Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T19:17:29.446Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Correlates of real-world goal-directed behavior in schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 November 2021

Jaisal T. Merchant*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO 63130, USA
Erin K. Moran
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO 63130, USA Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO 63110, USA
Michael J. Strube
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO 63130, USA
Deanna M. Barch
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO 63130, USA Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO 63110, USA Department of Radiology, Mallinckrodt Institute of Radiology, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO 63110, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Jaisal T. Merchant, E-mail: jaisalmerchant@wustl.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Deficits in goal-directed behavior (i.e. behavior conducted to achieve a specific goal or outcome) are core to schizophrenia, difficult to treat, and associated with poor functional outcomes. Factors such as negative symptoms, effort-cost decision-making, cognition, and functional skills have all been associated with goal-directed behavior in schizophrenia as indexed by clinical interviews or laboratory-based tasks. However, little work has examined whether these factors relate to the real-world pursuit of goal-directed activities in this population.

Methods

This study aimed to fill this gap by using Ecological Momentary Assessment (four survey prompts per day for 1 week) to test hypotheses about symptom, effort allocation, cognitive, and functional measures associated with planned and completed goal-directed behavior in the daily lives of 63 individuals with schizophrenia.

Results

Individuals with schizophrenia completed more goal-directed activities than they planned [t(62) = −4.01, p < 0.001]. Motivation and pleasure (i.e. experiential) negative symptoms, controlling for depressive symptoms, negatively related to planned goal-directed behavior [odds ratio (OR) 0.92, p = 0.005]. Increased effort expenditure for high probability rewards (planned: OR 1.01, p = 0.034, completed: OR 1.01, p = 0.034) along with performance on a daily functional skills task (planned: OR 1.04, p = 0.002, completed: OR 1.03, p = 0.047) negatively related to both planned and completed goal-directed activity.

Conclusions

Our results present correlates of real-world goal-directed behavior that largely align with impaired ability to make future estimations in schizophrenia. This insight could help identify targeted treatments for the elusive motivated behavior deficits in this population.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Deficits in motivation and goal-directed behavior are core to schizophrenia. Despite their close link to daily and long-term functioning (Barch et al., Reference Barch, Treadway and Schoen2014; Fervaha, Foussias, Agid, & Remington, Reference Fervaha, Foussias, Agid and Remington2014b; Foussias et al., Reference Foussias, Mann, Zakzanis, van Reekum, Agid and Remington2011), they remain particularly challenging treatment targets (Aleman et al., Reference Aleman, Lincoln, Bruggeman, Melle, Arends, Arango and Knegtering2017; Velthorst et al., Reference Velthorst, Koeter, Gaag, Nieman, Fett, Smit and de Haan2015). Research using laboratory-based clinical or behavioral indices of goal-directed behavior has associated this deficit with factors such as negative or depressive symptoms, effort-cost decision-making (ECDM), and cognitive and functional skills. However, this use of laboratory-based proxies for motivation may limit the ability to capture the real-world nature of this construct. Thus, there is a critical need to elucidate the nature and correlates of reduced motivated behavior as it appears in the daily lives of individuals with schizophrenia.

Motivation, which involves the planning and pursuit of behaviors that lead to desired outcomes (i.e. goal-directed behavior), is reduced in mental illnesses such as schizophrenia (e.g. Barch, Pagliaccio, & Luking, Reference Barch, Pagliaccio, Luking, Simpson and Balsam2015) and has largely been associated with the diminished motivation and pleasure factor of negative symptoms (MAP negative symptoms; e.g. Horan, Kring, Gur, Reise, & Blanchard, Reference Horan, Kring, Gur, Reise and Blanchard2011). Notably, individuals with schizophrenia can have symptoms of depression, and major depressive disorder is also associated with reduced goal-directed activity, although the magnitude of this deficit may be less than that in schizophrenia (Barch et al., Reference Barch, Pagliaccio, Luking, Simpson and Balsam2015). Previous work shows that negative symptoms account for functional impairment in schizophrenia beyond the effect of depressive symptoms (Fervaha, Foussias, Agid, & Remington, Reference Fervaha, Foussias, Agid and Remington2014a). However, it is unclear whether reductions in goal-directed behavior in schizophrenia are related to depressive symptoms independent of their relationship to negative symptoms.

Some research has suggested that decreased goal-directed behavior in schizophrenia is related to deficits in the anticipation of pleasure (i.e. hedonic deficits), but these findings are mixed (e.g. Gard, Kring, Gard, Horan, & Green, Reference Gard, Kring, Gard, Horan and Green2007; Gard et al., Reference Gard, Sanchez, Cooper, Fisher, Garrett and Vinogradov2014) and it is not clear that pursuit of goal-directed activities is related to anticipation of pleasurable outcomes. For example, some studies collecting real-time reports from patients' daily lives using Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA) have found that individuals with schizophrenia show intact anticipatory pleasure yet still participate in fewer goal-directed activities than healthy controls (Edwards, Cella, Emsley, Tarrier, & Wykes, Reference Edwards, Cella, Emsley, Tarrier and Wykes2018; Gard et al., Reference Gard, Sanchez, Cooper, Fisher, Garrett and Vinogradov2014). Therefore, the current work focuses on factors aside from hedonics, such as effort allocation and cognitive function, that may relate to the degree to which individuals with schizophrenia partake in goal-directed activities.

ECDM, the process of making decisions about whether to expend effort for favorable outcomes, has been identified as a key mechanism of motivated behavior in schizophrenia (see Culbreth, Moran, & Barch, Reference Culbreth, Moran and Barch2018 for a review). On behavioral ECDM tasks such as the Effort Expenditure for Reward Task (EEfRT; Treadway, Buckholtz, Schwartzman, Lambert, & Zald, Reference Treadway, Buckholtz, Schwartzman, Lambert and Zald2009), there is mixed literature on whether individuals with schizophrenia expend less overall effort than healthy controls, but consistent evidence that they are less willing than healthy controls to increase their effort expenditure for higher probability or higher magnitude rewards (Barch et al., Reference Barch, Treadway and Schoen2014; Fervaha et al., Reference Fervaha, Foussias, Agid and Remington2014b; Gold et al., Reference Gold, Strauss, Waltz, Robinson, Brown and Frank2013; Huang et al., Reference Huang, Yang, Lan, Zhu, Liu, Wang and Chan2016; McCarthy, Treadway, Bennett, & Blanchard, Reference McCarthy, Treadway, Bennett and Blanchard2016; Treadway, Peterman, Zald, & Park, Reference Treadway, Peterman, Zald and Park2015; Treadway et al., Reference Treadway, Peterman, Zald and Park2015; Whitton, Merchant, & Lewandowski, Reference Whitton, Merchant and Lewandowski2020). Furthermore, this difficulty using probability and value information to guide effortful decisions has been associated with MAP negative symptoms of psychosis (e.g. Gold et al., Reference Gold, Strauss, Waltz, Robinson, Brown and Frank2013), suggesting it is more related to motivation-related individual differences in schizophrenia than is effort expenditure as a whole. Very limited literature has examined factors associated with ECDM in daily life in schizophrenia, but EMA studies that have done so found that ECDM was associated with momentary survey reports of experiential negative symptoms (Moran et al., Reference Moran, Culbreth and Barch2017) or motivation (at a trend-level) in this population (Culbreth, Moran, Kandala, Westbrook, & Barch, Reference Culbreth, Moran, Kandala, Westbrook and Barch2020). Still, EEfRT task performance has not been examined in conjunction with the frequency of planned or completed goal-directed behaviors in schizophrenia.

Cognition has also been linked to laboratory-based proxies for goal-directed activity in schizophrenia (Siddiqui et al., Reference Siddiqui, Saperia, Fervaha, Da Silva, Jeffay, Zakzanis and Foussias2019; Sitnikova, Goff, & Kuperberg, Reference Sitnikova, Goff and Kuperberg2009). Although much of the work conducted on this topic examines cognition globally, it is important to parse out what specific components of cognition relate to motivated behavior. Working memory, which involves short-term storage and manipulation of information that can inform behavioral goals, has been associated with motivated behavior (Heerey & Gold, Reference Heerey and Gold2007) and functional capacity (Gold et al., Reference Gold, Barch, Feuerstahler, Carter, MacDonald, Ragland and Luck2019). Thus, it is of particular interest whether this domain would relate to goal-directed behavior in patients' daily lives. Laboratory-based measures of functioning, such as the University of California San Diego Performance-based Skills Assessment (UPSA; Patterson, Goldman, McKibbin, Hughs, & Jeste, Reference Patterson, Goldman, McKibbin, Hughs and Jeste2001), have also been associated with working memory and cognition (Bowie, Reichenberg, Patterson, Heaton, & Harvey, Reference Bowie, Reichenberg, Patterson, Heaton and Harvey2006; Kraus et al., Reference Kraus, Gold, Barch, Walker, Chun, Buchanan and Keefe2020; Twamley et al., Reference Twamley, Doshi, Nayak, Palmer, Golshan, Heaton and Jeste2002). Although a recent EMA study found that UPSA performance was not related to the number of daily productive behaviors (e.g. work/school, home-care) in schizophrenia, (Granholm et al., Reference Granholm, Holden, Mikhael, Link, Swendsen, Depp and Harvey2020), other studies have found that UPSA score is associated with real-world functioning and independence of living (Bowie et al., Reference Bowie, Reichenberg, Patterson, Heaton and Harvey2006; Mausbach et al., Reference Mausbach, Bowie, Harvey, Twamley, Goldman, Jeste and Patterson2008; Twamley et al., Reference Twamley, Doshi, Nayak, Palmer, Golshan, Heaton and Jeste2002). These mixed findings indicate a need to further examine whether laboratory-based measures of functioning, such as the UPSA, relate to real-world motivated behavior.

Overall, the literature suggests that negative or depressive symptoms, ECDM, working memory, and functional capacity for everyday activities may be associated with motivated behavior in psychosis. However, most of this work examines laboratory-based proxies for motivation via clinical reports or behavioral tasks – methods that are limited in their ability to capture real-world behavior in patients' day-to-day lives. For example, clinical reports rely on individuals' subjective memories of their past experiences, while behavioral tasks attempt to artificially induce scenarios that may not be externally valid to individuals' lives. EMA, however, in allowing individuals to report on their current and planned behaviors as they arise naturally, may overcome these limitations. Although it has been suggested that EMA serves as a beneficial tool to assess functioning in schizophrenia (e.g. Granholm et al., Reference Granholm, Holden, Mikhael, Link, Swendsen, Depp and Harvey2020), few studies have used it to compare performance on laboratory-based proxies for motivation to real-time reports of patients' daily goal-directed activity.

Aims and hypotheses

The current study aims to build on the previous EMA work on motivation in schizophrenia (Culbreth et al., Reference Culbreth, Moran, Kandala, Westbrook and Barch2020; Edwards et al., Reference Edwards, Cella, Emsley, Tarrier and Wykes2018; Gard et al., Reference Gard, Sanchez, Cooper, Fisher, Garrett and Vinogradov2014; Granholm et al., Reference Granholm, Holden, Mikhael, Link, Swendsen, Depp and Harvey2020; Moran et al., Reference Moran, Culbreth and Barch2017; Strassnig, Harvey, Miller, Depp, & Granholm, Reference Strassnig, Harvey, Miller, Depp and Granholm2021) and fill remaining gaps by investigating whether laboratory-based symptom or behavioral indices for motivation in schizophrenia relate to the real-world frequency of planned and completed goal-directed activity. We hypothesized that: (a) MAP negative symptoms would relate to reductions in both planned and completed goal-directed behavior over and above depressive symptoms, (b) increased effort expenditure for conditions with high reward probability and value would be more positively related to planned and completed goal-directed behavior than overall effort expenditure, and (c) both working memory and functional skills task performance would positively relate to the frequency of planned and completed goal-directed activities.

Methods

Participants

Outpatients meeting DSM-IV-TR criteria for schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder (SCZ; total: N = 66; schizophrenia: n = 54, schizoaffective: n = 12) participated in the study (see Table 1 for demographics and the supplementary materials for recruitment information). Consistent with prior EMA research (Myin-Germeys, Nicolson, & Delespaul, Reference Myin-Germeys, Nicolson and Delespaul2001), participants with an EMA survey response rate below 33% (n = 3) were excluded from the analyses. Our final sample size of 63 meets or exceeds the range of ~63% of EMA studies in schizophrenia (Vachon, Viechtbauer, Rintala, & Myin-Germeys, Reference Vachon, Viechtbauer, Rintala and Myin-Germeys2019). Of note, the sample did not include a healthy control group as the study goals were to dimensionally explore symptom and behavioral correlates of goal-directed behavior within a schizophrenia population.

Table 1. Demographic, clinical and behavioral characteristics

Note: the values for Probability increase exclude the significant outlier.

Procedure

The study involved: (1) an initial laboratory visit with a diagnostic interview and an EMA training session (i.e. review of EMA survey and protocol to ensure comprehension and ability to participate), (2) 1 week of the EMA protocol, and (3) a post-EMA laboratory visit with behavioral tasks and clinical measures documenting participants' EMA-week symptoms. Participants were compensated $1.75 per EMA survey completed and $40 per laboratory visit.

EMA

EMA protocol

Participants were provided with Android-enabled smartphones and prompted to complete the EMA survey four times per day for 1 week. These pseudorandom prompts occurred approximately every 3 hours between 10:00 AM and 7:00 PM. Any surveys started more than 15-minutes after the prompt were not counted. The mean response rate for the EMA surveys was 80.5%, with a standard deviation of 19.07% (including the response rates of the three excluded participants noted above). This represents high EMA compliance, as EMA study response rates in schizophrenia average approximately 70%, even after excluding low responders (Vachon et al., Reference Vachon, Viechtbauer, Rintala and Myin-Germeys2019).

EMA survey

At each EMA prompt, participants were asked to indicate their current (“right now”), past (“since the last beep”), and anticipated (“in the next 2 hours”-to focus on a time window with a high likelihood of occurrence before the next prompt) activities, by selecting “as many as apply” from a drop-down list (e.g. Running an Errand, Socializing). Activities endorsed as anticipated were characterized as planned behaviors, while those reported as past or current were combined to comprise completed behaviors. We categorized each activity as either goal-directed or non-goal-directed. Goal-directed activities were active behaviors, conducted to achieve a specific outcome (e.g. attending school for education, or socializing for pleasure or a social network). Non-goal-directed activities were those that tend to be completed passively (e.g. watching TV) or that are a necessary to live (e.g. eating or drinking). See Table 2 for our goal-directed behavior categorizations and reported frequencies. Of note, our grouping of goal-directed behavior aligns with that in prior EMA literature in schizophrenia (e.g. Granholm et al., Reference Granholm, Holden, Mikhael, Link, Swendsen, Depp and Harvey2020) and with the relevant (and highest loading) factors of the Specific Level of Functioning Scale (SLOF; Schneider & Struening, Reference Schneider and Struening1983): personal care, activities, working skills, and interpersonal relations. Planned and completed goal-directed behavior were coded (1 versus 0) at each timepoint based on the presence of at least one reported goal-directed activity.

Table 2. Average reported frequency of daily goal-directed and non-goal-directed behaviors

We focused on at least one goal-directed activity at each prompt rather than the total frequency of these activities for various reasons. First, since we combined current and past activities into a single “completed goal-directed activities” category, we did not want to double count extended activities that may have been reported as both past and current behavior. Additionally, since we did not have a sense of the length of engagement in each behavior, we did not want to give more weight to multiple short goal-directed activities (e.g. cleaning for 10 min, driving, and socializing) than a single, prolonged goal-directed activity (e.g. studying productively for a few hours). We also wanted to account for possible differences in response behavior among participants. For example, if bowling with friends, one participant could indicate both “socializing” and “entertainment away from home” (two goal-directed activities), while another might simply indicate “socializing” (one goal-directed activity). As such, we wanted to avoid confounding these differences in response style with frequency of goal-directed behavior.

Clinical and behavioral measures (see supplement for additional details)

Clinical assessments

We assessed MAP negative symptoms with the Motivation and Pleasure scale of the clinician-rated Clinical Assessment Interview for Negative Symptoms (CAINS-MAP); Kring, Gur, Blanchard, Horan, & Reise, Reference Kring, Gur, Blanchard, Horan and Reise2013), which includes sections related to motivation and frequency of pleasure for social relationships, work and school, and recreation. We used the Beck Depression Inventory – Second Edition (BDI-II; Beck, Steer, & Brown, Reference Beck, Steer and Brown1996) self-report measure to assess depressive symptoms. Higher scores on both the CAINS and BDI indicate increased impairment.

Behavioral tasks

EEfRT. Participants completed a modified version (Barch et al., Reference Barch, Treadway and Schoen2014; Moran et al., Reference Moran, Culbreth and Barch2017) of the original EEfRT task (Treadway et al., Reference Treadway, Buckholtz, Schwartzman, Lambert and Zald2009) to assess ECDM. We examined participants' likelihood to expend high physical effort for a monetary reward overall (Total Hard), when the probability of reward receipt increased from 50 to 88% (Probability Increase), and when the reward value increased from low to high (Monetary Increase).

Running Span. We used a Running Span Task (Gold et al., Reference Gold, Barch, Feuerstahler, Carter, MacDonald, Ragland and Luck2019) to assess working memory as per participants' ability to remember the last × letters (steadily increasing) from an unpredictable number of presented letters.

UPSA. Participants also completed the UPSA-B task (Maubach et al., Reference Mausbach, Harvey, Goldman, Jeste and Patterson2006) to measure their ability to perform functional tasks involving financial (e.g. paying bills) and communication (e.g. dialing the emergency number) skills.

Statistical analysis

All analyses were conducted using R statistical software (v 4.0.4; R Core Team, 2021). We conducted a paired samples t-test to compare the frequencies of planned versus completed goal-directed activities by using the percentage of timepoints per individual for which each of these behavior types were endorsed.

We then conducted a series of generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs) using the lme4 package (Bates, Maechler, & Bolker, Reference Bates, Maechler, Bolker, Aut, Cre, Walker, Christensen and K.2021) and the glmer function to examine the relationships between clinical symptoms or behavioral measures (numeric predictor variables) and frequency of planned and completed goal-directed activities (binary outcome variables), nested by participant ID. To account for missing data, each of these models controlled for EMA prompt adherence (the percentage of prompts to which each participant responded). Negative and depressive symptoms were added simultaneously to their models to account for the effect of the other. Except where noted, each model contained a single, fixed effect predictor of interest (a clinical symptom or behavioral measure). The underlying binomial model of the data was confirmed for each outcome variable using the DHARMa package (Hartig & Lohse, Reference Hartig and Lohse2021). We exponentiated the coefficients of our GLMMs to compute each odds ratio (OR) and calculated the associated confidence interval (CI) by exponentiating the endpoints of the original coefficient CIs.

Additionally, we examined whether within-person variance (time of day or day of week) would affect our significant results. We added each of these variables separately as random effects (controlling for adherence) to predict planned or completed goal-directed behavior. Any significant predictors (only observed for time of day's relation to planned goal-directed behavior) were then added to our originally significant models. Autocorrelation was also explored and found to be small (for planned goal-directed behavior) or non-existent (for completed goal-directed behavior). Notably, none of these model additions altered the results. Thus, the simpler model analyses are reported.

Results

Planned and completed goal-directed behavior

Table 2 contains the average percent of timepoints at which each EMA activity was planned and completed. As shown in Fig. 1, participants planned significantly fewer goal-directed activities than they completed (t(62) = −4.01, p < 0.001). Planned and completed goal-directed behavior were significantly correlated (r  = 0.82, p < 0.001). On average, participants endorsed planned goal-directed behavior at 66% of timepoints (s.d. = 26%) and completed goal-directed behavior at 74% of timepoints (s.d. = 28%).

Fig. 1. Percent of time points for which at least one goal-directed activity was endorsed as planned (M = 66, Med = 70, s.d. = 26) or completed (M = 74, Med = 85, s.d. = 28).

Negative and depressive symptom relationships to goal-directed behavior

Consistent with our hypothesis, we found that MAP negative symptoms negatively related to planned goal-directed behavior when controlling for depressive symptoms (Table 3, Fig. 2a). However, in contrast to our predictions, negative symptoms only related to completed goal-directed activity at a trend level (Table 3, Fig. 2a). Depressive symptoms did not relate to either planned or completed goal-directed behavior when controlling for MAP negative symptoms (Table 3). Of note, exploratory analyses found that momentary ratings of sadness also did not relate to either planned (OR 0.92, p = 0.483) or completed (OR 0.92, p = 0.899) goal-directed behavior.

Fig. 2. (a) Plot of CAINS MAP scores and Goal-Directed Activities with 95% confidence bands. Of note- the y axis reflects the residualized values of BDI and goal-directed behavior in order to control for that variable. (b): Plot of probability increase scores and goal-directed activities with 95% confidence bands. Of note- this graph represents the data that excludes the significant probability increase outlier. (c) Plot of UPSA scores and goal-directed activities with 95% confidence bands.

Table 3. GLMMs of clinical and behavioral measure relationships to goal-directed behavior (controlling for adherence)

Note: Adherence, was not significant in any of the models (OR range: 1.00–1.01, p value range 0.250–0.68); probability increase analyses conducted excluding significant outlier (see results section for more details).

ECDM and goal-directed behavior

In line with our hypotheses, Probability Increase but not Total Hard positively related to both planned and completed goal-directed behavior (Table 3, Fig. 2b). However, inconsistent with our prediction, Monetary Increase did not relate to either planned or completed goal-directed behavior (Table 3). Grubbs' test reported a significant outlier in the output values for Probability Increase (>5 s.d. from the mean). Therefore, we excluded that outlier for the Probability Increase analyses.

As a follow-up, we assessed whether Probability Increase and MAP negative symptoms' associations with goal-directed behavior would hold when they were entered simultaneously. In this model, probability increase had a trend-level relationship with planned (OR 1.00, p = 0.051) and completed (OR 1.01, p = 0.051) goal-directed behavior, while negative symptoms continued to significantly relate to planned (OR 0.92, p = 0.010) but not completed (OR 0.94, p = 0.127) goal-directed behavior.

Cognition, functioning, and goal-directed behavior

Inconsistent with our predictions, working memory did not significantly relate to either planned or completed goal-directed behavior (Table 3). However, in line with our hypothesis, UPSA-B score related to both planned and completed goal-directed behavior (Table 3, Fig. 2c). We conducted a follow-up analysis with both negative symptoms and UPSA-B to determine whether this UPSA-B significance would hold independently of negative symptoms. There was a trend-level association between UPSA-B and planned goal-directed activities (OR 1.01, p = 0.060), but no UPSA-B association with completed goal-directed activities (OR 1.01, p = 0.392). Negative symptoms continued to relate to planned (OR 0.93, p = 0.016) but not completed (OR 0.94, p = 0.123) goal-directed behavior.

Discussion

The goal of the current study was to examine correlates of real-world goal-directed behavior in the daily lives of individuals with schizophrenia. Our results showed that people with schizophrenia completed more goal-directed activities than they planned, MAP negative symptoms negatively related to planned goal-directed activity, and use of reward probability to guide effort expenditure along with performance on a functional skills task positively related to both planned and completed goal-directed activity.

Individuals with schizophrenia completed more goal-directed activities than they predicted they would complete. In line with research that has documented impairments in future projection in schizophrenia, such as deficits in planning ability (Holt, Wolf, Funke, Weisbrod, & Kaiser, Reference Holt, Wolf, Funke, Weisbrod and Kaiser2013; Semkovska, Bédard, Godbout, Limoge, & Stip, Reference Semkovska, Bédard, Godbout, Limoge and Stip2004; Seter, Giovannetti, Kessler, & Worth, Reference Seter, Giovannetti, Kessler and Worth2011) or anticipating future pleasure (e.g. Moran & Kring, Reference Moran and Kring2018), this finding could reflect difficulty estimating upcoming behavioral goals. Lack of agency or passivity could also contribute to this result, as individuals with schizophrenia may not actively plan for each of their goals but be reminded of them in-the-moment and supported in their completion by those around them. Overall, this decreased anticipation of later completed goals could indicate that a large part of the motivated behavior impairment documented in schizophrenia may be at the level of planning.

Interestingly, future thinking deficits observed in planning and anticipatory pleasure in schizophrenia have been associated with negative symptoms (Moran & Kring, Reference Moran and Kring2018; Semkovska et al., Reference Semkovska, Bédard, Godbout, Limoge and Stip2004), which is consistent with our finding that decreased planned (but not completed) goal-directed behavior related to these symptoms. This finding that MAP negative symptoms were significantly associated with planned goal-directed behavior independent of depression (but only associated at a trend-level for completed goal-directed behavior), indicates that individuals with more severe negative symptoms may be less likely to plan future goal-directed activities and that MAP negative symptoms may be somewhat more related to the planning than completion of motivated behavior. However, this trend-level finding indicates that more research should be conducted to determine whether MAP negative symptoms also relate to the completion of goal-directed activities.

Depressive symptoms did not relate to either planned or completed goal-directed behavior independent of negative symptoms. Momentary reports of sadness were also unrelated to goal-directed behavior. Although our findings differ from those of a recent EMA study that found that both overall depressive symptoms and momentary sadness were related to increased unproductive versus productive activities (Strassnig et al., Reference Strassnig, Harvey, Miller, Depp and Granholm2021), they align with work suggesting that depressive symptoms in schizophrenia are not associated with daily functioning (e.g. Strassnig et al., Reference Strassnig, Raykov, O'Gorman, Bowie, Sabbag, Durand and Harvey2015) and that MAP negative symptoms account for functional impairment in schizophrenia beyond the effect of depressive symptoms (Fervaha et al., Reference Fervaha, Foussias, Agid and Remington2014a). Overall, these findings suggest that the motivated behavior deficit in this population is more related to MAP negative symptoms than depression. However, our results do not rule out the possibility that depressed mood is related to motivated behavior in non-schizophrenia populations.

For ECDM, we found that increased planned and completed goal-directed behavior was associated with the use of reward probability information to selectively decide when to expend effort, rather than with effort expenditure overall. This suggests that motivated behavior deficits in schizophrenia may be more related to difficulty using outcome estimations to guide choice (another potential indicator of difficulty projecting into the future to consider goals) rather than an overall unwillingness to engage in effortful activities. We did not see this same goal-directed behavior association with reward value. This could indicate that probability, rather than reward magnitude, may be more associated with future thinking. Alternatively, it is possible that greater variability in reward values (ours was ~$1.30–$4.30) would show this association.

We also found that functional skills task performance on the UPSA-B, but not working memory, was associated with planned and completed goal-directed behavior. The null finding for working memory was at odds with our hypothesis and with a previous study that suggested this association (Heerey & Gold, Reference Heerey and Gold2007). However, research has strongly associated UPSA performance with cognition (Bowie et al., Reference Bowie, Reichenberg, Patterson, Heaton and Harvey2006; Kraus et al., Reference Kraus, Gold, Barch, Walker, Chun, Buchanan and Keefe2020), with the suggestion that this task may serve as a proxy for cognitive functioning (Keefe, Poe, Walker, & Harvey, Reference Keefe, Poe, Walker and Harvey2006). Therefore, our findings could reflect a link between components of cognition aside from working memory and real-world motivated behavior. Interestingly, some research suggests that planning in schizophrenia relates to the ability to perform daily functional skills and to cognitive measures of executive functioning but not to memory (Semkovska et al., Reference Semkovska, Bédard, Godbout, Limoge and Stip2004; Seter et al., Reference Seter, Giovannetti, Kessler and Worth2011). Thus, it is possible that real-world motivated behavior is more related to cognition via an executive functioning component such as cognitive control (which involves the representation and updating of information or goals) than to working memory.

Although our finding that UPSA-B relates to real-world goal-directed behavior is in line with work suggesting it is associated with functioning or independent living (Bowie et al., Reference Bowie, Reichenberg, Patterson, Heaton and Harvey2006; Mausbach et al., Reference Mausbach, Bowie, Harvey, Twamley, Goldman, Jeste and Patterson2008; Twamley et al., Reference Twamley, Doshi, Nayak, Palmer, Golshan, Heaton and Jeste2002), it is in contrast to a recent EMA study that did not find it was associated with number of real-world productive behaviors (Granholm et al., Reference Granholm, Holden, Mikhael, Link, Swendsen, Depp and Harvey2020). The reason for this difference is unclear, but it could relate to differences in the study samples. Our sample consisted (with the exception of one participant) of individuals living in the community, while Granholm and colleagues' included individuals in supported living facilities. It is possible that UPSA performance is more related to functioning in a community-living sample. For example, the ability to glean important information from medical appointment letters (a task in the UPSA-B), may be more related to whether individuals living independently attend these appointments (goal-directed behavior) than to whether individuals in supported housing, who likely have assistance in this process, do so. Additionally, as UPSA-B did not continue to relate to goal-directed behavior independently of MAP negative symptoms, these negative symptoms may have a stronger relationship than UPSA-B performance to the frequency of goal-directed behavior. Future research could further clarify the pathways by which functional skills versus negative symptoms versus cognition relate to real-world goal-directed behavior.

This work should be interpreted in the context of several limitations. Our study did not have a healthy control group for comparison, as our goal was to examine what factors within a schizophrenia population are associated with this documented impairment in motivated behavior. However, due to this lack of control comparison, we could not replicate the well-established findings of decreased real-world goal-directed behavior in schizophrenia (Edwards et al., Reference Edwards, Cella, Emsley, Tarrier and Wykes2018; Gard et al., Reference Gard, Sanchez, Cooper, Fisher, Garrett and Vinogradov2014; Granholm et al., Reference Granholm, Holden, Mikhael, Link, Swendsen, Depp and Harvey2020), nor could we determine whether the relationships we found would extend to a non-schizophrenia sample.

In addition, there are a variety of ways in which real-world goal-directed behavior could be operationalized. We defined goal-directed behavior as activities requiring effort (beyond that to simply survive) that are associated with a positive outcome (i.e. the financial benefit that comes with attending work). Our categorizations of goal-directed and non-goal directed behavior were developed ad-hoc, with the consensus of all three authors and align with the relevant SLOF factors as described above. Still, it is possible that our categorizations of goal-directed behavior may not align with each individuals' own interpretations of their goal-directed behavior. Therefore, there could be benefits to having participants self-report on which of their activities they perceive to be goal-directed and calculate the frequency from such self-report. However, there would be limitations to such an alternative method as well, as varying definitions of the goal-directed activity or lack of insight could confound findings. Definitions of unproductive (i.e. non-goal-directed) activities assessed by EMA are also still evolving (Strassnig et al., Reference Strassnig, Harvey, Miller, Depp and Granholm2021).

We measured frequency by assessing the number of time-points (controlling for adherence) for which at least one goal-directed activity was endorsed. As documented in our methods, we focused on at least one goal-directed activity rather than the total number of reported goal-directed activities at each prompt since we (1) combined reports of past and current activities to comprise completed goal-directed behavior and did not want to double count a single activity that may have been endorsed in both of these sections, (2) did not measure the length of engagement in each goal-directed behavior and wanted to avoid devaluing a single prolonged goal-directed activity with multiple shorter ones, and (3) wanted to avoid conflating differences in response behavior with frequency of goal-directed activities. However, there are also limitations to this method such as likely instances in which a single, short goal-directed activity was coded the same as multiple extended goal-directed activities. Future work could benefit by assessing the length of time spent on goal-directed behaviors, having participants indicate exact activities (rather than picking between categories) to assess for follow-through on specific goal-directed tasks, and distinguishing between behaviors to pursue long versus short-term goals.

Our results present correlates of real-world goal-directed behavior that largely align with impaired ability to make future estimations in schizophrenia. Future research should determine whether motivated behavior deficits in schizophrenia are largely at the level of planning, whether difficulty planning for goal-directed activities in schizophrenia also hinders their completion, and whether it does so beyond other populations. For example, if someone is unable to project into the future to plan for activities that would bring about their goals, it is possible that they would complete fewer goals as a result, even if they end up completing more goals than they predicted. Additionally, research could assess whether support from others (e.g. in-the-moment reminders) assists more in the completion than the planning of goal-directed behavior. If so, interventions that target planning difficulty directly (serving almost as an external prefrontal cortex) might also increase the completion of goal-directed activities. For example, mobile interventions, that send reminder prompts to patients (e.g. via text message) have shown improvements in areas such as medication adherence and social interactions (Granholm, Ben-Zeev, Link, Bradshaw, & Holden, Reference Granholm, Ben-Zeev, Link, Bradshaw and Holden2012). In this vein, it is possible that similar mobile interventions could serve as a planning tool that could assist participants in completing daily personal goals. Overall, further research into what specifically inhibits goal-directed behavior in schizophrenia in comparison to other serious forms of mental illness could clarify the nature of this deficit and contribute to long-term functional benefit for this population.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721004281.

Funding Source: Gregory Couch Professorship held by DMB and NIMH Grant R01MH066031

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the participants in this study for their time on this project, and Dr Jo Etzel for her advice on coding the goal-directed behavior data.

Financial support

Data for this study was provided by the Cognition, emotion, motivation and reward study which was funded by the Gregory Couch Professorship held by DMB. Authors received additional funding support from NIMH grant R01-MH066031. The funding sources had no role in the design, analysis, or interpretation of the data.

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare there is no conflict of interest for this study.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Aleman, A., Lincoln, T. M., Bruggeman, R., Melle, I., Arends, J., Arango, C., & Knegtering, H. (2017). Treatment of negative symptoms: Where do we stand, and where do we go? Schizophrenia Research, 186, 5562. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2016.05.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barch, D. M., Pagliaccio, D., & Luking, K. (2015). Mechanisms underlying motivational deficits in psychopathology: Similarities and differences in depression and schizophrenia. In Simpson, E. H. & Balsam, P. D. (Eds.), Behavioral neuroscience of motivation (pp. 411449). Cham: Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2015_376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barch, D. M., Treadway, M., & Schoen, N. (2014). Effort, anhedonia, and function in schizophrenia: Reduced effort allocation predicts amotivation and functional impairment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123(2), 387397. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, , Aut, B., Cre, , Walker, S., Christensen, R. H. B., … simulate.formula, K., P. N. (shared copyright on. (2021). lme4: Linear Mixed-Effects Models using “Eigen” and S4 (Version 1.1–27.1). Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4.Google Scholar
Beck, A., Steer, R., & Brown, G. (1996). Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowie, C. R., Reichenberg, A., Patterson, T. L., Heaton, R. K., & Harvey, P. D. (2006). Determinants of real-world functional performance in schizophrenia subjects: Correlations with cognition, functional capacity, and symptoms. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 418425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Culbreth, A. J., Moran, E. K., & Barch, D. M. (2018). Effort-cost decision-making in psychosis and depression: Could a similar behavioral deficit arise from disparate psychological and neural mechanisms? Psychological Medicine, 48(6), 889904. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717002525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Culbreth, A. J., Moran, E. K., Kandala, S., Westbrook, A., & Barch, D. M. (2020). Effort, avolition, and motivational experience in schizophrenia: Analysis of behavioral and neuroimaging data with relationships to daily motivational experience. Clinical Psychological Science, 8(3), 555568. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702620901558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards, C. J., Cella, M., Emsley, R., Tarrier, N., & Wykes, T. H. M. (2018). Exploring the relationship between the anticipation and experience of pleasure in people with schizophrenia: An experience sampling study. Schizophrenia Research, 202, 7279. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2018.06.040.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fervaha, G., Foussias, G., Agid, O., & Remington, G. (2014a). Impact of primary negative symptoms on functional outcomes in schizophrenia. European Psychiatry, 29(7), 449455. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2014.01.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fervaha, G., Foussias, G., Agid, O., & Remington, G. (2014b). Motivational and neurocognitive deficits are central to the prediction of longitudinal functional outcome in schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 130(4), 290299. https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.12289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foussias, G., Mann, S., Zakzanis, K. K., van Reekum, R., Agid, O., & Remington, G. (2011). Prediction of longitudinal functional outcomes in schizophrenia: The impact of baseline motivational deficits. Schizophrenia Research, 132(1), 2427. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2011.06.026.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gard, D. E., Kring, A. M., Gard, M. G., Horan, W. P., & Green, M. F. (2007). Anhedonia in schizophrenia: Distinctions between anticipatory and consummatory pleasure. Schizophrenia Research, 93(1), 253260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2007.03.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gard, D. E., Sanchez, A. H., Cooper, K., Fisher, M., Garrett, C., & Vinogradov, S. (2014). Do people with schizophrenia have difficulty anticipating pleasure, engaging in effortful behavior, or both? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123(4), 771782. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gold, J. M., Barch, D. M., Feuerstahler, L. M., Carter, C. S., MacDonald, A. W., Ragland, J. D., … Luck, S. J. (2019). Working memory impairment across psychotic disorders. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 45(4), 804812. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sby134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gold, J. M., Strauss, G. P., Waltz, J. A., Robinson, B. M., Brown, J. K., & Frank, M. J. (2013). Negative symptoms of schizophrenia are associated with abnormal effort-cost computations. Biological Psychiatry, 74(2), 130136. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.12.022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Granholm, E., Ben-Zeev, D., Link, P. C., Bradshaw, K. R., & Holden, J. L. (2012). Mobile assessment and treatment for schizophrenia (MATS): A pilot trial of an interactive text-messaging intervention for medication adherence, socialization, and auditory hallucinations. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38(3), 414425. psyh (2012-15724-010). https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbr155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granholm, E., Holden, J. L., Mikhael, T., Link, P. C., Swendsen, J., Depp, C., … Harvey, P. D. (2020). What do people with schizophrenia do all day? Ecological momentary assessment of real-world functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 46(2), 242251. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbz070.Google ScholarPubMed
Hartig, F., & Lohse, L. (2021). DHARMa: Residual Diagnostics for Hierarchical (Multi-Level / Mixed) Regression Models (Version 0.4.3). Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=DHARMa.Google Scholar
Heerey, E., & Gold, J. (2007). Patients with schizophrenia demonstrate dissociation between affective experience and motivated behavior. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 268278. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.116.2.268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holt, D. V., Wolf, J., Funke, J., Weisbrod, M., & Kaiser, S. (2013). Planning impairments in schizophrenia: Specificity, task independence and functional relevance. Schizophrenia Research, 149(1), 174179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2013.06.018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horan, W. P., Kring, A. M., Gur, R. E., Reise, S. P., & Blanchard, J. J. (2011). Development and psychometric validation of the clinical assessment interview for negative symptoms (CAINS). Schizophrenia Research, 132(2), 140145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2011.06.030.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, J., Yang, X., Lan, Y., Zhu, C., Liu, X., Wang, Y., … Chan, R. C. K. (2016). Neural substrates of the impaired effort expenditure decision making in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology, 30(6), 685696. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keefe, R. S. E., Poe, M., Walker, T. M., & Harvey, P. D. (2006). The relationship of the brief assessment of cognition in schizophrenia (BACS) to functional capacity and real-world functional outcome. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 260269. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390500360539.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kraus, M. S., Gold, J. M., Barch, D. M., Walker, T. M., Chun, C. A., Buchanan, R. W., … Keefe, R. S. E. (2020). The characteristics of cognitive neuroscience tests in a schizophrenia cognition clinical trial: Psychometric properties and correlations with standard measures. Schizophrenia Research: Cognition, 19, 100161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2019.100161.Google Scholar
Kring, A. M., Gur, R. E., Blanchard, J. J., Horan, W. P., & Reise, S. P. (2013). The clinical assessment interview for negative symptoms (CAINS): Final development and validation. American Journal of Psychiatry, 170(2), 165172. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12010109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mausbach, B. T., Bowie, C. R., Harvey, P. D., Twamley, E. W., Goldman, S. R., Jeste, D. V., & Patterson, T. L. (2008). Usefulness of the UCSD performance-based skills assessment (UPSA) for predicting residential independence in patients with chronic schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 42(4), 320327. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.12.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mausbach, B. T, Harvey, P. D, Goldman, S. R, Jeste, D. V, & Patterson, T. L. (2006). Development of a Brief Scale of Everyday Functioning in Persons with Serious Mental Illness. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33(6), 13641372. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbm014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, J. M., Treadway, M. T., Bennett, M. E., & Blanchard, J. J. (2016). Inefficient effort allocation and negative symptoms in individuals with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 170(2), 278284. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2015.12.017.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moran, E. K., Culbreth, A. J., & Barch, D. M. (2017). Ecological momentary assessment of negative symptoms in schizophrenia: Relationships to effort-based decision making and reinforcement learning. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126(1), 96105. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moran, E. K., & Kring, A. M. (2018). Anticipatory emotion in schizophrenia. Clinical Psychological Science, 6(1), 6375. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702617730877.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myin-Germeys, I., Nicolson, N. A., & Delespaul, P. A. E. G. (2001). The context of delusional experiences in the daily life of patients with schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 31(3), 489498. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291701003646.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patterson, T. L., Goldman, S., McKibbin, C. L., Hughs, T., & Jeste, D. V. (2001). UCSD performance-based skills assessment: Development of a new measure of everyday functioning for severely mentally ill adults. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 27(2), 235245. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a006870.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team. (2021). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (Version 1.4.1106) [R Foundation for Statistical Computing]. Vienna, Austria.Google Scholar
Schneider, L. C, & Struening, E. L. (1983). SLOF: a behavioral rating scale for assessing the mentally ill. Social Work Research and Abstracts, 19(3), 921. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/swra/19.3.9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Semkovska, M., Bédard, M.-A., Godbout, L., Limoge, F., & Stip, E. (2004). Assessment of executive dysfunction during activities of daily living in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 69(2), 289300. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2003.07.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seter, C., Giovannetti, T., Kessler, R. K., & Worth, S. (2011). Everyday action planning in schizophrenia. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 21(2), 27. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2010.544519.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siddiqui, I., Saperia, S., Fervaha, G., Da Silva, S., Jeffay, E., Zakzanis, K. K., … Foussias, G. (2019). Goal-directed planning and action impairments in schizophrenia evaluated in a virtual environment. Schizophrenia Research, 206, 400406. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2018.10.012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sitnikova, T., Goff, D., & Kuperberg, G. R. (2009). Neurocognitive abnormalities during comprehension of real-world goal-directed behaviors in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(2), 256. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015619.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strassnig, M. T., Harvey, P. D., Miller, M. L., Depp, C. A., & Granholm, E. (2021). Real world sedentary behavior and activity levels in patients with schizophrenia and controls: An ecological momentary assessment study. Mental Health and Physical Activity, 20, 100364. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mhpa.2020.100364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strassnig, M. T., Raykov, T., O'Gorman, C., Bowie, C. R., Sabbag, S., Durand, D., … Harvey, P. D. (2015). Determinants of different aspects of everyday outcome in schizophrenia: The roles of negative symptoms, cognition, and functional capacity. Schizophrenia Research, 165(1), 7682. psyh (2015-43647-012). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2015.03.033.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treadway, M. T., Buckholtz, J. W., Schwartzman, A. N., Lambert, W. E., & Zald, D. H. (2009). Worth the ‘EEfRT’? The effort expenditure for rewards task as an objective measure of motivation and anhedonia. PLoS ONE, 4(8), e6598. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0006598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treadway, M. T., Peterman, J. S., Zald, D. H., & Park, S. (2015). Impaired effort allocation in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 161(2), 382385. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2014.11.024.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Twamley, E. W., Doshi, R. R., Nayak, G. V., Palmer, B. W., Golshan, S., Heaton, R. K., … Jeste, D. V. (2002). Generalized cognitive impairments, ability to perform everyday tasks, and level of independence in community living situations of older patients with psychosis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(12), 20132020. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.159.12.2013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vachon, H., Viechtbauer, W., Rintala, A., & Myin-Germeys, I. (2019). Compliance and retention with the experience sampling method over the continuum of severe mental disorders: Meta-analysis and recommendations. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 21(12), e14475. https://doi.org/10.2196/14475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Velthorst, E., Koeter, M., Gaag, M., Nieman, D., Fett, A.-K., Smit, F., … de Haan, L. (2015). Adapted cognitive-behavioural therapy required for targeting negative symptoms in schizophrenia: Meta-analysis and meta-regression. Psychological Medicine, 45, 453465. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291714001147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitton, A. E., Merchant, J. T., & Lewandowski, K. E. (2020). Dissociable mechanisms underpinning effort-cost decision-making across the psychosis spectrum. Schizophrenia Research, 224, 133140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2020.09.014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic, clinical and behavioral characteristics

Figure 1

Table 2. Average reported frequency of daily goal-directed and non-goal-directed behaviors

Figure 2

Fig. 1. Percent of time points for which at least one goal-directed activity was endorsed as planned (M = 66, Med = 70, s.d. = 26) or completed (M = 74, Med = 85, s.d. = 28).

Figure 3

Fig. 2. (a) Plot of CAINS MAP scores and Goal-Directed Activities with 95% confidence bands. Of note- the y axis reflects the residualized values of BDI and goal-directed behavior in order to control for that variable. (b): Plot of probability increase scores and goal-directed activities with 95% confidence bands. Of note- this graph represents the data that excludes the significant probability increase outlier. (c) Plot of UPSA scores and goal-directed activities with 95% confidence bands.

Figure 4

Table 3. GLMMs of clinical and behavioral measure relationships to goal-directed behavior (controlling for adherence)

Supplementary material: File

Merchant et al. supplementary material

Merchant et al. supplementary material

Download Merchant et al. supplementary material(File)
File 28.2 KB