Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T19:07:37.378Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Exploring decision-making performance in young adults with mental health disorders: a comparative study using the Cambridge gambling task

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 May 2024

R. Effah*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, University of Southampton, Southampton, UK Southern Health NHS Foundation Trust, Southampton, UK
K. Ioannidis
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, University of Southampton, Southampton, UK Southern Health NHS Foundation Trust, Southampton, UK
J.E. Grant
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Neuroscience, University of Chicago, Pritzker School of Medicine, Chicago, IL, USA
S.R. Chamberlain
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, University of Southampton, Southampton, UK Southern Health NHS Foundation Trust, Southampton, UK
*
Corresponding author: Raymond Effah; Email: rwe1g17@soton.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Decision-making deficits, assessed cognitively, are often associated with mental health symptoms, however, this relationship is not fully understood. This paper explores the relationship between mental health disorders and decision-making, using the Cambridge Gambling Task (CGT). Our study investigated how decision-making varied across 20 different mental health conditions compared to controls in a sample of 572 young adults from the Minneapolis and Chicago metropolitan areas, using a computerized laboratory-based task. Almost all mental health conditions were associated with at least mild (i.e. at least small effect size) impairment in all three studied parameters of the CGT (risk adjustment, quality of decision-making and overall proportion of bet). Notably, binge eating disorder had the largest cognitive impairment and gambling disorder had moderate impairment. Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) was associated with impaired decision-making while obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD) and depression showed moderate impairment. Additionally, half of the disorders assessed had moderate to large impairment in risk adjustment.These findings suggest that mental health conditions may have a more complex cognitive profile than previously thought, and a better understanding of these impairments may aid in risk assessment and targeted clinical interventions. This study underscores the need for further research to determine the causal pathways between mental health conditions and cognition, as well as to better understand the day-to-day impact of such deficits.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Cognition can be broadly defined as the mental action or process of acquiring knowledge and understanding through thought, experience, and the senses. There are dissociable cognitive functions that have been implicated across a range of mental health conditions. A wide range of mental health disorders are associated with poor working memory, impaired decision-making, and attentional impairments (Gould, Reference Gould2010; Grace, Reference Grace2016; Murphy et al., Reference Murphy, Rubinsztein, Michael, Rogers, Robbins, Paykel and Sahakian2001; Pironti et al., Reference Pironti, Lai, Müller, Dodds, Suckling, Bullmore and Sahakian2014; Rogers et al., Reference Rogers, Everitt, Baldacchino, Blackshaw, Swainson, Wynne and Robbins1999a).

The profile of cognitive impairment is important for understanding the neurobiological changes that may underpin these conditions, however pragmatically they are vital in understanding how pathologies manifest themselves in the day-to-day activities of individuals. Abnormalities of decision-making have been researched in mental health conditions. For example, a stark example of this relationship is in gambling disorder (as defined in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual Version 5 [DSM-5] [American Psychiatric Association, 2013]): affected individuals repeatedly gamble, and make unwise decisions, despite negative consequences. Meta-analyses and systematic reviews have shown the differences in aspects of decision-making in gambling disorder documenting increased risk taking, impulsivity, and impaired judgment (Grant, Chamberlain, Schreiber, Odlaug, & Kim, Reference Grant, Chamberlain, Schreiber, Odlaug and Kim2011; Ioannidis, Hook, Wickham, Grant, & Chamberlain, Reference Ioannidis, Hook, Wickham, Grant and Chamberlain2019; Kertzman, Lidogoster, Aizer, Kotler, & Dannon, Reference Kertzman, Lidogoster, Aizer, Kotler and Dannon2011; Kräplin et al., Reference Kräplin, Dshemuchadse, Behrendt, Scherbaum, Goschke and Bühringer2014). These changes may not be as apparent in other conditions. A better understanding of how cognition is affected in mental health pathologies may assist in the assessment of possible risk, as impaired cognition may lead to adverse outcomes for themselves or those around them.

Risk taking and impaired decision-making can be objectively quantified using the Cambridge Gambling Task (CGT) (Brand, Labudda, & Markowitsch, Reference Brand, Labudda and Markowitsch2006; Rogers et al., Reference Rogers, Owen, Middleton, Williams, Pickard, Sahakian and Robbins1999b; Yazdi et al., Reference Yazdi, Rumetshofer, Gnauer, Csillag, Rosenleitner and Kleiser2019), which is part of the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) (Sahakian et al., Reference Sahakian, Morris, Evenden, Heald, Levy, Philpot and Robbins1988). The CGT has proven sensitive to impaired decision-making in groups of people with gambling disorder, v. groups of controls, with gamblers having a higher tendency to seek risk and prefer more immediate rewards (Kräplin et al., Reference Kräplin, Dshemuchadse, Behrendt, Scherbaum, Goschke and Bühringer2014). These changes are not specific only to gambling and can be potentially seen in other conditions. For example, use of the CGT in depressed patients have shown altered cognition with impaired reward processing of individuals (Halahakoon et al., Reference Halahakoon, Kieslich, O'Driscoll, Nair, Lewis and Roiser2020; Yazdi et al., Reference Yazdi, Rumetshofer, Gnauer, Csillag, Rosenleitner and Kleiser2019). Other studies have also shown decision-making changes in anxiety and ADHD (Murphy et al., Reference Murphy, Rubinsztein, Michael, Rogers, Robbins, Paykel and Sahakian2001; Sørensen et al., Reference Sørensen, Sonuga-Barke, Eichele, van Wageningen, Wollschlaeger and Plessen2017; Tolomeo, Matthews, Steele, & Baldacchino, Reference Tolomeo, Matthews, Steele and Baldacchino2018).

Furthermore, studies have employed the CGT to understand the neural substrates involved in decision-making (Clark, Cools, & Robbins, Reference Clark, Cools and Robbins2004). For example, in patients with cortical lesions, damage to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex affected betting (irrespective of the odds of winning) on the CGT, whereas insula cortex lesions were associated with problems adjusting bets as a function of the odds of winning (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Bechara, Damasio, Aitken, Sahakian and Robbins2008; Clark, Manes, Antoun, Sahakian, & Robbins, Reference Clark, Manes, Antoun, Sahakian and Robbins2003). Both types of brain lesion were associated with significant abnormalities in probability judgment. Continued research on the relationship between mental health disorders and cognition may lead to a greater understanding of the neurocircuitry that is implicated in the manifestation of these conditions.

Importantly, the relative profiles of decision-making abnormalities across these and other mental health disorders have not been well-characterized. Therefore, our study used the CGT in a large cohort with a rich profile of mental health psychopathology, allowing comparison to be made in how decision-making varies across diseases. The aim of the study was to explore the profile of decision-making performance (as indexed by the CGT) across a range of mental health disorders compared to people without the given disorder of interest (hereafter referred to as controls). We hypothesized that the decision-making impairments will be present across different mental health conditions compared to controls and this would be most significant in individuals affected by gambling disorder.

Methods

Sample

572 young adults (aged 18–29 years) were enrolled from general community settings using community advertisements in the Minneapolis and Chicago metropolitan areas were enrolled. Participants must have gambled at least five times in the past year (i.e. a proxy for some level of impulsive behavior) and be able to be interviewed in person to be included. Exclusion criteria for this study were hearing or vision problems that made performing cognitive tasks difficult, and an inability to understand and consent to the study – as determined by the study team following interview of the participants. Comorbidities were permitted in the clinical groups, for example if a given individual had both depression and anxiety, they were included in the clinical data for both the ‘depression’ computation and the ‘anxiety’ computation. Participants were recruited via media advertisements. Each participant received a $50 gift card to an online store as compensation. The Institutional Review Board of the University of Chicago approved the study and the consent statement. After receiving a complete description of the study, participants provided written informed consent. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Assessments

Demographic variables, including age, biological sex at birth, self-reported gender, self-reported racial-ethnic identity, and highest level of education completed, were recorded for all participants. Subjects received an in-person psychiatric evaluation from a member of the research team trained in the administration of all the instruments employed, these were:

The CGT from the CANTAB (Sandberg, Reference Sandberg, Kreutzer, DeLuca and Caplan2011) was used to assess cognition of participants. The CGT was chosen as it is computerized, can fractionate distinct aspects of decision-making, and has received extensive investigations into its neurobiological underpinnings. In this assessment task ten blue and/or red boxes are presented in varying ratios (e.g. 7:3. 6:4) on a touch-sensitive computer screen. Participants then decide whether the yellow token is hidden in a red or blue box, staking a proportion of their cumulative points on their choice being correct. The proportions shown are 5, 25, 50, 75 or 95% randomly displayed in either descending or ascending order. If their choice was correct the stake is added to their total accrued points, or subtracted (i.e. lost) if incorrect. Participants are told that they should try and gain as many points as possible. Thus, the task explores different aspects of decision-making under controlled laboratory conditions (Fig. 1).

Figure 1. Image of the CGT © Copyright 2018 Cambridge Cognition Limited. All rights reserved.

Three decision-making parameters were then recorded: the overall proportion of the bet, risk adjustment and the quality of decision-making. The overall proportion of the bet was the average proportion of points that was staked across the whole of the task. Risk adjustment was the degree to which a subject changed their risk taking in response to the ratios of red to blue boxes on each trial. Consistent riskier betting would lead to a lower risk adjustment score. A high risk adjustment score indicated a tendency to bet more in the rounds with better odds (Deakin, Aitken, Robbins, & Sahakian, Reference Deakin, Aitken, Robbins and Sahakian2004). Lastly, the quality of decision-making was the mean proportion of rounds where the most probable color (i.e. logical color choice) was chosen.

Data analysis

Only psychiatric disorders endorsed by at least 1% (5 or more) of participants were included in the data analysis. To assess the degree of impairment as quantified using the CGT, results were calculated into relative z-scores (effect size) v. controls. For each disorder of interest, controls comprised everyone in the total sample who did not have that disorder. For example, the control group for depression consisted of all participants that did not have depression as assessed by our instruments. Individuals that had comorbidities were included in the clinical data of all assessed conditions. For instance, an individual with anxiety and depression was included in the Z-score computation for both anxiety and depression. The effect sizes were interpreted per convention using Cohen's D criteria, with 0.2 being regarded as small/mild, 0.5 being moderate, and lastly 0.8 being large. Scores represented the proportion of participants either below or above the average of the control group (Fig. 1).

Results

The sample consisted of 572 participants, 65.7% were female and 34.3% male. The average age was 22.3 years old, with the majority (73.6%) having college education or higher. 72.1% were white Caucasian, 14.6% were African-American, and 6.3% were Asian. The remaining 7% consisted of Latino/Hispanic, Middle Eastern, Native American and mixed race. Table 1 shows the number of participants with each disorder.

Table 1. Number of participants with each mental health disorder

Figures 2–4 illustrate the results of the three different parameters as assessed via the CGT. Almost all mental health disorders were associated with at least a mild impairment in all parameters, as compared to respective control groups of individuals without the given condition.

Figure 2. Comparison of the effect size in the overall proportion of bet staked in the CGT in different mental health disorders compared against controls. Horizontal lines indicate magnitude of impairment, mild (−0.2), moderate (−0.5) and large (−0.8 and less). A more negative z-score indicates larger bets v. controls.

Figure 3. Comparison of the effect size in the quality of decision-making assessed via the CGT in different mental health disorders compared against controls. Horizontal lines indicate the magnitude of impairment, mild (0.2), moderate (0.5) and large (0.8 and greater). A more positive z-score indicates greater relative impairment v. controls.

Figure 4. Comparison of the effect size in the adjustment of risk assessed via the CGT in different mental health disorders compared against controls. Horizontal lines indicate the magnitude of impairment, mild (0.2), moderate (0.5) and large (0.8 and greater). A more positive z-score indicates greater relative impairment v. controls.

Interestingly gambling disorder was associated with moderate impairment consistently in all three parameters, whereas binge eating disorder had large impairment throughout. Participants with intermittent explosive disorder also had a large tendency to bet larger proportions of their points and exhibited large impairment in risk adjustment.

Participants with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) had greater impairment in the quality of decision-making, whilst participants with major depressive disorder and obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD) exhibited moderately impaired quality of decision-making (Fig. 3).

Participants' ability to manage risk and adapt was moderately impaired in half of the mental health disorders assessed. In contrast only ADHD and compulsive sexual behavior disorder failed to show at least a mild impairment on this domain (Fig. 4).

Discussion

This study is one of the first to report on the relative profiles of decision-making impairments across a broad range of mental disorders, using a laboratory-based paradigm. The key findings were that almost all disorders were linked to at least mild (i.e. small effect size) impairment in one or more decision-making measures. Some disorders were more profoundly affected than others, as described in detail below.

These results importantly highlight that decision-making was impaired – in relative terms – across different conditions, and contrary to our expectation while gambling disorder was linked to deficits, some other disorders had more profound impairments. Notably, binge eating disorder had larger cognitive impairment in comparison to all other psychiatric disorders, whereas in contrast gambling disorder had a moderate impairment throughout. The fact that decision-making was particularly impaired in binge-eating is interesting in light of a recent systematic review and meta-analysis of the available literature (Colton, Wilson, Chong, & Verdejo-Garcia, Reference Colton, Wilson, Chong and Verdejo-Garcia2023) – which found a range of impairments, across tasks and measures, relating to aspects of decision-making in binge-eating disorder. The authors suggested that the deficits could arise from a combination of processes, such as: difficulty forming stable preferences in situations involving ambiguous or complex outcomes; attentional response disinhibition; inflexibility; and difficulties using moment-by-moment feedback to optimize decisions (Colton et al., Reference Colton, Wilson, Chong and Verdejo-Garcia2023). Thus, these processes (or a combination of them) may have contributed to the particularly pronounced deficits seen in the current study, though future work would be needed to confirm this.

Those with intermittent explosive disorder were also the only participants that had a large effect size tendency to make larger bets, with poorer risk adjustment. As one would expect, those with gambling disorder also bet more which is consistent with the known psychological profile of the disorder (Ioannidis et al., Reference Ioannidis, Hook, Wickham, Grant and Chamberlain2019).

Participants with PTSD had large impairment in their quality of decision-making which was an interesting result. This may be due to a reduced expectation of a positive outcome as seen in a similar study assessing reward processing in PTSD (Hopper et al., Reference Hopper, Pitman, Su, Heyman, Lasko, Macklin and Elman2008; May & Wisco, Reference May and Wisco2020). Therefore, this may have led to participants believing the majority color was not as favorable due to a perception that regardless of their actions the rewards would be minimal (i.e. an ‘assumption of defeat’).

Most importantly, half of the mental health disorders assessed in this study showed a moderate to large impairment in risk adjustment, manifesting as a relative inability to flexibly adjust gambling behavior as a function of risk. However, it is unclear whether this is due to a tendency to engage in risky behavior. Two different strains of thought may be implicated in this result. Either the individual had a psychiatric disorder that caused them to be more risk averse, less pursuant in reward, and thus more rigid in their thinking e.g. PTSD, depression (Halahakoon et al., Reference Halahakoon, Kieslich, O'Driscoll, Nair, Lewis and Roiser2020; May & Wisco, Reference May and Wisco2020). Or alternatively the individual had a condition that is more risk prone such as gambling disorder (Kräplin et al., Reference Kräplin, Dshemuchadse, Behrendt, Scherbaum, Goschke and Bühringer2014). Therefore, they would be less likely to alter their risky behavior in pursuit of higher gains because their overall profile of approach was to take higher risks.

These results suggest that mental health conditions not conventionally studied/conceptualized in terms of cognition may nonetheless have complex psychological profiles that include a degree of relative decision-making impairment. A better understanding of such impairment may allow for clinical treatment to better assess the risk that could be involved with these disorders. A holistic appreciation of the psychological profile may allow clinicians to better understand behavior and the subsequent choices patients may make and work towards better outcomes. For example, cognitive training is being explored as a candidate intervention for gambling disorder (Luquiens, Miranda, Benyamina, Carré, & Aubin, Reference Luquiens, Miranda, Benyamina, Carré and Aubin2019). Because some degree of deficit (in relative terms) was found across disorders, impaired decision-making may not be specific to any disorder, but could be viewed as a trans-diagnostic treatment target, and possibly an indication of vulnerability. Relatedly, the overlapping deficits may reflect overlapping comorbidities across disorders.

Limitations

Despite examining decision-making profiles across disorders, it is important to consider a number of limitations. Firstly, it is important to note that the CGT examines aspects of decision-making and is not designed to fully capture ‘impulsivity’ – especially in terms of disinhibition, which is better measured using tasks such as stop-signal paradigms. As a naturalistic study comparing people with v. without each mental health condition of interest, cognitive findings could have been contributed to by other variables (such as comorbidities, or demographic differences between groups). Some of the disorders studied had relatively small sample sizes, and it will be important to attempt to replicate findings using larger samples in future. The sample's average age was 22.4 years old, being majority female, which may reduce the generalizability of the data (Rolison, Hanoch, Wood, & Liu, Reference Rolison, Hanoch, Wood and Liu2014). Some research has shown that age and gender can influence CGT performance to some degree (Deakin et al., Reference Deakin, Aitken, Robbins and Sahakian2004; Rolison et al., Reference Rolison, Hanoch, Wood and Liu2014). Future work should address the influence of age and gender, and other potential confounding variables (e.g. comorbidities), but this would require a larger sample than was available here. The participants in this sample were non-treatment seeking which could reduce the applicability of the findings to people presenting in clinical settings. In addition, we were unable to control for co-morbidities or the use of medications or drugs that affect cognition due to the small sample size and the nature of the study. As comorbidities are common in mental health conditions it is likely that some participants had more than one disorder. Another limitation is that we focused on effect sizes, due to the variable and in some cases relatively small sample sizes in particular groups, rather than conducting formal statistical p values tests. Some of the disorders studied had relatively small sample sizes, and it will be important to attempt to replicate findings using larger samples in future. Lastly, as a cross sectional study, these results cannot determine causality between mental health disorders and cognition. Addressing this issue would require a very large-scale longitudinal study with inclusion of detailed screening for such a broad range of mental disorders, including gambling disorder (which is often overlooked).

Conclusion

In conclusion, our study presented the relative decision-making profiles of people with a range of mental health disorders, using a computerized laboratory-based task. The findings highlight the need to consider decision-making not only in conditions conventionally likely to be linked to impairments, such as gambling disorder, but also other conditions such as PTSD and binge-eating disorder. If these findings generalize to people with these conditions, the relative cognitive deficits may constitute trans-diagnostic targets for treatment interventions, or indicate the need to adapt existing treatments in order to help address these additional difficulties.

Author contributions

RE led on analysis of the data and writing of the report. JG designed the study and collected the data. SRC, JG, and KI provided input into the analytical approach, writing of the manuscript and interpretation of the findings. All authors take joint responsibility for integrity of the data.

Funding statement

This research was funded by the National Center for Responsible Gaming (NCRG) Center of Excellence grant (JEG).

Competing interests

SRC and KI receive a stipend from Elsevier for journal editorial work. JG has received research grants from Janssen and Biohaven Pharmaceuticals. He receives yearly compensation from Springer Publishing for acting as Editor-in-Chief of the Journal of Gambling Studies and has received royalties from Oxford University Press, American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc., Norton Press, and McGraw Hill. RE has no disclosures.

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5 (Vol. 5). American psychiatric association Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Brand, M., Labudda, K., & Markowitsch, H. J. (2006). Neuropsychological correlates of decision-making in ambiguous and risky situations. Neural Networks, 19(8), 12661276. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2006.03.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chamberlain, S. R., & Grant, J. E. (2018). Minnesota impulse disorders interview (MIDI): Validation of a structured diagnostic clinical interview for impulse control disorders in an enriched community sample. Psychiatry Research, 265, 279283. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.05.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, L., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Aitken, M. R., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2008). Differential effects of insular and ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions on risky decision-making. Brain, 131(Pt 5), 13111322. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn066CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, L., Cools, R., & Robbins, T. W. (2004). The neuropsychology of ventral prefrontal cortex: Decision-making and reversal learning. Brain and Cognition, 55(1), 4153. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00284-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, L., Manes, F., Antoun, N., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2003). The contributions of lesion laterality and lesion volume to decision-making impairment following frontal lobe damage. Neuropsychologia, 41(11), 14741483. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00081-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colton, E., Wilson, K. E., Chong, T. T., & Verdejo-Garcia, A. (2023). Dysfunctional decision-making in binge-eating disorder: A meta-analysis and systematic review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 152, 105250. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105250CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deakin, J., Aitken, M., Robbins, T., & Sahakian, B. J. (2004). Risk taking during decision-making in normal volunteers changes with age. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10(4), 590598. doi: 10.1017/s1355617704104104CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gould, T. J. (2010). Addiction and cognition. Addiction Science & Clinical Practice, 5(2), 414.Google ScholarPubMed
Grace, A. A. (2016). Dysregulation of the dopamine system in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia and depression. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(8), 524532. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.57CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grant, J. E. (2008). Impulse control disorders: A clinician's guide to understanding and treating behavioral addictions. New York, NY, USA: W W Norton & Co.Google Scholar
Grant, J. E., Chamberlain, S. R., Schreiber, L. R., Odlaug, B. L., & Kim, S. W. (2011). Selective decision-making deficits in at-risk gamblers. Psychiatry Research, 189(1), 115120. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.05.034CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grant, J. E., Steinberg, M. A., Kim, S. W., Rounsaville, B. J., & Potenza, M. N. (2004). Preliminary validity and reliability testing of a structured clinical interview for pathological gambling. Psychiatry Research, 128(1), 7988. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.05.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halahakoon, D. C., Kieslich, K., O'Driscoll, C., Nair, A., Lewis, G., & Roiser, J. P. (2020). Reward-processing behavior in depressed participants relative to healthy volunteers: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry, 77(12), 12861295. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.2139CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopper, J. W., Pitman, R. K., Su, Z., Heyman, G. M., Lasko, N. B., Macklin, M. L., … Elman, I. (2008). Probing reward function in posttraumatic stress disorder: Expectancy and satisfaction with monetary gains and losses. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 42(10), 802807. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2007.10.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ioannidis, K., Hook, R., Wickham, K., Grant, J. E., & Chamberlain, S. R. (2019). Impulsivity in gambling disorder and problem gambling: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychopharmacology, 44(8), 13541361. doi: 10.1038/s41386-019-0393-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kertzman, S., Lidogoster, H., Aizer, A., Kotler, M., & Dannon, P. N. (2011). Risk-taking decisions in pathological gamblers is not a result of their impaired inhibition ability. Psychiatry Research, 188(1), 7177. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.02.021CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Adler, L., Ames, M., Demler, O., Faraone, S., Hiripi, E. V. A., … Walters, E. E. (2005). The World Health Organization adult ADHD self-report scale (ASRS): A short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychological Medicine, 35(2), 245256. doi: 10.1017/S0033291704002892CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kräplin, A., Dshemuchadse, M., Behrendt, S., Scherbaum, S., Goschke, T., & Bühringer, G. (2014). Dysfunctional decision-making in pathological gambling: Pattern specificity and the role of impulsivity. Psychiatry Research, 215(3), 675682. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.12.041CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luquiens, A., Miranda, R., Benyamina, A., Carré, A., & Aubin, H.-J. (2019). Cognitive training: A new avenue in gambling disorder management? Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 106, 227233. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.10.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
May, C. L., & Wisco, B. E. (2020). Reward processing and decision-making in posttraumatic stress disorder. Behavior Therapy, 51(5), 814828. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2019.11.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, F. C., Rubinsztein, J. S., Michael, A., Rogers, R. D., Robbins, T. W., Paykel, E. S., & Sahakian, B. J. (2001). Decision-making cognition in mania and depression. Psychological Medicine, 31(4), 679693. doi: 10.1017/S0033291701003804CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pironti, V. A., Lai, M. C., Müller, U., Dodds, C. M., Suckling, J., Bullmore, E. T., & Sahakian, B. J. (2014). Neuroanatomical abnormalities and cognitive impairments are shared by adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and their unaffected first-degree relatives. Biological Psychiatry, 76(8), 639647. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.09.025CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, R. D., Everitt, B. J., Baldacchino, A., Blackshaw, A. J., Swainson, R., Wynne, K., … Robbins, T. W. (1999a). Dissociable deficits in the decision-making cognition of chronic amphetamine abusers, opiate abusers, patients with focal damage to prefrontal cortex, and tryptophan-depleted normal volunteers: Evidence for monoaminergic mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology, 20(4), 322339. doi: 10.1016/s0893-133x(98)00091-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, R. D., Owen, A. M., Middleton, H. C., Williams, E. J., Pickard, J. D., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (1999b). Choosing between small, likely rewards and large, unlikely rewards activates inferior and orbital prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 90299038.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rolison, J. J., Hanoch, Y., Wood, S., & Liu, P.-J. (2014). Risk-taking differences across the adult life span: A question of age and domain. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 69(6), 870880. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbt081CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sahakian, B. J., Morris, R. G., Evenden, J. L., Heald, A., Levy, R., Philpot, M., & Robbins, T. W. (1988). A comparative study of visuospatial memory and learning in Alzheimer-type dementia and Parkinson's disease. Brain, 111(Pt 3), 695718. doi: 10.1093/brain/111.3.695CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandberg, M. A. (2011). Cambridge neuropsychological testing automated battery. In Kreutzer, J. S., DeLuca, J., & Caplan, B. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of clinical neuropsychology (pp. 480482). New York, NY: Springer New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., … Dunbar, G. C. (1998). The Mini-international neuropsychiatric interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59(Suppl 20), 22-33, 3457.Google ScholarPubMed
Sørensen, L., Sonuga-Barke, E., Eichele, H., van Wageningen, H., Wollschlaeger, D., & Plessen, K. J. (2017). Suboptimal decision making by children with ADHD in the face of risk: Poor risk adjustment and delay aversion rather than general proneness to taking risks. Neuropsychology, 31(2), 119128. doi: 10.1037/neu0000297CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tolomeo, S., Matthews, K., Steele, D., & Baldacchino, A. (2018). Compulsivity in opioid dependence. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 81, 333339. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.09.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yazdi, K., Rumetshofer, T., Gnauer, M., Csillag, D., Rosenleitner, J., & Kleiser, R. (2019). Neurobiological processes during the Cambridge gambling task. Behavioural Brain Research, 356, 295304. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.08.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Image of the CGT © Copyright 2018 Cambridge Cognition Limited. All rights reserved.

Figure 1

Table 1. Number of participants with each mental health disorder

Figure 2

Figure 2. Comparison of the effect size in the overall proportion of bet staked in the CGT in different mental health disorders compared against controls. Horizontal lines indicate magnitude of impairment, mild (−0.2), moderate (−0.5) and large (−0.8 and less). A more negative z-score indicates larger bets v. controls.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Comparison of the effect size in the quality of decision-making assessed via the CGT in different mental health disorders compared against controls. Horizontal lines indicate the magnitude of impairment, mild (0.2), moderate (0.5) and large (0.8 and greater). A more positive z-score indicates greater relative impairment v. controls.

Figure 4

Figure 4. Comparison of the effect size in the adjustment of risk assessed via the CGT in different mental health disorders compared against controls. Horizontal lines indicate the magnitude of impairment, mild (0.2), moderate (0.5) and large (0.8 and greater). A more positive z-score indicates greater relative impairment v. controls.