Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-cx56b Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-20T03:40:13.533Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The development of cognitive flexibility and its implications for mental health disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 September 2024

Ke Tong
Affiliation:
Cambridge-NTU Centre for Lifelong Learning and Individualised Cognition (CLIC), Nanyang Technological University, Singapore
Xinchen Fu
Affiliation:
Cambridge-NTU Centre for Lifelong Learning and Individualised Cognition (CLIC), Nanyang Technological University, Singapore
Natalie P. Hoo
Affiliation:
Cambridge-NTU Centre for Lifelong Learning and Individualised Cognition (CLIC), Nanyang Technological University, Singapore
Lee Kean Mun
Affiliation:
Cambridge-NTU Centre for Lifelong Learning and Individualised Cognition (CLIC), Nanyang Technological University, Singapore Early Mental Potential and Wellbeing Research (EMPOWER) Centre, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore
Chrysoula Vassiliu
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
Christelle Langley
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
Barbara J. Sahakian*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
Victoria Leong
Affiliation:
Cambridge-NTU Centre for Lifelong Learning and Individualised Cognition (CLIC), Nanyang Technological University, Singapore Early Mental Potential and Wellbeing Research (EMPOWER) Centre, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore
*
Corresponding author: Barbara J. Sahakian; Email: bjs1001@cam.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Editorial
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Overview

Cognitive flexibility (CF) represents the ability to adapt one's thinking and behavior in response to changing environmental demands (Uddin, Reference Uddin2021). CF is multifaceted and involves a range of skills, including attentional shifting, strategy updating, response to feedback, reversal learning, exploration, and task switching. As a core component of executive function (EF), CF works in tandem with working memory and inhibitory control to facilitate goal-oriented behavior (Friedman & Robbins, Reference Friedman and Robbins2022). However, this editorial will focus on the development of CF and its implications for mental health disorders. CF is also impaired in a number of mental health disorders, including autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (Hughes, Russell, & Robbins, Reference Hughes, Russell and Robbins1994), obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) (Gottwald et al., Reference Gottwald, de Wit, Apergis-Schoute, Morein-Zamir, Kaser, Cormack and Sahakian2018; Vaghi et al., Reference Vaghi, Vértes, Kitzbichler, Apergis-Schoute, van der Flier, Fineberg and Robbins2017), and schizophrenia (Murray et al., Reference Murray, Cheng, Clark, Barnett, Blackwell, Fletcher and Jones2008). CF exhibits a prolonged maturational developmental trajectory, although early precursors of these skills can already be measured from infancy. Figure 1 provides a graphical illustration of the lifespan trajectory of CF development during infancy, adolescence, young adulthood, and older adulthood. This is also important considering that many mental health disorders begin in childhood and adolescence. Here, we discuss key environmental factors that may be important for shaping CF development across different life stages and their implications for mental health.

Figure 1. Developmental trajectory of CF maturation across the lifespan. The yellow curve is a diagrammatic representation of CF maturation across development in a healthy population (NB: this is not intended to indicate performance on a specific empirical measure). Notable milestones and inflections in the CF developmental trajectory are highlighted. These are expected to vary as a function of environmental influences and individual differences.

Development of cognitive flexibility

Neurobiological origins

The emergence of CF is linked to maturation of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and inferior parietal cortex (Ezekiel, Bosma, & Morton, Reference Ezekiel, Bosma and Morton2013). Although the PFC is relatively late maturing, this region already begins to undergo synaptic pruning and myelination from the first year of life (Collin & van den Heuvel, Reference Collin and van den Heuvel2013). Between 1 and 2 years, there is a pronounced acceleration in the volume of prefrontal gray matter, with expansion in both cortical thickness and surface area (Gilmore et al., Reference Gilmore, Shi, Woolson, Knickmeyer, Short, Lin and Shen2012). During the early years, external environmental stimuli are important for shaping the developmental trajectory of the PFC (Chini & Hanganu-Opatz, Reference Chini and Hanganu-Opatz2021). Specifically, factors such as caregiver interactions (Nelson, Reference Nelson2007), pronounced sensory deprivation (McLaughlin, Sheridan, & Lambert, Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan and Lambert2014), prenatal exposure to substances (Mackey, Raizada, & Bunge, Reference Mackey, Raizada, Bunge, Bunge, Toga, Stuss and Knight2013), and early adverse experiences (Hodel, Reference Hodel2018) can impact PFC maturation. Neurotransmitter systems are involved in the neuromodulation of CF. For example, studies have shown that serotonin (Chamberlain et al., Reference Chamberlain, Muller, Blackwell, Clark, Robbins and Sahakian2006; Skandali et al., Reference Skandali, Rowe, Voon, Deakin, Cardinal, Cormack and Sahakian2018) and dopamine (Dang, Donde, Madison, O'Neil, & Jagust, Reference Dang, Donde, Madison, O'Neil and Jagust2012) both affect performance on CF tasks. In contrast noradrenaline does not seem to affect at least some CF tasks such as the CANTAB IED (Chamberlain et al., Reference Chamberlain, Muller, Blackwell, Clark, Robbins and Sahakian2006).

Measuring emerging cognitive flexibility

Precursors of CF such as attention set-shifting, reversal learning, and overcoming perseveration begin to emerge during infancy. Specifically, one can assess infants' ability to overcome perseveration and engage in reversal learning from as early as 6–12 months (de Sousa, de Gil, & McIlvane, Reference de Sousa, de Gil and McIlvane2015). The sequential touching task (Ellis & Oakes, Reference Ellis and Oakes2006) has been adapted as an infant CF measure to assess flexible attention set-shifting from 12 months of age, particularly when the shift is scaffolded by a social partner (Fig. 2a). Piaget's A-not-B task, a classic test of infant cognitive development, requires basic shifting, memory and inhibitory control skills, and children still make errors on this task until ~12 months of age (MacNeill, Ram, Bell, Fox, & Pérez-Edgar, Reference MacNeill, Ram, Bell, Fox and Pérez-Edgar2018). Together with other core EF skills, CF development accelerates during the preschool years (Hughes, Reference Hughes1998) and is only thought to reach maturity during late childhood or early adolescence (Kupis & Uddin, Reference Kupis and Uddin2023). In children, a more formal measure of rule switching is the Dimensional Change Card Sort task (DCCS, Fig. 2b), in which children are asked to sort cards according to one dimension (e.g. color, shape, or number) and this sorting role is changed after several trials. While 4-year-old children are typically able to switch successfully between different dimensions, 3-year-old children tend to perseverate on one dimension (Doebel & Zelazo, Reference Doebel and Zelazo2015). In the Intra-Extra Dimensional set shift task (IED, from Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery), typically-developing 5-year-old children already display successful attentional set-shifting, but 7 to 18-year-old children with autism show significant CF dysfunction (Hughes et al., Reference Hughes, Russell and Robbins1994; Langley, Sahakian, & Robbins, Reference Langley, Sahakian, Robbins, Boyle, Stein, Stern, Sahakian, Golden, Lee and Chen2023). Childhood CF is linked to essential life outcomes such as social skills, learning, financial stability, and overall well-being (Arán Filippetti & Krumm, Reference Arán Filippetti and Krumm2020; Broomell & Bell, Reference Broomell and Bell2022).

Figure 2. Examples of age-appropriate CF tasks for infants, children, and adults. (a) In the Sequential Touching and Object Categorization (STOC) task, infants are presented with objects that can be categorized by either a high-salience dimension (e.g. shape: balls v. blocks) or a low-salience dimension (e.g. material: soft v. hard). This task comprises three phases: phase 1- infant free play; phase 2- parent demonstration of toy material (compressibility); phase 3- infant free play. The STOC measures flexible attention set-shifting in infants' mental categorization of toy objects from 12 months of age, particularly when the shift is scaffolded by a social partner (Tan & Leong, Reference Tan and Leong2023). (b) In the Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS) task, children sort cards based on one dimension (e.g. color) and after several trials, they are instructed to switch and sort by another dimension (e.g. shape). The task assesses their ability to shift between different sets of rules and adapt to new instructions (Zelazo, Reference Zelazo2006). (c) In the Wisconsin Card Sort Task (WCST), participants sort a target card into one of four decks without knowing the initial sorting rule. After each sort, they receive feedback on its correctness. From this feedback, they must infer the underlying rule. After several consecutive correct responses, the sorting rule changes without notice, challenging participants to detect the shift and adjust their strategy accordingly (Tong et al., Reference Tong, Chan, Cheng, Cheon, Ellefson and Fauziana2023).

Adolescence and young adulthood

During adolescence, performance on CF tasks continues to improve in tandem with increasing brain specialization (Kupis & Uddin, Reference Kupis and Uddin2023). Although adolescent CF approximates adult levels from ~12 years (Huizinga, Dolan, & van der Molen, Reference Huizinga, Dolan and van der Molen2006), peak performance is achieved approximately between the ages of 21 and 30 (Cepeda, Kramer, & Gonzalez de Sather, Reference Cepeda, Kramer and Gonzalez de Sather2001), during which the frontal-striatal networks maturation contributes to both cognitive and behavioral flexibility processes (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Kundu, Dowell, Mechelmans, Favre, Irvine and Voon2016). The maturation of EF during adolescence may have interesting associations with higher levels of risk-taking behavior observed during this stage, as teenagers shift their priorities to avoid peer rejection, and inhibit health or legal concerns to engage in more risky behaviors (Blakemore, Reference Blakemore2018). Studies employing a latent-factor approach suggest that a single unified EF factor may best describe the capacities of young children up to ~8 years of age, however by age 10, two separable EF components (putatively memory and ‘general’ EF) may be identified using statistical models (Brydges, Fox, Reid, & Anderson, Reference Brydges, Fox, Reid and Anderson2014). In young adults, a model of three correlated factors, i.e., working memory, inhibitory control, and CF, emerges as the best EF model, termed ‘unity and diversity’ (Friedman & Miyake, Reference Friedman and Miyake2017). Although this data fits the narrative of a developmental transition from relatively undifferentiated unidimensional EF to separable but correlated EF components, the relative paucity (and specificity) of age-appropriate tasks to measure emerging EF during infancy and childhood confounds this interpretation. Many child EF tasks require language skills to comprehend task instructions, which precludes their use in preverbal children and presents additional non-EF related task demands. There are mental health disorders which affect and impair the development of cognitive flexible thinking which start in childhood or adolescence, such as ASD (Hughes et al., Reference Hughes, Russell and Robbins1994), OCD (Gottwald et al., Reference Gottwald, de Wit, Apergis-Schoute, Morein-Zamir, Kaser, Cormack and Sahakian2018; Vaghi et al., Reference Vaghi, Vértes, Kitzbichler, Apergis-Schoute, van der Flier, Fineberg and Robbins2017), and schizophrenia (Murray et al., Reference Murray, Cheng, Clark, Barnett, Blackwell, Fletcher and Jones2008). Indeed, children and adolescents with OCD have impaired functioning at school and at home and experience severe distress. Critical cognitive domains for daily functioning and academic success are learning, memory, CF and goal-directed behavioral control. These domains, particularly learning and memory as well goal-directed control and cognitive plasticity are impaired early in the development of OCD (Gottwald et al., Reference Gottwald, de Wit, Apergis-Schoute, Morein-Zamir, Kaser, Cormack and Sahakian2018). In adults with OCD, a severe impairment in CF has been shown. Moreover, this impairment is likely due to disruptions in the fronto-striatal circuitry (Vaghi et al., Reference Vaghi, Vértes, Kitzbichler, Apergis-Schoute, van der Flier, Fineberg and Robbins2017) that typically subserve CF.

Older adulthood

As individuals age, cognitive abilities typically decline, including CF (Murman, Reference Murman2015). This CF decline presents as increased perseverative behaviors measurable in tests such as the Wisconsin Card Sort Task (WCST, Fig. 2c) (Ashendorf & McCaffrey, Reference Ashendorf and McCaffrey2008). A heightened propensity for older adults to perseverate can be linked to their declined set-shifting capabilities (Ridderinkhof, Span, & van der Molen, Reference Ridderinkhof, Span and van der Molen2002). At the neural level, the PFC experiences significantly greater gray matter volume loss during normal ageing as compared to other developmental stages (Raz et al., Reference Raz, Gunning, Head, Dupuis, McQuain, Briggs and Acker1997), which contributes to the age-related decline of EF and CF. Age-related neurodegenerative diseases, such as mild cognitive impairment, dementia, and Alzheimer disease (AD), can speed up neuronal dysfunction and exacerbate cognitive declines, including CF (Guarino, Forte, Giovannoli, & Casagrande, Reference Guarino, Forte, Giovannoli and Casagrande2020). Recent functional imaging studies investigating CF-related brain network dynamics suggest that older adults who have extended dwell time in co-activation among the lateral frontoparietal network (L-FPN, ‘executive control’ network) and medial frontoparietal network (M-FPN, ‘default’ network) show diminished CF, compared with young adults (Kupis et al., Reference Kupis, Goodman, Kornfeld, Hoang, Romero, Dirks and Uddin2021).

In older adults, CF also appears to have a significant impact on motor control. Older adults demonstrate reduced motor flexibility when switching between different walking patterns, which is positively associated with higher levels of cognitive perseveration (Sombric & Torres-Oviedo, Reference Sombric and Torres-Oviedo2021). This connection suggests a shared mechanism that governs both cognitive and motor perseveration as individuals age. As older adults with diminished CF (measured by WCST) have a greater risk of losing balance and falling (Pieruccini-Faria, Lord, Toson, Kemmler, & Schoene, Reference Pieruccini-Faria, Lord, Toson, Kemmler and Schoene2019), interventions focusing on enhancing cognitive and motor flexibility could be instrumental in maintaining the quality of life for older adults by preventing falls.

More longitudinal data are required to understand CF across the lifespan, however when examining young children, there are some data to suggest that EF assessed by a battery of tests at 24 months does relate to EF assessed at age 4 (see Miller, Galvagno, & Elgier, Reference Miller, Galvagno and Elgier2023). Stability prior to that is unclear as studies have given mixed results. There are cross sectional data on CF across the lifespan using the CANTAB IED, which shows that CF performance improves from childhood to adolescence and is optimal in early young adulthood and then remains stable until age about 50 and slowly declines during older adulthood (Langley et al., Reference Langley, Sahakian, Robbins, Boyle, Stein, Stern, Sahakian, Golden, Lee and Chen2023).

Potential intervention strategies to improve CF across the lifespan

Quality of early caregiving

The quality of early caregiving is an important and modifiable factor that affects early CF development. Parental social interactive behaviors, including sensitivity, have been linked to higher CF abilities in children (Bernier, Carlson, Deschênes, & Matte-Gagné, Reference Bernier, Carlson, Deschênes and Matte-Gagné2012) whilst traumatic social experiences can negatively impact children's EF, including CF (Kavanaugh, Dupont-Frechette, Jerskey, & Holler, Reference Kavanaugh, Dupont-Frechette, Jerskey and Holler2017). One promising parent-based EF training program is the Attachment and Biobehavioral Catch-up intervention for infants (ABC-I). This 10-session home-based program fosters nurturing and synchronous parent-child interactions (Dozier & Bernard, Reference Dozier and Bernard2017) and has been shown to positively affect attachment security, emotion expression, and cognitive control in children whose foster parents underwent training (Bernard, Hostinar, & Dozier, Reference Bernard, Hostinar and Dozier2015). These encouraging data highlight the potential for home-based programs that target parent-child relationships to enhance early EF development.

Lifestyle factors

A range of lifestyle factors, such as sleep, exercise, nutrition, stress management, social connection, and learning new skills, can enhance EF and combat the cognitive decline associated with ageing (Beddington et al., Reference Beddington, Cooper, Field, Goswami, Huppert, Jenkins and Thomas2008). Here, we focus on stress-management via mindfulness and exercise as an illustrative example for potential intervention. Chronic and acute stress can negatively affect CF throughout one's life. For example, stress can hinder attentional shifting in infants (Seehagen, Schneider, Rudolph, Ernst, & Zmyj, Reference Seehagen, Schneider, Rudolph, Ernst and Zmyj2015) and task-switching abilities in young adults (Plessow, Kiesel, & Kirschbaum, Reference Plessow, Kiesel and Kirschbaum2012). Moreover, the brain is particularly vulnerable to toxic stress during early life and older adulthood (Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar, & Heim, Reference Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar and Heim2009), so preventative interventions are indicated during these life stages. Research suggests that mindfulness practice and exercise may be efficacious in reducing stress (Vatansever, Wang, & Sahakian, Reference Vatansever, Wang and Sahakian2021) and boosting EF (Lerche et al., Reference Lerche, Gutfreund, Brockmann, Hobert, Wurster, Sünkel and Berg2018). Notably, exercises incorporating mindfulness, like Tai-Chi and Taekwondo, are more effective in improving EFs compared to standard resistance and aerobic exercises (Diamond & Ling, Reference Diamond and Ling2020). One explanation is that mindful exercises demand greater cognitive control, but further research is needed the clarify mechanistic relationships between CF, mindfulness, and general well-being.

Language factors

The relationship between bi-/multilingualism and EFs, including CF, has generated significant scientific discussion. Studies suggest that bilinguals maintain constant activations for both their languages (Thierry & Wu, Reference Thierry and Wu2007) and these continuously active language representations compete for selection during language use, necessitating monitoring and control from bilinguals to achieve successful communication (Valian, Reference Valian2015). Mechanisms of selection, inhibition and shifting operate in tandem to manage interference but also facilitate language switching when necessary (Gallo, Novitskiy, Myachykov, & Shtyrov, Reference Gallo, Novitskiy, Myachykov and Shtyrov2021). Clearly, bilinguals need to exercise extensive linguistic control to use their languages effectively, which may be related to domain-general cognitive control, as measured in EF tasks. Despite active research, results remain inconclusive. Several behavioral and neuroimaging studies report better EF performance in bilinguals (Barac, Moreno, & Bialystok, Reference Barac, Moreno and Bialystok2016), but others dispute the existence of any advantage (Duñabeitia et al., Reference Duñabeitia, Hernández, Antón, Macizo, Estévez, Fuentes and Carreiras2014). This lack of consensus may be due in part to inconsistent measurement of bilingualism as a binary variable rather than as a multifactorial continuum, which obscures deeper differences between monolingual and bilingual groups (Kaushanskaya & Prior, Reference Kaushanskaya and Prior2015). To advance the field, longitudinal studies and training studies that allow for more careful dissection of causal relationships between multilingualism and mental flexibility are needed. Indicatively, short second-language (L2) learning interventions yield benefits for attention switching in both children (Janus, Lee, Moreno, & Bialystok, Reference Janus, Lee, Moreno and Bialystok2016) and adults (Bak, Long, Vega-Mendoza, & Sorace, Reference Bak, Long, Vega-Mendoza and Sorace2016). Explicit training in a language-switching paradigm has shown potential transfer outside the linguistic domain, manifesting in reduced switching (Timmer, Calabria, & Costa, Reference Timmer, Calabria and Costa2019) and mixing costs (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Yang, Jiao, Schwieter, Sun and Wang2019) in adult bilinguals. Therefore, learning a new language may potentially be an effective strategy for improving CF throughout the lifespan.

Future directions and conclusions

The protracted developmental trajectory of CF maturation presents both vulnerability to adverse environmental effects and opportunities for intervention. For instance, during the early years CF interventions might prioritize improving the quality of caregiving through parent-based interventions. During adolescence, peer-to-peer influences within the school environment are of particular importance. Therefore, embedding CF-oriented pedagogy alongside team-based approaches that enhance creativity and inventiveness into school programs and assessments may enhance adolescents' CF and their readiness for the future (Stad, Wiedl, Vogelaar, Bakker, & Resing, Reference Stad, Wiedl, Vogelaar, Bakker and Resing2019).

Maintaining healthy lifestyle habits that boost CF can offer lasting advantages for adults. However, the real challenge often lies in cultivating these habits and sustaining engagement. While gamification techniques might enhance participation in intervention programs (Kappen, Mirza-Babaei, & Nacke, Reference Kappen, Mirza-Babaei and Nacke2020), addressing underlying factors influencing lifestyle choices – like socioeconomic disparities, work-life balance, and social ties – requires broader societal change.

Game-based CF training has potential to enhance CF across the lifespan and may be particularly appealing to children and adolescents (Johann & Karbach, Reference Johann and Karbach2020). However, challenges for this field include the age-appropriate adaptation of tasks and ensuring far transfer of training benefits to real-life scenarios. Recent studies have developed innovative paradigms to address these issues, aiming to translate lab-based paradigms to games with superior transfer in real-life contexts, also in clinical trials and practice (Hauser, Iannaccone, Walitza, Brandeis, & Brem, Reference Hauser, Iannaccone, Walitza, Brandeis and Brem2015; Langley et al., Reference Langley, Sahakian, Robbins, Boyle, Stein, Stern, Sahakian, Golden, Lee and Chen2023). In twin studies it has been shown that CF is impacted to a greater extent by environmental factors in contrast to genetic ones, which have been shown to be of relatively low influence, particularly when compared to other EF tasks, for example working memory (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Mosing, Henry, Trollor, Ames and Martin2012). This suggests that CF may be a target which could be improved through training.

In summary, CF skills can be fostered and improved at all life stages, though different interventions may be suitable at each age. To harness this potential, more research is required on CF training to address inconsistent findings and ambiguous transfer effects (Dougherty, Hamovitz, & Tidwell, Reference Dougherty, Hamovitz and Tidwell2016). For example, one study has shown that CF is separable from EF (Feng et al., Reference Feng, Zhang, Chen, Sheng, Ye, Feng and Xue2022), as such training of CF may not have far transfer to other EFs. Nevertheless, many processes such as adaptive learning require CF, and this would be a core component of other EFs, for example problem solving. Therefore, there may be some transfer of CF training to other EFs. Given the importance of CF for lifelong learning, problem-solving, and the mental health of individuals (Buttelmann & Karbach, Reference Buttelmann and Karbach2017), further research is essential to better understand associated brain plasticity mechanisms, and to broaden our understanding of the construct and its malleability by social and environmental factors across the lifespan. Furthermore, due to the importance of CF for learning and problem-solving, greater attention needs to be focused on deficits in CF in patients with mental health disorders. It may be possible to improve CF and therefore the impact of impairments in CF may be mitigated if detected early in patients by psychiatrists and psychologists.

Funding statement

This work was supported by grants to the Cambridge-NTU Centre for Lifelong Learning and Individualised Cognition (CLIC), a project by the National Research Foundation, Prime Minister's Office, Singapore, under its Campus for Research Excellence and Technological Enterprise (NRF-CREATE SoL) Programme with the funding administered by the Cambridge Centre for Advanced Research and Education in Singapore Ltd. (CARES) and housed at the Centre for Research and Development in Learning (CRADLE@NTU). VL is supported by the Ministry of Education, Singapore, under its Academic Research Fund Tier 2 (MOE-T2EP40121-0001) and Social Science & Humanities Research Fellowship (MOE2020-SSHR-008), and by A*STAR under its Human Potential Programme Prenatal/Early Childhood Grant (H22P0M0002). The research of BJS and CL is supported by the NIHR Cambridge Biomedical Research Centre (BRC-1215-20014) and NIHR Applied Research Centre.

Disclosures

BJS consults for Cambridge Cognition.

Footnotes

*

Equal Contribution.

References

Arán Filippetti, V., & Krumm, G. (2020). A hierarchical model of cognitive flexibility in children: Extending the relationship between flexibility, creativity and academic achievement. Child Neuropsychology, 26(6), 770800. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2019.1711034CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashendorf, L., & McCaffrey, R. J. (2008). Exploring age-related decline on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 22(2), 262272. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854040701218436CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bak, T. H., Long, M. R., Vega-Mendoza, M., & Sorace, A. (2016). Novelty, challenge, and practice: The impact of intensive language learning on attentional functions. PLoS ONE, 11(4), e0153485. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0153485CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barac, R., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E. (2016). Behavioral and electrophysiological differences in executive control between monolingual and bilingual children. Child Development, 87(4), 12771290. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12538CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beddington, J., Cooper, C. L., Field, J., Goswami, U., Huppert, F. A., Jenkins, R., … Thomas, S. M. (2008). The mental wealth of nations. Nature, 455(7216), 10571060. https://doi.org/10.1038/4551057aCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernard, K., Hostinar, C. E., & Dozier, M. (2015). Intervention effects on diurnal cortisol rhythms of child protective services-referred infants in early childhood: Preschool follow-up results of a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatrics, 169(2), 112119. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2014.2369CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., Deschênes, M., & Matte-Gagné, C. (2012). Social factors in the development of early executive functioning: A closer look at the caregiving environment. Developmental Science, 15(1), 1224. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01093.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blakemore, S.-J. (2018). Avoiding social risk in adolescence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(2), 116122. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417738144CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broomell, A., & Bell, M. A. (2022). Longitudinal development of executive function from infancy to late childhood. Cognitive Development, 63, 101229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2022.101229CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brydges, C. R., Fox, A. M., Reid, C. L., & Anderson, M. (2014). The differentiation of executive functions in middle and late childhood: A longitudinal latent-variable analysis. Intelligence, 47, 3443. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2014.08.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buttelmann, F., & Karbach, J. (2017). Development and plasticity of cognitive flexibility in early and middle childhood. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 258078. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01040CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cepeda, N. J., Kramer, A. F., & Gonzalez de Sather, J. C. (2001). Changes in executive control across the life span: Examination of task-switching performance. Developmental Psychology, 37(5), 715730.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chamberlain, S. R., Muller, U., Blackwell, A. D., Clark, L., Robbins, T. W., & Sahakian, B. J. (2006). Neurochemical modulation of response inhibition and probabilistic learning in humans. Science (New York, N.Y.), 311(5762), 861863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chini, M., & Hanganu-Opatz, I. L. (2021). Prefrontal cortex development in health and disease: Lessons from rodents and humans. Trends in Neurosciences, 44(3), 227240. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2020.10.017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collin, G., & van den Heuvel, M. P. (2013). The ontogeny of the human connectome: Development and dynamic changes of brain connectivity across the life span. The Neuroscientist, 19(6), 616628. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858413503712CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dang, L. C., Donde, A., Madison, C., O'Neil, J. P., & Jagust, W. J. (2012). Striatal dopamine influences the default mode network to affect shifting between object features. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(9), 19601970.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Sousa, N. M., de Gil, M. S. C. A., & McIlvane, W. J. (2015). Discrimination and reversal learning by toddlers aged 15–23 months. The Psychological Record, 65(1), 4147. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-014-0084-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A., & Ling, D. S. (2020). Review of the evidence on, and fundamental questions about, efforts to improve executive functions, including working memory. In Cognitive and working memory training: Perspectives from psychology, neuroscience, and human development (pp. 143431). New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780199974467.003.0008Google Scholar
Doebel, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2015). A meta-analysis of the dimensional change card sort: Implications for developmental theories and the measurement of executive function in children. Developmental Review: DR, 38, 241268. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2015.09.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dougherty, M. R., Hamovitz, T., & Tidwell, J. W. (2016). Reevaluating the effectiveness of n-back training on transfer through the Bayesian lens: Support for the null. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(1), 306316. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0865-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dozier, M., & Bernard, K. (2017). Attachment and biobehavioral catch-up: Addressing the needs of infants and toddlers exposed to inadequate or problematic caregiving. Current Opinion in Psychology, 15, 111117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duñabeitia, J. A., Hernández, J. A., Antón, E., Macizo, P., Estévez, A., Fuentes, L. J., & Carreiras, M. (2014). The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited. Experimental Psychology, 61(3), 234251. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000243CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, A. E., & Oakes, L. M. (2006). Infants flexibly use different dimensions to categorize objects. Developmental Psychology, 42(6), 10001011. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1000CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ezekiel, F., Bosma, R., & Morton, J. B. (2013). Dimensional change card sort performance associated with age-related differences in functional connectivity of lateral prefrontal cortex. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 4050. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2012.12.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feng, J., Zhang, L., Chen, C., Sheng, J., Ye, Z., Feng, K., … Xue, G. (2022). A cognitive neurogenetic approach to uncovering the structure of executive functions. Nature Communications, 13(1), 4588.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2017). Unity and diversity of executive functions: Individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 86, 186204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2016.04.023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, N. P., & Robbins, T. W. (2022). The role of prefrontal cortex in cognitive control and executive function. Neuropsychopharmacology, 47(1), 118. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-021-01132-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallo, F., Novitskiy, N., Myachykov, A., & Shtyrov, Y. (2021). Individual differences in bilingual experience modulate executive control network and performance: Behavioral and structural neuroimaging evidence. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 24(2), 293304. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728920000486CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilmore, J. H., Shi, F., Woolson, S. L., Knickmeyer, R. C., Short, S. J., Lin, W., … Shen, D. (2012). Longitudinal development of cortical and subcortical gray matter from birth to 2 years. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y.: 1991), 22(11), 24782485. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr327CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottwald, J., de Wit, S., Apergis-Schoute, A. M., Morein-Zamir, S., Kaser, M., Cormack, F., … Sahakian, B. J. (2018). Impaired cognitive plasticity and goal-directed control in adolescent obsessive–compulsive disorder. Psychological Medicine, 48(11), 19001908.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guarino, A., Forte, G., Giovannoli, J., & Casagrande, M. (2020). Executive functions in the elderly with mild cognitive impairment: A systematic review on motor and cognitive inhibition, conflict control and cognitive flexibility. Aging & Mental Health, 24(7), 10281045. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2019.1584785CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauser, T. U., Iannaccone, R., Walitza, S., Brandeis, D., & Brem, S. (2015). Cognitive flexibility in adolescence: Neural and behavioral mechanisms of reward prediction error processing in adaptive decision making during development. NeuroImage, 104, 347354. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hodel, A. S. (2018). Rapid infant prefrontal cortex development and sensitivity to early environmental experience. Developmental Review: DR, 48, 113144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2018.02.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, C. (1998). Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16(2), 233253. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1998.tb00921.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, C., Russell, J., & Robbins, T. W. (1994). Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia, 32(4), 477492. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(94)90092-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huizinga, M., Dolan, C. V., & van der Molen, M. W. (2006). Age-related change in executive function: Developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 20172036. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janus, M., Lee, Y., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E. (2016). Effects of short-term music and second-language training on executive control. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 144, 8497. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.11.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johann, V. E., & Karbach, J. (2020). Effects of game-based and standard executive control training on cognitive and academic abilities in elementary school children. Developmental Science, 23(4), e12866. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12866CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kappen, D. L., Mirza-Babaei, P., & Nacke, L. E. (2020). Technology facilitates physical activity through gamification: A thematic analysis of an 8-week study. Frontiers in Computer Science, 2, 530309. Retrieved from https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fcomp.2020.530309CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaushanskaya, M., & Prior, A. (2015). Variability in the effects of bilingualism on cognition: It is not just about cognition, it is also about bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 2728. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000510CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kavanaugh, B. C., Dupont-Frechette, J. A., Jerskey, B. A., & Holler, K. A. (2017). Neurocognitive deficits in children and adolescents following maltreatment: Neurodevelopmental consequences and neuropsychological implications of traumatic stress. Applied Neuropsychology. Child, 6(1), 6478. https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2015.1079712CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kupis, L., Goodman, Z. T., Kornfeld, S., Hoang, S., Romero, C., Dirks, B., … Uddin, L. Q. (2021). Brain dynamics underlying cognitive flexibility across the lifespan. Cerebral Cortex, 31(11), 52635274. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhab156CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kupis, L., & Uddin, L. (2023). Developmental neuroimaging of cognitive flexibility: Update and future directions. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology, 5(1), 263–284. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-120221-035310CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langley, C., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2023). The SAGE handbook of clinical neuropsychology: Clinical neuropsychological assessment and diagnosis. In Boyle, G. J., Stein, D. J., Stern, Y., Sahakian, B. J., Golden, C. J., Lee, T. M.-C., & Chen, S.-H. A. (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of clinical neuropsychology: Clinical neuropsychological assessment and diagnosis (Vols 1–0, pp. 435468). 55 City Road: SAGE Publications Ltd. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781529789539CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, T., Mosing, M. A., Henry, J. D., Trollor, J. N., Ames, D., & Martin, N. G., … OATS Research Team. (2012). Genetic influences on four measures of executive functions and their covariation with general cognitive ability: The older Australian twins study. Behavior Genetics, 42, 528538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lerche, S., Gutfreund, A., Brockmann, K., Hobert, M. A., Wurster, I., Sünkel, U., … Berg, D. (2018). Effect of physical activity on cognitive flexibility, depression and RBD in healthy elderly. Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery, 165, 8893. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clineuro.2018.01.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, C., Yang, C.-L., Jiao, L., Schwieter, J. W., Sun, X., & Wang, R. (2019). Training in language switching facilitates bilinguals’ monitoring and inhibitory control. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 458621. Retrieved from https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01839CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., & Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 10(6), 434445. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2639CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mackey, A. P., Raizada, R. D. S., & Bunge, S. A. (2013). Environmental influences on prefrontal development. In Bunge, S. A., Toga, A. W., Stuss, D. T., & Knight, R. T. (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (p. 0). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/med/9780199837755.003.0013Google Scholar
MacNeill, L. A., Ram, N., Bell, M. A., Fox, N. A., & Pérez-Edgar, K. (2018). Trajectories of infants’ biobehavioral development: Timing and rate of A-not-B performance gains and EEG maturation. Child Development, 89(3), 711724. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, K. A., Sheridan, M. A., & Lambert, H. K. (2014). Childhood adversity and neural development: Deprivation and threat as distinct dimensions of early experience. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 47, 578591. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, S. E., Galvagno, L. G., & Elgier, Á (2023). Universality and context-specificity in early executive function development. Infant Behavior and Development, 71, 101841.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, L. S., Kundu, P., Dowell, N., Mechelmans, D. J., Favre, P., Irvine, M. A., … Voon, V. (2016). Fronto-striatal organization: Defining functional and microstructural substrates of behavioural flexibility. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 74, 118133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.11.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murman, D. L. (2015). The impact of age on cognition. Seminars in Hearing, 36(3), 111121. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0035-1555115Google ScholarPubMed
Murray, G. K., Cheng, F., Clark, L., Barnett, J. H., Blackwell, A. D., Fletcher, P. C., … Jones, P. B. (2008). Reinforcement and reversal learning in first-episode psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 34(5), 848855.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, C. A. (2007). A neurobiological perspective on early human deprivation. Child Development Perspectives, 1(1), 1318. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2007.00004.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pieruccini-Faria, F., Lord, S. R., Toson, B., Kemmler, W., & Schoene, D. (2019). Mental flexibility influences the association between poor balance and falls in older people – a secondary analysis. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 11. Retrieved from https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2019.00133CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plessow, F., Kiesel, A., & Kirschbaum, C. (2012). The stressed prefrontal cortex and goal-directed behaviour: Acute psychosocial stress impairs the flexible implementation of task goals. Experimental Brain Research, 216(3), 397408. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2943-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raz, N., Gunning, F. M., Head, D., Dupuis, J. H., McQuain, J., Briggs, S. D., … Acker, J. D. (1997). Selective aging of the human cerebral cortex observed in vivo: Differential vulnerability of the prefrontal gray matter. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y.: 1991), 7(3), 268282. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/7.3.268CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ridderinkhof, K. R., Span, M. M., & van der Molen, M. W. (2002). Perseverative behavior and adaptive control in older adults: Performance monitoring, rule induction, and set shifting. Brain and Cognition, 49(3), 382401. https://doi.org/10.1006/brcg.2001.1506CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seehagen, S., Schneider, S., Rudolph, J., Ernst, S., & Zmyj, N. (2015). Stress impairs cognitive flexibility in infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(41), 1288212886. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1508345112CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skandali, N., Rowe, J. B., Voon, V., Deakin, J. B., Cardinal, R. N., Cormack, F., … Sahakian, B. J. (2018). Dissociable effects of acute SSRI (escitalopram) on executive, learning and emotional functions in healthy humans. Neuropsychopharmacology, 43(13), 26452651.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sombric, C. J., & Torres-Oviedo, G. (2021). Cognitive and motor perseveration are associated in older adults. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 13, 610359. Retrieved from https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2021.610359CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stad, F. E., Wiedl, K. H., Vogelaar, B., Bakker, M., & Resing, W. C. M. (2019). The role of cognitive flexibility in young children's potential for learning under dynamic testing conditions. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 34(1), 123146. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-018-0379-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tan, X. X., & Leong, V. (2023). A protocol for social interactive assessment of infant attention set-shifting between 12–24 months of age. MethodsX, 11, 102273. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mex.2023.102273CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thierry, G., & Wu, Y. J. (2007). Brain potentials reveal unconscious translation during foreign-language comprehension. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(30), 1253012535. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0609927104CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Timmer, K., Calabria, M., & Costa, A. (2019). Non-linguistic effects of language switching training. Cognition, 182, 1424. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.09.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tong, K., Chan, Y. N., Cheng, X., Cheon, B., Ellefson, M., & Fauziana, R., … Consortium, C. P. 1. (2023). Study protocol: How does cognitive flexibility relate to other executive functions and learning in healthy young adults? PLoS ONE, 18(7), e0286208. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0286208CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uddin, L. Q. (2021). Cognitive and behavioural flexibility: Neural mechanisms and clinical considerations. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 22(3), 167179. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-021-00428-wCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaghi, M. M., Vértes, P. E., Kitzbichler, M. G., Apergis-Schoute, A. M., van der Flier, F. E., Fineberg, N. A., … Robbins, T. W. (2017). Specific frontostriatal circuits for impaired cognitive flexibility and goal-directed planning in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Evidence from resting-state functional connectivity. Biological Psychiatry, 81(8), 708717. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.08.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valian, V. (2015). Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 324. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000522CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vatansever, D., Wang, S., & Sahakian, B. J. (2021). COVID-19 and promising solutions to combat symptoms of stress, anxiety and depression. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 46(1), 217218. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-020-00791-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The dimensional change card sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols, 1(1), 297301. https://doi.org/10.1038/nprot.2006.46CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Developmental trajectory of CF maturation across the lifespan. The yellow curve is a diagrammatic representation of CF maturation across development in a healthy population (NB: this is not intended to indicate performance on a specific empirical measure). Notable milestones and inflections in the CF developmental trajectory are highlighted. These are expected to vary as a function of environmental influences and individual differences.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Examples of age-appropriate CF tasks for infants, children, and adults. (a) In the Sequential Touching and Object Categorization (STOC) task, infants are presented with objects that can be categorized by either a high-salience dimension (e.g. shape: balls v. blocks) or a low-salience dimension (e.g. material: soft v. hard). This task comprises three phases: phase 1- infant free play; phase 2- parent demonstration of toy material (compressibility); phase 3- infant free play. The STOC measures flexible attention set-shifting in infants' mental categorization of toy objects from 12 months of age, particularly when the shift is scaffolded by a social partner (Tan & Leong, 2023). (b) In the Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS) task, children sort cards based on one dimension (e.g. color) and after several trials, they are instructed to switch and sort by another dimension (e.g. shape). The task assesses their ability to shift between different sets of rules and adapt to new instructions (Zelazo, 2006). (c) In the Wisconsin Card Sort Task (WCST), participants sort a target card into one of four decks without knowing the initial sorting rule. After each sort, they receive feedback on its correctness. From this feedback, they must infer the underlying rule. After several consecutive correct responses, the sorting rule changes without notice, challenging participants to detect the shift and adjust their strategy accordingly (Tong et al., 2023).