
Affiliation:
Univ Paris Est Creteil, INSERM, IMRB, Fondation FondaMental, F-94010 Creteil, France AP-HP, Hopitaux Universitaires “ H. Mondor ”, DMU IMPACT, F-94010 Creteil, France
B.P. Rutten
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, Maastricht University Medical Center, Maastricht, Netherlands
S. A. Stilo
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK Department of Mental Health and Addiction Services, ASP Crotone, Crotone, Italy
I. Tarricone
Affiliation:
Bologna Transcultural Psychosomatic Team (BoTPT), Department of Medical and Surgical Sciences, Alma Mater Studiorum University of Bologna
A. Lasalvia
Affiliation:
Section of Psychiatry, Azienda Ospedaliera Universitaria Integrata di Verona, Verona, Italy
S. Tosato
Affiliation:
Université Clermont Auvergne, EA 7280, Clermont-Ferrand 63000, France
P.-M. Llorca
Affiliation:
Université Clermont Auvergne, EA 7280, Clermont-Ferrand 63000, France
P. Rossi Menezes
Affiliation:
Department of Preventive Medicine, Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
J-P Selten
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, Maastricht University Medical Center, Maastricht, Netherlands
A. Tortelli
Affiliation:
Etablissement Public de Santé Maison Blanche, Paris 75020, France
E. Velthorst
Affiliation:
Mental Health Service Organization ‘GGZ Noord-Holland-Noord’, Department of Research, the Netherlands
C.M. Del-Ben
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, Department of Neuroscience and Behavior, Ribeirão Preto Medical School, University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
C. Arango*
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry and Mental Health, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón, IiSGM, CIBERSAM, School of Medicine, Universidad Complutense, Madrid, Spain
C. M. Díaz-Caneja*
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry and Mental Health, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón, IiSGM, CIBERSAM, School of Medicine, Universidad Complutense, Madrid, Spain
*
Corresponding author: L. Roldan; Email: laroldan@ucm.es; C. Arango; Email: carango@hggm.es; C. M. Díaz-Caneja; Email: covadonga.martinez@iisgm.com
Corresponding author: L. Roldan; Email: laroldan@ucm.es; C. Arango; Email: carango@hggm.es; C. M. Díaz-Caneja; Email: covadonga.martinez@iisgm.com
Corresponding author: L. Roldan; Email: laroldan@ucm.es; C. Arango; Email: carango@hggm.es; C. M. Díaz-Caneja; Email: covadonga.martinez@iisgm.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Cannabis use and familial vulnerability to psychosis have been associated with social cognition deficits. This study examined the potential relationship between cannabis use and cognitive biases underlying social cognition and functioning in patients with first episode psychosis (FEP), their siblings, and controls.

Methods

We analyzed a sample of 543 participants with FEP, 203 siblings, and 1168 controls from the EU-GEI study using a correlational design. We used logistic regression analyses to examine the influence of clinical group, lifetime cannabis use frequency, and potency of cannabis use on cognitive biases, accounting for demographic and cognitive variables.

Results

FEP patients showed increased odds of facial recognition processing (FRP) deficits (OR = 1.642, CI 1.123–2.402) relative to controls but not of speech illusions (SI) or jumping to conclusions (JTC) bias, with no statistically significant differences relative to siblings. Daily and occasional lifetime cannabis use were associated with decreased odds of SI (OR = 0.605, CI 0.368–0.997 and OR = 0.646, CI 0.457–0.913 respectively) and JTC bias (OR = 0.625, CI 0.422–0.925 and OR = 0.602, CI 0.460–0.787 respectively) compared with lifetime abstinence, but not with FRP deficits, in the whole sample. Within the cannabis user group, low-potency cannabis use was associated with increased odds of SI (OR = 1.829, CI 1.297–2.578, FRP deficits (OR = 1.393, CI 1.031–1.882, and JTC (OR = 1.661, CI 1.271–2.171) relative to high-potency cannabis use, with comparable effects in the three clinical groups.

Conclusions

Our findings suggest increased odds of cognitive biases in FEP patients who have never used cannabis and in low-potency users. Future studies should elucidate this association and its potential implications.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Background

Cannabis use can increase the risk of psychosis (Di Forti et al., Reference Di Forti, Marconi, Carra, Fraietta, Trotta, Bonomo and Murray2015; Di Forti et al., Reference Di Forti, Quattrone, Freeman, Tripoli, Gayer-Anderson and Quigley2019; Karpov, Lindgren, Kieseppa, Wegelius, & Suvisaari, Reference Karpov, Lindgren, Kieseppa, Wegelius and Suvisaari2021; Luzi, Morrison, Powell, di Forti, & Murray, Reference Luzi, Morrison, Powell, di Forti and Murray2008; Murray, Paparelli, Morrison, Marconi, & Di Forti, Reference Murray, Paparelli, Morrison, Marconi and Di Forti2013) and its prevalence is higher in patients diagnosed with schizophrenia (Bersani, Orlandi, Kotzalidis, & Pancheri, Reference Bersani, Orlandi, Kotzalidis and Pancheri2002; Hartz et al., Reference Hartz, Pato, Medeiros, Cavazos-Rehg, Sobell and Knowles2014), bipolar disorder, depressive and anxiety disorders, and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Koenfal, Gabrys, & Porath, Reference Koenfal, Gabrys and Porath2019; Lev-Ran, Le Foll, McKenzie, George, & Rehm, Reference Lev-Ran, Le Foll, McKenzie, George and Rehm2013) than in healthy controls.

Some cognitive and reasoning biases have been reported across the psychosis continuum including people with first episode psychosis (FEP), chronic psychosis, clinical and familial high risk for psychosis, and non-clinical samples with psychotic-like experiences (Henquet et al., Reference Henquet, van Os, Pries, Rauschenberg, Delespaul, Kenis and Guloksuz2022; Langdon, Still, Connors, Ward, & Catts, Reference Langdon, Still, Connors, Ward and Catts2014; Linney, Peters, & Ayton, Reference Linney, Peters and Ayton1998; Moritz, Van Quaquebeke, & Lincoln, Reference Moritz, Van Quaquebeke and Lincoln2012; Van Dael et al., Reference Van Dael, Versmissen, Janssen, Myin-Germeys, van Os and Krabbendam2006). These cognitive biases could increase psychosis risk by affecting the perception of others, decision-making about social situations, the meaning attributed to certain social stimuli, and real-life functioning (Green, Horan, & Lee, Reference Green, Horan and Lee2019; Mucci et al., Reference Mucci, Galderisi, Gibertoni, Rossi, Rocca and Bertolino2021).

Aberrant salience is the unusual or incorrect assignment of meaning to neutral stimuli, which can favor the development of attentional biases that may in turn lead to the perception that the environment is dangerous and to the development of paranoid ideation (Howes & Murray, Reference Howes and Murray2014; Kapur, Reference Kapur2003). Experimental illusion studies have focused on the paradigm of hearing voices in neutral random signals (white noise) in the absence of actual speech (speech illusion, SI) (Galdos et al., Reference Galdos, Simons, Fernandez-Rivas, Wichers, Peralta, Lataster and van Os2011).

Other cognitive biases have also consistently been reported in patients with schizophrenia (Hofer, Biedermann, Yalcin, & Fleischhacker, Reference Hofer, Biedermann, Yalcin and Fleischhacker2010; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Altshuler, Glahn, Miklowitz, Ochsner and Green2013), such as the jumping to conclusion (JTC) reasoning bias: under conditions of uncertainty, people with delusions use less information to arrive at a decision and express greater confidence in their judgment than controls (Dudley, Taylor, Wickham, & Hutton, Reference Dudley, Taylor, Wickham and Hutton2016; Garety & Freeman, Reference Garety and Freeman2013; Lincoln, Ziegler, Mehl, & Rief, Reference Lincoln, Ziegler, Mehl and Rief2010; Murray et al., Reference Murray, Mondelli, Stilo, Trotta, Sideli, Ajnakina and Di Forti2020; So, Siu, Wong, Chan, & Garety, Reference So, Siu, Wong, Chan and Garety2016; Tripoli et al., Reference Tripoli, Quattrone, Ferraro, Gayer-Anderson, Rodriguez, La Cascia and Di Forti2021).

Also, deficits in facial recognition processing (discrimination, encoding, and recognition) have been found in people with schizophrenia (Archer, Hay, & Young, Reference Archer, Hay and Young1992; Shin et al., Reference Shin, Na, Ha, Kang, Yoo and Kwon2008; van 't Wout, Aleman, Kessels, Laroi, & Kahn, Reference van 't Wout, Aleman, Kessels, Laroi and Kahn2004; Walther et al., Reference Walther, Federspiel, Horn, Bianchi, Wiest, Wirth and Muller2009), with milder degrees of facial recognition impairments observed in non-affected first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia (Li, Chan, Zhao, Hong, & Gong, Reference Li, Chan, Zhao, Hong and Gong2010), thus suggesting that this could be a potential endophenotypic marker of psychosis (Fusar-Poli et al., Reference Fusar-Poli, Pries, van Os, Erzin, Delespaul, Kenis and Guloksuz2022a; Soria Bauser et al., Reference Soria Bauser, Thoma, Aizenberg, Brune, Juckel and Daum2012; Tripoli et al., Reference Tripoli, Quattrone, Ferraro, Gayer-Anderson, La Cascia, La Barbera and Murray2022).

Despite consistent evidence supporting the presence of impairments across cognition and social cognition in schizophrenia (Green et al., Reference Green, Horan and Lee2019; Velthorst et al., Reference Velthorst, Fett, Reichenberg, Perlman, van Os, Bromet and Kotov2017), the effect of cannabis use on cognitive and social cognitive functioning in patients with FEP and their siblings is still controversial (Arnold, Allott, Farhall, Killackey, & Cotton, Reference Arnold, Allott, Farhall, Killackey and Cotton2015; Bruins, Pijnenborg, investigators, Visser, & Castelein, Reference Bruins, Pijnenborg, Visser and Castelein2021; Clausen et al., Reference Clausen, Hjorthoj, Thorup, Jeppesen, Petersen, Bertelsen and Nordentoft2014; Meijer et al., Reference Meijer, Dekker, Koeter, Quee, van Beveren and Meijer2012). Most studies report better or similar neuropsychological functioning in people with schizophrenia and FEP that use cannabis than in those who do not (Rabin, Zakzanis, & George, Reference Rabin, Zakzanis and George2011; Wobrock et al., Reference Wobrock, Falkai, Schneider-Axmann, Hasan, Galderisi and Davidson2013; Yucel et al., Reference Yucel, Bora, Lubman, Solowij, Brewer, Cotton and Pantelis2012). A recent study in the EU-GEI sample using the degraded facial affect recognition task reported better facial emotion recognition processing in cannabis users with schizophrenia, their siblings, and healthy controls, both for total scores and for specific emotion recognition (neutral, happy, fearful, and angry) relative to non-users (Fusar-Poli et al., Reference Fusar-Poli, Pries, van Os, Radhakrishnan, Pence, Erzin and Guloksuz2022b). However, other studies have observed poorer cognitive performance (D'Souza et al., Reference D'Souza, Abi-Saab, Madonick, Forselius-Bielen, Doersch, Braley and Krystal2005; Mata et al., Reference Mata, Rodriguez-Sanchez, Pelayo-Teran, Perez-Iglesias, Gonzalez-Blanch, Ramirez-Bonilla and Crespo-Facorro2008) or no differences in some cognitive tasks in people with schizophrenia using cannabis (Ahuir et al., Reference Ahuir, Crosas, Estrada, Zabala, Perez-Munoz, Gonzalez-Fernandez and Labad2021; Jockers-Scherubl et al., Reference Jockers-Scherubl, Wolf, Radzei, Schlattmann, Rentzsch, Gomez-Carrillo de Castro and Kuhl2007; Sevy et al., Reference Sevy, Burdick, Visweswaraiah, Abdelmessih, Lukin, Yechiam and Bechara2007).

As social skills may be necessary for patients to acquire and use some substances, especially in context where access is more restricted, some researchers suggest that cannabis use in people with FEP could correlate with better social abilities, more drug taking opportunities, and better neurocognitive functioning (Arnold et al., Reference Arnold, Allott, Farhall, Killackey and Cotton2015; Menendez-Miranda et al., Reference Menendez-Miranda, Garcia-Alvarez, Garcia-Portilla, Gonzalez-Blanco, Saiz and Bobes2019; Rodriguez-Sanchez et al., Reference Rodriguez-Sanchez, Ayesa-Arriola, Mata, Moreno-Calle, Perez-Iglesias, Gonzalez-Blanch and Crespo-Facorro2010; Yucel et al., Reference Yucel, Bora, Lubman, Solowij, Brewer, Cotton and Pantelis2012). Indeed, previous research in the EU-GEI sample suggests that better premorbid social and cognitive functioning could contribute to the likelihood of beginning to use cannabis before psychosis onset in patients with FEP (Ferraro et al., Reference Ferraro, La Cascia, Quattrone, Sideli, Matranga, Capuccio and Di Forti2020).

In this study, we sought to examine for the first time the relationship between cannabis use and speech illusions, facial recognition processing deficits, and jumping to conclusions in patients with FEP, their non-psychotic siblings, and controls. These biases may underlie processes in social cognitive deficits in schizophrenia related with positive symptoms of schizophrenia such as persecutory delusions and auditory hallucinations. Following previous studies, we hypothesized that lifetime cannabis use would correlate with lower odds of cognitive biases relative to lifetime abstinence. As secondary objectives, we aimed to explore the association of frequency of cannabis use and cannabis potency with the odds of cognitive biases in cannabis users.

Methods

Study design

Members of a large, international, multisite, observational study, The European Network of National Schizophrenia Networks Studying Gene-Environment Interactions (EU-GEI), recruited participants between May 2010 and April 2015 from 17 catchment areas in 6 countries (Brazil, France, Italy, the Netherlands, Spain, and the United Kingdom). Study goals include analyzing the effects of genetic, environmental, and clinical variables and their interaction on the development, severity, and outcome of schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders. The EU-GEI team recruited a subset of people with FEP for a concurrent case-control study and controls from the same catchment areas. Additionally, they recruited patients' siblings to focus on the role of gene-environment interaction of the vulnerability and severity of psychosis in a family-based setting. Detailed study procedures are available in (Gayer-Anderson et al., Reference Gayer-Anderson, Jongsma, Di Forti, Quattrone, Velthorst, de Haan and Morgan2020; Jongsma et al., Reference Jongsma, Gayer-Anderson, Lasalvia, Quattrone, Mule and Szoke2018).

Participants

The EU-GEI study sample included 1130 participants with FEP who attended mental health services in the catchment areas. Inclusion criteria for patients were: (1) diagnosis of non-organic psychotic disorder, (2) 18–64 years of age, (3) resident within one of the study catchment areas at the time of their first presentation. Exclusion criteria included: (1) presence of psychotic symptoms due to acute intoxication (ICD10: F1X.5) or organic psychosis (ICD10: F09), (2) previous contact with mental health services because of psychotic symptoms outside of the study period.

We also recruited siblings of FEP participants, aged 18 years or older (N = 265) via communications with the patient and/or his or her reference clinician. We excluded siblings if they presented current or past psychotic disorders (including a psychosis diagnosis within the time frame of the study) or if they had received treatment with antipsychotic medication.

We recruited volunteers from the same catchment areas and same age range as patients for the control sample by using a mixture of random and quota sampling to maximize the representativeness of samples in each catchment area. A total of 1497 controls agreed to participate in the study. We excluded controls if they had received psychotic disorder diagnoses, including during the study, or had undergone treatment with antipsychotic medication. More information about the recruitment procedures is available elsewhere (Di Forti et al., Reference Di Forti, Quattrone, Freeman, Tripoli, Gayer-Anderson and Quigley2019; Gayer-Anderson et al., Reference Gayer-Anderson, Jongsma, Di Forti, Quattrone, Velthorst, de Haan and Morgan2020; van Os et al., Reference van Os, Rutten, Myin-Germeys, Delespaul, Viechtbauer, van Zelst and Mirjanic2014).

For the purposes of this study, we included participants that had complete data on cannabis use and complete assessments of each of the three cognitive biases. This yielded a final sample size of 543 participants with FEP, 203 siblings, and 1168 controls (see online Supplementary Figure 1).

Local ethical committees of all sites approved the study at all the study sites. All participants gave written informed consent before entering the study. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Measures

Demographic and clinical measures

We used the modified version of the Medical Research Council (MRC) socio-demographic scale to collect socio-demographic data (Mallett, Leff, Bhugra, Pang, & Zhao, Reference Mallett, Leff, Bhugra, Pang and Zhao2002). Diagnoses were operationalized through the 90-item computerized Operational CRITeria (OPCRIT) system for psychosis (McGuffin, Farmer, & Harvey, Reference McGuffin, Farmer and Harvey1991; Williams, Farmer, Ackenheil, Kaufmann, & McGuffin, Reference Williams, Farmer, Ackenheil, Kaufmann and McGuffin1996) in the group of patients. To estimate full scale-IQ scores (including Digit Symbol Substitution, Arithmetic, BlockDesign, and Information subtests) we used an abbreviated and adapted version of the WAIS (Velthorst et al., Reference Velthorst, Levine, Henquet, de Haan, van Os, Myin-Germeys and Reichenberg2013). We measured clinical symptoms with the CAPE scale (Konings, Bak, Hanssen, van Os, & Krabbendam, Reference Konings, Bak, Hanssen, van Os and Krabbendam2006; Mossaheb et al., Reference Mossaheb, Becker, Schaefer, Klier, Schloegelhofer, Papageorgiou and Amminger2012), a questionnaire designed to rate self-reports of positive, negative, and depressive psychotic experiences. Duration of untreated psychosis (in weeks) was estimated with the Notting-ham Onset Schedule (Singh et al., Reference Singh, Cooper, Fisher, Tarrant, Lloyd, Banjo and Jones2005).

Measures of cannabis use

Information about cannabis use was collected with the Cannabis Experience Questionnaire (CEQ) further modified for the EUGEI study (CEQEU−GEI) (Di Forti et al., Reference Di Forti, Quattrone, Freeman, Tripoli, Gayer-Anderson and Quigley2019). The CEQ was modified to (1) include questions to assess dependence for cannabis use and other drugs, and (2) to describe use and changes in cannabis use over specific age periods. We asked participants whether they had ever used cannabis in their lifetime and, if so, we asked about the frequency, type (high or low potency), and duration of use based on the current pattern of use, or, when there was no current use of cannabis, based on the pattern of use that described best the overall pattern of cannabis use. We subdivided cannabis use frequency into three levels: daily use, occasional use (ranging from only once or twice in a lifetime to more than once a week), and never use (absence of lifetime cannabis use) (Di Forti et al., Reference Di Forti, Quattrone, Freeman, Tripoli, Gayer-Anderson and Quigley2019; Ferraro et al., Reference Ferraro, La Cascia, Quattrone, Sideli, Matranga, Capuccio and Di Forti2020).

We used data on the concentration of Δ⁹-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) in the different types of cannabis available across Europe from in the European Monitoring Centre for Drugs and Drug Addiction 2016 report to create a measure of cannabis potency. A cutoff of THC = 10% was used to define the potency variable [high (> = 10%) v. low (<10%) potency] based on the mean THC concentration expected in the different types of cannabis available across the sites (for further information, please see online Supplementary Methods and Di Forti et al. (Reference Di Forti, Quattrone, Freeman, Tripoli, Gayer-Anderson and Quigley2019)).

Cognitive bias assessment

To explore speech illusions and aberrance salience we used the White Noise Task. The task consists of a random presentation of 75 audio fragments, 25 of which include just white noise, 25 that contain white noise and barely audible speech, and 25 that include white noise mixed with clearly audible speech, with positive, negative, or neutral affective content. Participants listened to the sounds binaurally through headphones. The length of the task was approximately 15 min. After each fragment, we asked participants to press specific buttons on a keyboard which reflected what they heard: (1) speech with positive content; (2) speech with negative content; (3) speech with neutral content; (4) absence of speech, and (5) presence of speech but uncertain to choose between positive, negative, or neutral emotional valence. Speech illusion was defined as a white noise fragment in which any speech was heard (option 1, 2, 3, or 5 in just white noise fragments). Only 25 out of 75 fragments contained white noises, so the maximum score for speech illusion was 25. A dichotomous variable was then calculated based on a cut-off of two or more speech illusions independently of the emotional valence attributed by the participant (Catalan et al., Reference Catalan, Simons, Bustamante, Drukker, Madrazo, de Artaza and Gonzalez-Torres2014).

We used the Benton Facial Recognition Test (BFRT) (Benton, Reference Benton1994; Benton and Van Allen, Reference Benton and Van Allen1968) to assess facial recognition processing. The BFRT is a face discrimination test in which participants are required to match a target face to either one face keeping the same viewpoint and lighting conditions (6 items) or three or six faces presented simultaneously that vary in viewpoints and lighting (16 items). The total score is calculated based on the total correct answers given (maximum score is 54). Deficits in facial recognition processing were defined as scores of 20 or fewer correct answers in the BFRT (Duchaine & Nakayama, Reference Duchaine and Nakayama2006).

The Drawing to Decisions (DTD) index (Garety et al., Reference Garety, Waller, Emsley, Jolley, Kuipers, Bebbington and Freeman2015; So et al., Reference So, Siu, Wong, Chan and Garety2016) was used to assess Jumping To Conclusion (JTC) bias. We obtained this index from the probabilistic reasoning (beads) task, with 60:40 task ratios. Two jars of beads were shown to participants in equal but opposite ratios (85 red and 15 blue and vice versa). Both jars were hidden, and researchers told participants that individual beads were drawn consecutively from one jar. The beads were actually presented in a prespecified sequence. Participants were required to either decide from which jar the beads had come or postpone the decision (up to a maximum of 20 beads). The key outcome variable employed as an index of the JTC bias was the number of ‘Draws-To-Decision’ (DTD); the lower the DTD, the greater the JTC bias. For the purposes of this study JTC bias was defined as a DTD index of 2 or less in the beads task (Klein & Pinkham, Reference Klein and Pinkham2018), as participants are considered to have an extreme JTC bias when a decision is made after presentation of two or fewer items (Garety et al., Reference Garety, Freeman, Jolley, Dunn, Bebbington, Fowler and Dudley2005).

Statistical analysis

We calculated means and standard deviations for continuous variables and frequency and percentages for categorical variables. For the comparisons in cognitive biases and frequency of cannabis use between FEP patients, siblings, and controls, we used Chi-square tests. We used ANOVA analyses to compare quantitative socio-demographic variables between the three groups. We used the Bonferroni test for post-hoc pairwise comparisons.

Considering the uneven distribution of the cognitive biases, we decided on a dichotomous analysis and used cutoffs based on previous studies using customary definitions of cognitive biases from the literature. To examine the influence of group and frequency of cannabis use on cognitive biases variables we conducted three sets of stratified logistic regression model analyses, as we were interested in analyzing the effect of cannabis use on the three cognitive biases separately. We adjusted our analyses for demographic variables: sex, age, years in education, ethnicity, employment (yes/no), migrant status (yes/no), and intelligence quotient (IQ). Variables were included in two models; Model 1 included clinical group (FEP, sibling, or control), demographic variables (sex, age, years of education, employment, and migration), and IQ; Model 2 added the frequency of lifetime cannabis use (never/occasional/daily) and the interaction between the frequency and the clinical group. We performed secondary analyses to examine the effects of frequency of use (occasional v. daily) and potency of cannabis (high v. low) in the group of cannabis users only.

We conducted supplementary analyses in the FEP group to additionally adjust Model 2 for the (i) severity of negative psychotic symptomatology (CAPE Negative) and (ii) Duration of Untreated Psychosis (DUP).

The level of statistical significance was set at p < 0.05. All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS 25.

Results

Demographic and clinical characteristics

Socio-demographic characteristics of the sample are described in Table 1. We observed significant differences between the three clinical groups in sex, age, country, ethnicity, years in education, estimated IQ, and frequency of cannabis use. The FEP sample not analyzed in this study (n = 587; see online Supplementary Figure 1) showed significantly lower IQ, included lower rates of white and mixed ethnic groups and higher rates of black and north African ethnic groups and migrants, and higher rates of high-potency cannabis use relative to the FEP sample analyzed in the study (n = 543). Relative to the sibling and control samples not analyzed in the study, the sibling sample not analyzed in the study was significantly older and had fewer years in education and the control sample not analyzed in the study was older and included higher rates of white ethnic group. We did not find any significant differences in any other demographic or cannabis use variables on cognitive bias variables between the samples analyzed and not analyzed in this study.

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the sample: patients with first-episode psychosis, siblings, and controls

CAPE, Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences scale; DUP, Duration of Untreated Psychosis; FEP, First-Episode Psychosis; IQ, Intelligence Quotient.

a Outlier cases for DUP were not included in the analysis (n = 14).

Cannabis use

Participants with FEP were less likely to have never used cannabis (v. occasional and daily use) than siblings and controls. Thus, 38.9% of FEPs, 59.6% of siblings, and 52.3% of controls had never used cannabis (χ2 (4) = 176.6 < 0.001). Patients were also more likely to have used cannabis daily (29.3%) relative to controls (6.9%) and siblings (5.9%); (χ2 (2) = 176.6; p < 0.001). Occasional use of cannabis was more frequent in the control (40.8%) than in the FEP group (31.9%); (χ2 (4) = 176.6 < 0.001) (see Table 1). In the subgroup of cannabis users, high potency (64%) was more frequent than low potency use (36%) in siblings (χ2 (2) = 8.248; p < 0.016), with no significant differences in the FEP or control groups.

Association of clinical group and frequency of cannabis use with cognitive biases

FEP patients presented a higher proportion of all cognitive biases than siblings and controls. Across the whole sample, cognitive biases in the three tests were significantly more frequent in participants that had never used cannabis relative to those with occasional or daily use. Occasional users showed cognitive biases less frequently than daily users (all p < 0.001) (Table 2 and online Supplementary Figure 1).

Table 2. Percentage of participants with cognitive biases (speech illusions, facial recognition processing deficit (BFR), and jumping to cbias (DTD) according to clinical group and cannabis use

BFR, Benton facial recognition; DTD, drawing to decision.

Table 3 shows the logistic regression models of speech illusions (SI). In model 1, clinical group did not show a significant association with the presence of SI. In model 2, we found that frequency of cannabis use was a predictive factor in the model, with no significant effects of clinical group or the interaction between clinical group and frequency of use. Specifically, occasional use of cannabis correlated with 0.64-fold decreased odds (OR = 0.646; 95% CI 0.457–0.9113), and daily use of cannabis with 0.60-fold decreased odds (OR = 0.605; 95% CI 0.368–0.997) of having SI relative to never use of cannabis. The resulting final model explained 16.5% of the variance of the presence of SI (R 2 = 0.165, p < 0.001). Secondary analyses conducted in cannabis users showed a significant effect of low-potency cannabis on SI (OR = 1.829; 95%CI 1.297–2.578). Frequency of cannabis use (occasional v. daily) was no longer a predictive factor in the model and the effects decreased substantially. We found no significant interaction between clinical group and the frequency of cannabis use or potency (see online Supplementary Table 1).

Table 3. Logistic regression models for speech illusion

Note. All the models are adjusted for sex, age, ethnicity, years in education, employment, migration, and estimated intelligence quotient (IQ). Age was significant in Model 1 (p < 0.001); ethnicity and education were significant in Models 1 and 2 (p < 0.001) (not shown in the Table).

Model 1 = Group + sex, age, ethnicity, years in education, employment, migrant; Model 2 = Model 1 + frequency of cannabis use + interaction group × frequency of cannabis use (p > 0.05 for all interactions studied, data not shown in the Table). Significant results are highlighted in bold.

Presence of aberrance salience was considered as presence of speech illusion (speech illusion/yes) using a cutoff of two or more speech illusions.

R 2=Nagelkerke's r 2; Hosmer–Lemeshow test p > 0.05 for Models 1 and 2.

CI, confidence interval; FEP, first-episode psychosis; OR, odds ratio.

Table 4 shows the logistic regression models for the BFR. In model 1, the FEP group showed significantly higher odds of facial recognition processing (FRP) deficits compared with controls (OR = 1.467;95%CI 1.122–1.918), with no significant differences with their siblings. After controlling for frequency of cannabis in model 2, the difference in patients and controls remained significant, with comparable effects (OR = 1.642; 95%CI 1.123–2.402), while frequency of cannabis use was not a significant predictor in the model. The interaction effect between clinical group and frequency of cannabis use in the model was also not significant. The final model accounted for 12.1% of the explained variance (R 2 = 0.121, p < 0.001). Secondary analyses including cannabis users only showed a comparable effect of clinical group (OR = 1.588; 95%CI 1.104–2.284, a significant effect of potency (OR = 1.393; 95%CI 1.031–1.882), and no significant effects of frequency of cannabis use or the interaction between clinical group and frequency of use or potency in the models (see online Supplementary Table 2).

Table 4. Logistic regression models for facial recognition processing (FRP) deficit

Note. All the models are adjusted for sex, age, ethnicity, years in education, employment, migration, and estimated intelligence quotient (IQ). Age was significant in Model 1 (p < 0.001); ethnicity and education were significant in Models 1 and 2 (p < 0.001); IQ was significant in Model 1 (p < 0.001) and 2 (p = 0.002) (not shown in Table).

Model 1 = Group + sex, age, ethnicity, education, employment, migrant; Model 2 = Model 1 + Frequency of cannabis use + Interaction group x frequency of cannabis use (p > 0.05 in all interactions studied, data not shown in the Table). Significant results are highlighted in bold.

Deficits in FRP deficits were defined as scores of 20 or less in the BFR test (BFR low).

R 2 = Nagelkerke's r 2; Hosmer–Lemeshow test p > 0.05 for Models 1 and 2.

BFR, Benton facial recognition; CI, confidence interval; FEP, first-episode psychosis; OR, odds ratio.

Table 5 shows the logistic regression analyses for the DTD. We did not find a significant effect of clinical group on JTC bias in Model 1 or 2. After including frequency of use in Model 2, participants who used cannabis occasionally or daily showed significantly lower odds of JTC bias compared with abstainers in the whole sample (OR = 0.602; 95%CI 0.460–0.787) and (OR = 0.625; 95% CI 0.422–0.925), respectively. There was no significant interaction between clinical group and frequency of cannabis use in the model. This model explained 19.3% of the variance (R2 = 0.193, p < 0.001;).

Table 5. Logistic regression models for jumping to conclusions bias (drawing to decision index)

Note. All the models are adjusted for sex, age, ethnicity, years in education, employment, migration, and estimated intelligence quotient (IQ). Age was significant in Model 1 (p < 0.001); ethnicity, education, and IQ were significant in Models 1 and 2 (p < 0.001) (not shown in the Table).

Model 1 = group + sex, age, ethnicity, education, employment, migrant; Model 2 = Model 1 + frequency of cannabis use + Interaction group × frequency of cannabis use (p > 0.05 for all the interactions studied, data not shown). Significant results are highlighted in bold.

Jumping to conclusions bias was defined as a DTD index of 2 or less draws in the beads task.

R 2=Nagelkerke's r 2; Hosmer–Lemeshow test p > 0.05 for Models 1 and 2.

CI, confidence interval; DTD, drawing to decision; FEP, first-episode psychosis; OR, odds ratio.

Secondary analyses in the subgroup of cannabis users showed low-potency as a significant predictor of DTD (OR = 1.661; 95%CI 1.271–2.171), with no significant effect of frequency of use (occasional v. daily) or the interaction between clinical group and frequency of use or potency in the model (see online Supplementary Table 3).

Supplementary analyses in the FEP sample adjusted for the duration of untreated psychosis or the score on the negative CAPE subscale found comparable effects for the frequency of cannabis use variables and similar predictive capacity to those from the main models (see online Supplementary Tables 4–6).

Conclusions

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to examine the odds of speech illusions, facial recognition deficits, and jumping to conclusions biases in a large international sample of patients with FEP, their siblings, and healthy controls. We found that FEP patients showed greater likelihood of facial recognition processing (FRP) deficits than siblings and controls. Contrary to previous findings in schizophrenia (Hoffman, Reference Hoffman1999; Hoffman et al., Reference Hoffman, Woods, Hawkins, Pittman, Tohen, Preda and McGlashan2007; Holt et al., Reference Holt, Titone, Long, Goff, Cather, Rauch and Kuperberg2006; Kapur, Reference Kapur2003), we did not find significantly higher odds of speech illusions (SI) and JTC in FEP patients relative to siblings and controls, in this large international study.

Together with theory of mind, facial processing is critical for meaningful social interactions and for guiding social behavior and deficits in FRP may underlie some of the social cognition deficits in patients with psychosis (Comparelli et al., Reference Comparelli, De Carolis, Corigliano, Di Pietro, Trovini, Granese and Girardi2014; Green et al., Reference Green, Bearden, Cannon, Fiske, Hellemann, Horan and Nuechterlein2012; Mucci et al., Reference Mucci, Galderisi, Gibertoni, Rossi, Rocca and Bertolino2021; Velthorst et al., Reference Velthorst, Fett, Reichenberg, Perlman, van Os, Bromet and Kotov2017). In this study, we found more frequent FRP deficits in individuals with FEP than in their siblings and controls, thus suggesting that FRP deficits could be a specific marker related to psychosis but not genetically shared with siblings. This is consistent with previous studies reporting increased likelihood of cognitive biases related to facial processing deficits in individuals with psychosis relative to their siblings or controls (Fusar-Poli et al., Reference Fusar-Poli, Pries, van Os, Radhakrishnan, Pence, Erzin and Guloksuz2022b; Shin et al., Reference Shin, Na, Ha, Kang, Yoo and Kwon2008; Tripoli et al., Reference Tripoli, Quattrone, Ferraro, Gayer-Anderson, La Cascia, La Barbera and Murray2022; van 't Wout et al., Reference van 't Wout, Aleman, Kessels, Laroi and Kahn2004) and with previous evidence in the EU-GEI sample. For example, Fusar-Poli et al. (Reference Fusar-Poli, Pries, van Os, Erzin, Delespaul, Kenis and Guloksuz2022a, Reference Fusar-Poli, Pries, van Os, Radhakrishnan, Pence, Erzin and Guloksuz2022b) found that deficits in facial emotion recognition (measured by total Degraded Facial Affect Recognition task scores) were greater in individuals with schizophrenia than in siblings, who showed greater deficits than healthy controls. In our study, we did not find differences between siblings and controls regarding FRP deficits. Our divergent findings may be due to differing tasks or a more diagnostically heterogenous sample of FEP and their siblings in our study (Fusar-Poli et al., Reference Fusar-Poli, Pries, van Os, Radhakrishnan, Pence, Erzin and Guloksuz2022b).

We did not find an increased likelihood of aberrant salience in FEP patients than in siblings or healthy controls. Previous studies found that proneness to aberrant salience correlated with familial vulnerability for psychosis, and was more likely found in delusional schizophrenia patients than in non-delusional and healthy subjects or their siblings, thus suggesting a familial liability to psychosis associated with aberrant salience (Catalan et al., Reference Catalan, Simons, Bustamante, Drukker, Madrazo, de Artaza and Gonzalez-Torres2014; Galdos et al., Reference Galdos, Simons, Fernandez-Rivas, Wichers, Peralta, Lataster and van Os2011; Hoffman, Reference Hoffman1999; Hoffman et al., Reference Hoffman, Woods, Hawkins, Pittman, Tohen, Preda and McGlashan2007; Holt et al., Reference Holt, Titone, Long, Goff, Cather, Rauch and Kuperberg2006; Schepers et al., Reference Schepers, Lousberg, Guloksuz, Pries, Delespaul, Kenis and van Os2019). Also, previous studies focusing on FEP patients showed higher rates of speech illusions than the control group, contrary to our findings, and speech illusions correlated with positive symptomatology (Catalan et al., Reference Catalan, Simons, Bustamante, Drukker, Madrazo, de Artaza and Gonzalez-Torres2014). Our results may differ in part because most of the studies mentioned did not include FEP-only samples or studied smaller samples with more unspecific tasks to measure these cognitive processes.

Contrary to our JTC findings, previous literature reports that patients with psychosis frequently use less information to arrive at a decision than controls and show increased risk of JTC bias (Ahuir et al., Reference Ahuir, Crosas, Estrada, Zabala, Perez-Munoz, Gonzalez-Fernandez and Labad2021; Dudley et al., Reference Dudley, Taylor, Wickham and Hutton2016; Henquet et al., Reference Henquet, van Os, Pries, Rauschenberg, Delespaul, Kenis and Guloksuz2022; Hofer et al., Reference Hofer, Biedermann, Yalcin and Fleischhacker2010; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Altshuler, Glahn, Miklowitz, Ochsner and Green2013; Ross, McKay, Coltheart, & Langdon, Reference Ross, McKay, Coltheart and Langdon2015). Similarly, previous studies based on the EU-GEI WP6 sample including schizophrenia patients, their siblings and healthy controls, showed increased odds of JTC bias in both patients and siblings relative to controls, thus suggesting an association between JTC bias and familial risk for psychosis (Henquet et al., Reference Henquet, van Os, Pries, Rauschenberg, Delespaul, Kenis and Guloksuz2022). Our different results may be because some of these studies were conducted in chronic schizophrenia samples or did not include FEP patients only. Indeed, a previous study observed a higher prevalence of JTC bias in individuals with schizophrenia than in individuals with recent-onset psychosis. Inconsistent findings could also result from previous studies not including IQ as a potential confounding variable in the analyses. In fact, the FEP group showed increased odds of JCT bias relative to controls in our sample in the analyses not adjusted by IQ, with no significant effect of the clinical group after controlling for this variable.

Our overall results confirmed our hypothesis that lifetime abstention from cannabis correlated with the presence of SI and JTC bias compared to daily or occasional cannabis use, with a comparable effect across FEP patients, siblings, and controls. As social skills could enable FEP patients to acquire and maintain a drug habit, or facilitate the use of drugs in social environments (Menendez-Miranda et al., Reference Menendez-Miranda, Garcia-Alvarez, Garcia-Portilla, Gonzalez-Blanco, Saiz and Bobes2019), cannabis use in people with FEP could be indicative of better or preserved social abilities, more drug taking opportunities, better neurocognitive functioning, and therefore, fewer and less significant cognitive biases (Arnold et al., Reference Arnold, Allott, Farhall, Killackey and Cotton2015; Rodriguez-Sanchez et al., Reference Rodriguez-Sanchez, Ayesa-Arriola, Mata, Moreno-Calle, Perez-Iglesias, Gonzalez-Blanch and Crespo-Facorro2010; Yucel et al., Reference Yucel, Bora, Lubman, Solowij, Brewer, Cotton and Pantelis2012). In this respect, a previous analysis of the EU-GEI sample observed better social adjustment prior to FEP in occasional and daily cannabis users relative to non-users. This difference was not observed in the control group (Ferraro et al., Reference Ferraro, La Cascia, Quattrone, Sideli, Matranga, Capuccio and Di Forti2020). Previous literature on patients with psychosis has also reported premorbid differences between cannabis users and non-users, with cannabis users potentially developing psychosis at an earlier age through an alternative pathway with less cognitive vulnerability (both neurocognitive and social cognitive) (Myles, Myles, & Large, Reference Myles, Myles and Large2016; Schnakenberg Martin et al., Reference Schnakenberg Martin, Bonfils, Davis, Smith, Schuder and Lysaker2016).

Taken together, we conclude that FEP cannabis users could constitute a group of less cognitively impaired individuals (Arnold et al., Reference Arnold, Allott, Farhall, Killackey and Cotton2015) in whom cannabis use would be an important contributor to psychosis risk (Ferraro et al., Reference Ferraro, La Cascia, Quattrone, Sideli, Matranga, Capuccio and Di Forti2020; Ferraro et al., Reference Ferraro, Murray, Di Forti, Quattrone, Tripoli, Sideli and La Cascia2019; Ferraro et al., Reference Ferraro, Russo, O'Connor, Wiffen, Falcone, Sideli and Di Forti2013). In fact, we found no significant differences in cognitive biases between occasional and daily use in cannabis users, supporting the idea that FEP cannabis users and nonusers may represent distinct populations with differing premorbid load and risk factors, rather than there being a global direct protective effect of cannabis use on social cognition. Exploring if these associations exist both before and after the FEP warrants future longitudinal studies.

Among lifetime cannabis users, the use of low-potency cannabis was significantly associated with increased odds of SI, FRP, and JTC relative to high-potency cannabis use, with comparable effects across FEP patients, their siblings, and controls. Potent cannabis varieties, with high concentrations of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (Δ9-THC), correlate with the most harm to mental health (Di Forti et al., Reference Di Forti, Marconi, Carra, Fraietta, Trotta, Bonomo and Murray2015; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, van der Pol, Kuijpers, Wisselink, Das, Rigter and Lynskey2018) and recent studies based on the EU-GEI sample have shown how FEP patients with a history of daily use of high-potency cannabis present with more positive symptoms, compared with those who never used cannabis or used low potency variants (Quattrone et al., Reference Quattrone, Ferraro, Tripoli, La Cascia, Quigley, Quattrone and Di Forti2021). However, the relationship between the potency of cannabis and neurocognition is still unclear, and growing evidence suggests that two of the main cannabinoids, THC and CBD, display opposing neural, cognitive, and behavioral effects (Iseger & Bossong, Reference Iseger and Bossong2015). Moreover, a study of cannabis users implicated THC in impaired facial emotional recognition, while CBD improved facial emotional recognition and attenuated THC-induced impairment (Hindocha et al., Reference Hindocha, Freeman, Schafer, Gardener, Das, Morgan and Curran2015). Our results show that FEP patients who use high potency variants of cannabis show less cognitive bias than those who use low potency cannabis, contrary to previous findings. One possible explanation could be related with the gateway hypothesis; FEP patients who smoke low potency variants of cannabis and show social cognitive impairment may not progress to later consumption of higher potency variants of cannabis.

This study has several limitations. First, cross-sectional studies limit analyses to a descriptive level and we could not assess causality. We also lacked a direct measure of social skills. Future studies using methodologies that allow for a more in-depth analysis and using a prospective design could clarify the longitudinal association between cannabis use and the risk of cognitive biases, as well as ascertain the specific role of social skills in this association. Second, cannabis use was not corroborated with the collection of biological samples. Instead, we used self-reporting tools, with a risk of recall bias and we did not conduct specific memory assessments. Notwithstanding, previous studies reported that the use of interviews to collect information about drug use in the adult population is generally reliable and valid (Curran et al., Reference Curran, Hindocha, Morgan, Shaban, Das and Freeman2019; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Morgan, Hindocha, Schafer, Das and Curran2014; Van Dorn, Desmarais, Scott Young, Sellers, & Swartz, Reference Van Dorn, Desmarais, Scott Young, Sellers and Swartz2012). Third, although we analyzed a large sample of participants with FEP, their siblings, and healthy controls, our findings may not be generalizable to all FEP and sibling populations. Exclusion of participants from the analysis due to lacking information on cannabis use or cognitive bias variables could influence the representativeness of our findings. Fourth, in keeping with other EU-GEI publications we classified cannabis use frequency in three categories (never use, occasional use, daily use). However, the heterogeneity in the “occasional use” category may have influenced our results. Finally, despite potential effect of antipsychotic treatments and other interventions on cognitive functioning in first-episode psychosis (Allot et al., Reference Allott, Yuen, Baldwin, O’Donoghue, Fornito, Chopra and Wood2023), we did not consider pharmacological treatments and other cognitive or psychosocial interventions in our analyses. Future studies should assess the effect of these factors on the association between cannabis use and cognitive biases.

In conclusion, FRP deficits were more prevalent in FEP patients than in their siblings or control participants, with no significant differences between the three groups in other cognitive biases such as speech illusions or JTC bias in the adjusted analyses. Cognitive biases were more frequent in cannabis abstainers and in participants who used low potency of cannabis relative to daily and occasional users and high potency participants respectively. Our findings suggest that FEP patients who have never used cannabis on a regular basis would be more likely to present cognitive biases. Considering the detrimental effects of cannabis on psychosis risk even beyond genetic predisposition (Ferraro et al., Reference Ferraro, Quattrone, La Barbera, La Cascia, Morgan, Kirkbride and Murray2023), future studies should elucidate this association and its potential clinical implications further.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291724001715.

Acknowledgements

We thank the patients and their families for their participation in this study.

Funding statement

The European Network of National Schizophrenia Networks Studying Gene-Environment Interactions (EU-GEI) Project was supported by grant agreement HEALTH-F2-2010-241909 (Project EU-GEI) from the European Community's Seventh Framework Programme. This work was partially supported by the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation, Instituto de Salud Carlos III (ISCIII) (PI19/01024, PI20/00721, PI22/01824, PI23/00625), CIBER -Consorcio Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red- (CB/07/09/0023), co-financed by the European Union and ERDF Funds from the European Commission, ‘A way of making Europe’, financed by the European Union - NextGenerationEU (PMP21/00051), Madrid Regional Government (S2022/BMD-7216 AGES 3-CM), European Union Structural Funds, EU Seventh Framework Program, H2020 Program under the Innovative Medicines Initiative 2 Joint Undertaking (grant agreement No.101034377, Project PRISM-2), and Horizon Europe (HORIZON-HLTH-2021-STAYHLTH-01-02, grant agreement No 101057182, project Youth-GEMs), the National Institute of Mental Health of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number 1U01MH124639-01 (Project ProNET) and Award Number 5P50MH115846-03 (project FEP-CAUSAL), Fundación Familia Alonso, and Fundación Alicia Koplowitz. MRC is a Ramon y Cajal Research Fellow (RYC-2017-23144), Spanish Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities and was supported by a NARSAD independent investigator grant (no. 24628) from the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation. MRC has received grant support from Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (PI18/00753, PI21/00701) and the Alicia Koplowitz Foundation. CDC holds a Juan Rodés Grant from Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (JR19/00024). JBK is supported by the National Institute of Health Research (NIHR) University College London Hospitals (UCLH) Biomedical Research Centre (BRC).

Competing interests

Celso Arango has been a consultant to or has received honoraria or grants from Acadia, Angelini, Gedeon Richter, Janssen Cilag, Lundbeck, Minerva, Otsuka, Roche, Sage, Servier, Shire, Schering Plough, Sumitomo Dainippon Pharma, Sunovion and Takeda. Miquel Bernardo has been a consultant for, received grant/research support and honoraria from, and been on the speakers/advisory board of ABBiotics, Adamed, Angelini, Casen Recordati, Janssen-Cilag, Menarini, Rovi and Takeda. Covadonga M. Díaz-Caneja has received honoraria from Angelini and support to attend conferences from Janssen and Angelini. Peter B. Jones declares grant support from NIHR (0616-20003) and consulted for MSD. Carmen Moreno has received honoraria as a consultant and/or advisor and/or for lectures from Angelini, Esteve, Exeltis Janssen, Lundbeck, Neuraxpharm, Nuvelution, Otsuka, Pfizer, Servier and Sunovion outside the submitted work.

References

Ahuir, M., Crosas, J. M., Estrada, F., Zabala, W., Perez-Munoz, S., Gonzalez-Fernandez, A., … Labad, J. (2021). Cognitive biases are associated with clinical and functional variables in psychosis: A comparison across schizophrenia, early psychosis and healthy individuals. Revista de Psiquiatría y Salud Mental (English Edition), 14(1), 415. doi:10.1016/j.rpsm.2020.07.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allott, K., Yuen, H. P., Baldwin, L., O’Donoghue, B., Fornito, A., Chopra, S., …Wood, S. J. (2023). Effects of risperidone/paliperidone versus placebo on cognitive functioning over the first 6 months of treatment for psychotic disorder: Secondary analysis of a triple-blind randomised clinical trial. Translational Psychiatry, 13(1), 199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Archer, J., Hay, D. C., & Young, A. W. (1992). Face processing in psychiatric conditions. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 31(1), 4561. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8260.1992.tb00967.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnold, C., Allott, K., Farhall, J., Killackey, E., & Cotton, S. (2015). Neurocognitive and social cognitive predictors of cannabis use in first-episode psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 168(1–2), 231237. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2015.07.051CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benton, A. L. (1994). Contributions to neuropsychological assessment: A clinical manual. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Benton, A. L., & Van Allen, M. W. (1968). Impairment in facial recognition in patients with cerebral disease. Transactions of the American Neurological Association, 93, 3842. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(68)80018-8Google ScholarPubMed
Bersani, G., Orlandi, V., Kotzalidis, G. D., & Pancheri, P. (2002). Cannabis and schizophrenia: Impact on onset, course, psychopathology and outcomes. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 252(2), 8692. doi:10.1007/s00406-002-0366-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catalan, A., Simons, C. J., Bustamante, S., Drukker, M., Madrazo, A., de Artaza, M. G., … Gonzalez-Torres, M. A. (2014). Novel evidence that attributing affectively salient signal to random noise is associated with psychosis. PLoS One, 9(7), e102520. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0102520CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clausen, L., Hjorthoj, C. R., Thorup, A., Jeppesen, P., Petersen, L., Bertelsen, M., & Nordentoft, M. (2014). Change in cannabis use, clinical symptoms and social functioning among patients with first-episode psychosis: A 5-year follow-up study of patients in the OPUS trial. Psychological Medicine, 44(1), 117126. doi:10.1017/S0033291713000433CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Comparelli, A., De Carolis, A., Corigliano, V., Di Pietro, S., Trovini, G., Granese, C., … Girardi, P. (2014). Symptom correlates of facial emotion recognition impairment in schizophrenia. Psychopathology, 47(1), 6570. doi:10.1159/000350453CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curran, H. V., Hindocha, C., Morgan, C. J. A., Shaban, N., Das, R. K., & Freeman, T. P. (2019). Which biological and self-report measures of cannabis use predict cannabis dependency and acute psychotic-like effects? Psychological Medicine, 49(9), 15741580. doi:10.1017/S003329171800226XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Di Forti, M., Marconi, A., Carra, E., Fraietta, S., Trotta, A., Bonomo, M., … Murray, R. M. (2015). Proportion of patients in south London with first-episode psychosis attributable to use of high potency cannabis: A case-control study. The Lancet. Psychiatry, 2(3), 233238. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(14)00117-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D'Souza, D. C., Abi-Saab, W. M., Madonick, S., Forselius-Bielen, K., Doersch, A., Braley, G., … Krystal, J. H. (2005). Delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol effects in schizophrenia: Implications for cognition, psychosis, and addiction. Biological Psychiatry, 57(6), 594608. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duchaine, B. C., & Nakayama, K. (2006). Developmental prosopagnosia: A window to content-specific face processing. Current Opinon in Neurobiology, 16(2), 166173. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2006.03.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dudley, R., Taylor, P., Wickham, S., & Hutton, P. (2016). Psychosis, delusions and the “jumping to conclusions” reasoning bias: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 42(3), 652665. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbv150CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Di Forti, M., Quattrone, D., Freeman, T. P., Tripoli, G., Gayer-Anderson, C., & Quigley, H., … EU-GEI WP2 Group. (2019). The contribution of cannabis use to variation in the incidence of psychotic disorder across Europe (EU-GEI): A multicentre case-control study. The Lancet. Psychiatry, 6(5), 427436. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30048-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jongsma, H. E., Gayer-Anderson, C., Lasalvia, A., Quattrone, D., Mule, A., & Szoke, A., … European Network of National Schizophrenia Networks Studying Gene-Environment Interactions Work Package, G. (2018). Treated incidence of psychotic disorders in the multinational EU-GEI study. JAMA Psychiatry, 75(1), 3646. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.3554CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferraro, L., Russo, M., O'Connor, J., Wiffen, B. D., Falcone, M. A., Sideli, L., … Di Forti, M. (2013). Cannabis users have higher premorbid IQ than other patients with first onset psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 150(1), 129135. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2013.07.046CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferraro, L., Murray, R. M., Di Forti, M., Quattrone, D., Tripoli, G., Sideli, L., … La Cascia, C. (2019). IQ differences between patients with first episode psychosis in London and Palermo reflect differences in patterns of cannabis use. Schizophrenia Research, 210, 8188. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2019.06.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferraro, L., La Cascia, C., Quattrone, D., Sideli, L., Matranga, D., Capuccio, V., … Di Forti, M. (2020). Premorbid adjustment and IQ in patients with first-episode psychosis: A multisite case-control study of their relationship with cannabis use. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 46(3), 517529. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbz077CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferraro, L., Quattrone, D., La Barbera, D., La Cascia, C., Morgan, C., Kirkbride, J. B., … Murray, R. M. (2023). First-Episode psychosis patients who deteriorated in the premorbid period do not have higher polygenic risk scores than others: A cluster analysis of EU-GEI data. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 49(1), 218227. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbac100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, T. P., Morgan, C. J., Hindocha, C., Schafer, G., Das, R. K., & Curran, H. V. (2014). Just say ‘know’: How do cannabinoid concentrations influence users’ estimates of cannabis potency and the amount they roll in joints? Addiction, 109(10), 16861694. doi:10.1111/add.12634CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, T. P., van der Pol, P., Kuijpers, W., Wisselink, J., Das, R. K., Rigter, S., … Lynskey, M. T. (2018). Changes in cannabis potency and first-time admissions to drug treatment: A 16-year study in the Netherlands. Psychological Medicine, 48(14), 23462352. doi:10.1017/S0033291717003877CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fusar-Poli, L., Pries, L. K., van Os, J., Erzin, G., Delespaul, P., Kenis, G., … Guloksuz, S. (2022a). Examining facial emotion recognition as an intermediate phenotype for psychosis: Findings from the EUGEI study. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 113, 110440. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2021.110440CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fusar-Poli, L., Pries, L. K., van Os, J., Radhakrishnan, R., Pence, A. Y., Erzin, G., … Guloksuz, S. (2022b). The association between cannabis use and facial emotion recognition in schizophrenia, siblings, and healthy controls: Results from the EUGEI study. Eur Neuropsychopharmacology, 63, 4759. doi:10.1016/j.euroneuro.2022.08.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galdos, M., Simons, C., Fernandez-Rivas, A., Wichers, M., Peralta, C., Lataster, T., … van Os, J. (2011). Affectively salient meaning in random noise: A task sensitive to psychosis liability. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 37(6), 11791186. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbq029CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garety, P. A., & Freeman, D. (2013). The past and future of delusions research: From the inexplicable to the treatable. British Journal of Psychiatry, 203(5), 327333. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.113.126953CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garety, P. A., Freeman, D., Jolley, S., Dunn, G., Bebbington, P. E., Fowler, D. G., … Dudley, R. (2005). Reasoning, emotions, and delusional conviction in psychosis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(3), 373384. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.114.3.373CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garety, P., Waller, H., Emsley, R., Jolley, S., Kuipers, E., Bebbington, P., … Freeman, D. (2015). Cognitive mechanisms of change in delusions: An experimental investigation targeting reasoning to effect change in paranoia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 41(2), 400410. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbu103CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gayer-Anderson, C., Jongsma, H. E., Di Forti, M., Quattrone, D., Velthorst, E., de Haan, L., … Morgan, C. (2020). The European network of national schizophrenia networks studying gene-environment interactions (EU-GEI): Incidence and first-episode case-control programme. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 55(5), 645657. doi:10.1007/s00127-020-01831-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartz, S. M., Pato, C. N., Medeiros, H., Cavazos-Rehg, P., Sobell, J. L., & Knowles, J. A., … Genomic Psychiatry Cohort, C. (2014). Comorbidity of severe psychotic disorders with measures of substance use. JAMA Psychiatry, 71(3), 248254. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.3726CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, M. F., Bearden, C. E., Cannon, T. D., Fiske, A. P., Hellemann, G. S., Horan, W. P., … Nuechterlein, K. H. (2012). Social cognition in schizophrenia, Part 1: Performance across phase of illness. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38(4), 854864. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbq171CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, M. F., Horan, W. P., & Lee, J. (2019). Nonsocial and social cognition in schizophrenia: Current evidence and future directions. World Psychiatry, 18(2), 146161. doi:10.1002/wps.20624CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wobrock, T., Falkai, P., Schneider-Axmann, T., Hasan, A., Galderisi, S., & Davidson, M., … group, E. s. (2013). Comorbid substance abuse in first-episode schizophrenia: Effects on cognition and psychopathology in the EUFEST study. Schizophrenia Research, 147(1), 132139. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2013.03.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henquet, C., van Os, J., Pries, L. K., Rauschenberg, C., Delespaul, P., Kenis, G., … Guloksuz, S. (2022). A replication study of JTC bias, genetic liability for psychosis and delusional ideation. Psychological Medicine, 52(9), 17771783. doi:10.1017/S0033291720003578CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hindocha, C., Freeman, T. P., Schafer, G., Gardener, C., Das, R. K., Morgan, C. J., & Curran, H. V. (2015). Acute effects of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol, cannabidiol and their combination on facial emotion recognition: A randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled study in cannabis users. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 25(3), 325334. doi:10.1016/j.euroneuro.2014.11.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofer, A., Biedermann, F., Yalcin, N., & Fleischhacker, W. (2010). Neurocognition and social cognition in patients with schizophrenia or mood disorders. Neuropsychiatrie: Klinik, Diagnostik, Therapie und Rehabilitation, 24(3), 161169.Google ScholarPubMed
Hoffman, R. E. (1999). New methods for studying hallucinated ‘voices’ in schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica Supplemment, 395, 8994. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.1999.tb05987.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffman, R. E., Woods, S. W., Hawkins, K. A., Pittman, B., Tohen, M., Preda, A., … McGlashan, T. H. (2007). Extracting spurious messages from noise and risk of schizophrenia-spectrum disorders in a prodromal population. British Journal of Psychiatry, 191, 355356. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.106.031195CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holt, D. J., Titone, D., Long, L. S., Goff, D. C., Cather, C., Rauch, S. L., … Kuperberg, G. R. (2006). The misattribution of salience in delusional patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 83(2–3), 247256. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2005.12.858CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howes, O. D., & Murray, R. M. (2014). Schizophrenia: An integrated sociodevelopmental-cognitive model. Lancet (London, England), 383(9929), 16771687. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(13)62036-XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iseger, T. A., & Bossong, M. G. (2015). A systematic review of the antipsychotic properties of cannabidiol in humans. Schizophrenia Research, 162(1-3), 153161. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2015.01.033CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mucci, A., Galderisi, S., Gibertoni, D., Rossi, A., Rocca, P., & Bertolino, A., … Italian Network for Research on, P. (2021). Factors associated with real-life functioning in persons with schizophrenia in a 4-year follow-up study of the Italian network for research on psychoses. JAMA Psychiatry, 78(5), 550559. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.4614CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jockers-Scherubl, M. C., Wolf, T., Radzei, N., Schlattmann, P., Rentzsch, J., Gomez-Carrillo de Castro, A., & Kuhl, K. P. (2007). Cannabis induces different cognitive changes in schizophrenic patients and in healthy controls. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacolgy and Biological Psychiatry, 31(5), 10541063. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2007.03.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kapur, S. (2003). Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: A framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 1323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karpov, B., Lindgren, M., Kieseppa, T., Wegelius, A., & Suvisaari, J. (2021). Cognitive functioning and cannabis use in first-episode psychosis. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 76(7), 551558. doi:10.1080/08039488.2021.2018038CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, H. S., & Pinkham, A. E. (2018). Examining reasoning biases in schizophrenia using a modified “Jumping to Conclusions” probabilistic reasoning task. Psychiatry Research, 270, 180186. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2018.09.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koenfal, S., Gabrys, R., & Porath, A. (2019). Clearing the smoke on cannabis: Regular use and mental health. Ottawa: Canadian Centre on Substance Use and Addiction. https://www.ccsa.ca/sites/default/files/2019-08/CCSACannabis-Use-Mental-HealthReport-2019-en_0.pdfGoogle Scholar
Konings, M., Bak, M., Hanssen, M., van Os, J., & Krabbendam, L. (2006). Validity and reliability of the CAPE: A self-report instrument for the measurement of psychotic experiences in the general population. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 114(1), 5561. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00741.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langdon, R., Still, M., Connors, M. H., Ward, P. B., & Catts, S. V. (2014). Jumping to delusions in early psychosis. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 19(3), 241256. doi:10.1080/13546805.2013.854198CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, J., Altshuler, L., Glahn, D. C., Miklowitz, D. J., Ochsner, K., & Green, M. F. (2013). Social and nonsocial cognition in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: Relative levels of impairment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 170(3), 334341. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12040490CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lev-Ran, S., Le Foll, B., McKenzie, K., George, T. P., & Rehm, J. (2013). Cannabis use and cannabis use disorders among individuals with mental illness. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 54(6), 589598. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2012.12.021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, H., Chan, R. C., Zhao, Q., Hong, X., & Gong, Q. Y. (2010). Facial emotion perception in Chinese patients with schizophrenia and non-psychotic first-degree relatives. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 34(2), 393400. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.01.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lincoln, T. M., Ziegler, M., Mehl, S., & Rief, W. (2010). The jumping to conclusions bias in delusions: Specificity and changeability. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119(1), 4049. doi:10.1037/a0018118CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linney, Y. M., Peters, E. R., & Ayton, P. (1998). Reasoning biases in delusion-prone individuals. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 37(3), 285302. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8260.1998.tb01386.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luzi, S., Morrison, P. D., Powell, J., di Forti, M., & Murray, R. M. (2008). What is the mechanism whereby cannabis use increases risk of psychosis? Neurotoxicity Research, 14(2-3), 105112. doi:10.1007/BF03033802CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mallett, R., Leff, J., Bhugra, D., Pang, D., & Zhao, J. H. (2002). Social environment, ethnicity and schizophrenia. A case-control study. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 37(7), 329335. doi:10.1007/s00127-002-0557-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mata, I., Rodriguez-Sanchez, J. M., Pelayo-Teran, J. M., Perez-Iglesias, R., Gonzalez-Blanch, C., Ramirez-Bonilla, M., … Crespo-Facorro, B. (2008). Cannabis abuse is associated with decision-making impairment among first-episode patients with schizophrenia-spectrum psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 38(9), 12571266. doi:10.1017/S0033291707002218CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGuffin, P., Farmer, A., & Harvey, I. (1991). A polydiagnostic application of operational criteria in studies of psychotic illness. Development and reliability of the OPCRIT system. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48(8), 764770. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1991.01810320088015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menendez-Miranda, I., Garcia-Alvarez, L., Garcia-Portilla, M. P., Gonzalez-Blanco, L., Saiz, P. A., & Bobes, J. (2019). History of lifetime cannabis use is associated with better cognition and worse real-world functioning in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. European Addiction Research, 25(3), 111118. doi:10.1159/000497317CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moritz, S., Van Quaquebeke, N., & Lincoln, T. M. (2012). Jumping to conclusions is associated with paranoia but not general suspiciousness: A comparison of two versions of the probabilistic reasoning paradigm. Schizophrenia Research Treatment, 2012, 384039. doi:10.1155/2012/384039CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mossaheb, N., Becker, J., Schaefer, M. R., Klier, C. M., Schloegelhofer, M., Papageorgiou, K., & Amminger, G. P. (2012). The Community Assessment of Psychic Experience (CAPE) questionnaire as a screening-instrument in the detection of individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 141(2–3), 210214. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2012.08.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, R. M., Paparelli, A., Morrison, P. D., Marconi, A., & Di Forti, M. (2013). What can we learn about schizophrenia from studying the human model, drug-induced psychosis? American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 162B(7), 661670. doi:10.1002/ajmg.b.32177CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, R. M., Mondelli, V., Stilo, S. A., Trotta, A., Sideli, L., Ajnakina, O., … Di Forti, M. (2020). The influence of risk factors on the onset and outcome of psychosis: What we learned from the GAP study. Schizophrenia Research, 225, 6368. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2020.01.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myles, H., Myles, N., & Large, M. (2016). Cannabis use in first episode psychosis: Meta-analysis of prevalence, and the time course of initiation and continued use. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 50(3), 208219. doi:10.1177/0004867415599846CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meijer, J. H., Dekker, N., Koeter, M. W., Quee, P. J., van Beveren, N. J., & Meijer, C. J., … Outcome of Psychosis, I. (2012). Cannabis and cognitive performance in psychosis: A cross-sectional study in patients with non-affective psychotic illness and their unaffected siblings. Psychological Medicine, 42(4), 705716. doi:10.1017/S0033291711001656CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruins, J., Pijnenborg, G. H. M., PHAMOUS investigators, Visser, E., & Castelein, S. (2021). The association of cannabis use with quality of life and psychosocial functioning in psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 228, 229234. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2020.11.059CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quattrone, D., Ferraro, L., Tripoli, G., La Cascia, C., Quigley, H., Quattrone, A., … Di Forti, M. (2021). Daily use of high-potency cannabis is associated with more positive symptoms in first-episode psychosis patients: The EU-GEI case-control study. Psychological Medicine, 51(8), 13291337. doi:10.1017/S0033291720000082CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rabin, R. A., Zakzanis, K. K., & George, T. P. (2011). The effects of cannabis use on neurocognition in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Research, 128(1–3), 111116. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2011.02.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodriguez-Sanchez, J. M., Ayesa-Arriola, R., Mata, I., Moreno-Calle, T., Perez-Iglesias, R., Gonzalez-Blanch, C., … Crespo-Facorro, B. (2010). Cannabis use and cognitive functioning in first-episode schizophrenia patients. Schizophrenia Research, 124(1–3), 142151. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2010.08.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ross, R. M., McKay, R., Coltheart, M., & Langdon, R. (2015). Jumping to conclusions about the beads task? A meta-analysis of delusional ideation and data-gathering. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 41(5), 11831191. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbu187CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schepers, E., Lousberg, R., Guloksuz, S., Pries, L. K., Delespaul, P., Kenis, G., … van Os, J. (2019). White noise speech illusions: A trait-dependent risk marker for psychotic disorder? Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10, 676. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00676CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schnakenberg Martin, A. M., Bonfils, K. A., Davis, B. J., Smith, E. A., Schuder, K., & Lysaker, P. H. (2016). Compared to high and low cannabis use, moderate use is associated with fewer cognitive deficits in psychosis. Schizophrenia Research: Cognition, 6, 1521. doi:10.1016/j.scog.2016.09.001Google ScholarPubMed
Sevy, S., Burdick, K. E., Visweswaraiah, H., Abdelmessih, S., Lukin, M., Yechiam, E., & Bechara, A. (2007). Iowa gambling task in schizophrenia: A review and new data in patients with schizophrenia and co-occurring cannabis use disorders. Schizophrenia Research, 92(1–3), 7484. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2007.01.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shin, Y. W., Na, M. H., Ha, T. H., Kang, D. H., Yoo, S. Y., & Kwon, J. S. (2008). Dysfunction in configural face processing in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 34(3), 538543. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbm118CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singh, S. P., Cooper, J. E., Fisher, H. L., Tarrant, C. J., Lloyd, T., Banjo, J., … Jones, P. (2005). Determining the chronology and components of psychosis onset: The Nottingham Onset Schedule (NOS). Schizophrenia Research, 80(1), 117130. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2005.04.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
So, S. H., Siu, N. Y., Wong, H. L., Chan, W., & Garety, P. A. (2016). ‘Jumping to conclusions’ data-gathering bias in psychosis and other psychiatric disorders – two meta-analyses of comparisons between patients and healthy individuals. Clinical Psychology Review, 46, 151167. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2016.05.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soria Bauser, D., Thoma, P., Aizenberg, V., Brune, M., Juckel, G., & Daum, I. (2012). Face and body perception in schizophrenia: A configural processing deficit? Psychiatry Resarch, 195(1–2), 917. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2011.07.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tripoli, G., Quattrone, D., Ferraro, L., Gayer-Anderson, C., Rodriguez, V., La Cascia, C., … Di Forti, M. (2021). Jumping to conclusions, general intelligence, and psychosis liability: Findings from the multi-centre EU-GEI case-control study. Psychological Medicine, 51(4), 623633. doi:10.1017/S003329171900357XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tripoli, G., Quattrone, D., Ferraro, L., Gayer-Anderson, C., La Cascia, C., La Barbera, D., … Murray, G. K. (2022). Facial emotion recognition in psychosis and associations with polygenic risk for schizophrenia: Findings from the multi-center EU-GEI case-control study. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 48(5), 11041114. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbac022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Dael, F., Versmissen, D., Janssen, I., Myin-Germeys, I., van Os, J., & Krabbendam, L. (2006). Data gathering: Biased in psychosis? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 32(2), 341351. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbj021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Dorn, R. A., Desmarais, S. L., Scott Young, M., Sellers, B. G., & Swartz, M. S. (2012). Assessing illicit drug use among adults with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 200(2–3), 228236. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2012.05.028CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Os, J., Rutten, B. P., Myin-Germeys, I., Delespaul, P., Viechtbauer, W., van Zelst, C., … Mirjanic, T. (2014). Identifying gene environment interactions in schizophrenia: Contemporary challenges for integrated, large-scale investigations. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 40(4), 729736. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbu069Google ScholarPubMed
van 't Wout, M., Aleman, A., Kessels, R. P., Laroi, F., & Kahn, R. S. (2004). Emotional processing in a non-clinical psychosis-prone sample. Schizophrenia Research, 68(2–3), 271281. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2003.09.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Velthorst, E., Levine, S. Z., Henquet, C., de Haan, L., van Os, J., Myin-Germeys, I., & Reichenberg, A. (2013). To cut a short test even shorter: Reliability and validity of a brief assessment of intellectual ability in schizophrenia – A control-case family study. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 18(6), 574593. doi:10.1080/13546805.2012.731390CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Velthorst, E., Fett, A. J., Reichenberg, A., Perlman, G., van Os, J., Bromet, E. J., & Kotov, R. (2017). The 20-year longitudinal trajectories of social functioning in individuals with psychotic disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 174(11), 10751085. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.15111419CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walther, S., Federspiel, A., Horn, H., Bianchi, P., Wiest, R., Wirth, M., … Muller, T. J. (2009). Encoding deficit during face processing within the right fusiform face area in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 172(3), 184191. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.07.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, J., Farmer, A. E., Ackenheil, M., Kaufmann, C. A., & McGuffin, P. (1996). A multicentre inter-rater reliability study using the OPCRIT computerized diagnostic system. Psychological Medicine, 26(4), 775783. doi:10.1017/s003329170003779xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yucel, M., Bora, E., Lubman, D. I., Solowij, N., Brewer, W. J., Cotton, S. M., … Pantelis, C. (2012). The impact of cannabis use on cognitive functioning in patients with schizophrenia: A meta-analysis of existing findings and new data in a first-episode sample. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38(2), 316330. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbq079CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the sample: patients with first-episode psychosis, siblings, and controls

Figure 1

Table 2. Percentage of participants with cognitive biases (speech illusions, facial recognition processing deficit (BFR), and jumping to cbias (DTD) according to clinical group and cannabis use

Figure 2

Table 3. Logistic regression models for speech illusion

Figure 3

Table 4. Logistic regression models for facial recognition processing (FRP) deficit

Figure 4

Table 5. Logistic regression models for jumping to conclusions bias (drawing to decision index)

Supplementary material: File

Roldan et al. supplementary material

Roldan et al. supplementary material
Download Roldan et al. supplementary material(File)
File 137.7 KB