Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T10:43:21.722Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Modulation of anterior cingulate cortex reward and penalty signalling in medication-naive young-adult subjects with depressive symptoms following acute dose lurasidone

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 January 2019

Selina A. Wolke*
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK Mood Brain and Development Unit, Emotion and Development Branch, National Institute of Mental Health, National Institutes of Health, MD, USA
Mitul A. Mehta
Affiliation:
Department of Neuroimaging, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
Owen O'Daly
Affiliation:
Department of Neuroimaging, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
Fernando Zelaya
Affiliation:
Department of Neuroimaging, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
Nada Zahreddine
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Saint-Joseph University, Beirut, Lebanon
Hanna Keren
Affiliation:
Mood Brain and Development Unit, Emotion and Development Branch, National Institute of Mental Health, National Institutes of Health, MD, USA
Georgia O'Callaghan
Affiliation:
Mood Brain and Development Unit, Emotion and Development Branch, National Institute of Mental Health, National Institutes of Health, MD, USA
Allan H. Young
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
Ellen Leibenluft
Affiliation:
Section on Mood Dysregulation and Neuroscience, Emotion and Development Branch, National Institute of Mental Health, National Institutes of Health, MD, USA
Daniel S. Pine
Affiliation:
Section on Development and Affective Neuroscience, Emotion and Development Branch, National Institute of Mental Health, MD, USA
Argyris Stringaris
Affiliation:
Mood Brain and Development Unit, Emotion and Development Branch, National Institute of Mental Health, National Institutes of Health, MD, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Selina A. Wolke, E-mail: selina.wolke@kcl.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Aberrations in reward and penalty processing are implicated in depression and putatively reflect altered dopamine signalling. This study exploits the advantages of a placebo-controlled design to examine how a novel D2 antagonist with adjunctive antidepressant properties modifies activity in the brain's reward network in depression.

Methods

We recruited 43 medication-naïve subjects across the range of depression severity (Beck's Depression Inventory-II score range: 0–43), including healthy volunteers, as well as people meeting full-criteria for major depressive disorder. In a double-blind placebo-controlled cross-over design, all subjects received either placebo or lurasidone (20 mg) across two visits separated by 1 week. Functional magnetic resonance imaging with the Monetary Incentive Delay (MID) task assessed reward functions via neural responses during anticipation and receipt of gains and losses. Arterial spin labelling measured cerebral blood flow (CBF) at rest.

Results

Lurasidone altered fronto-striatal activity during anticipation and outcome phases of the MID task. A significant three-way Medication-by-Depression severity-by-Outcome interaction emerged in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) after correction for multiple comparisons. Follow-up analyses revealed significantly higher ACC activation to losses in high- v. low depression participants in the placebo condition, with a normalisation by lurasidone. This effect could not be accounted for by shifts in resting CBF.

Conclusions

Lurasidone acutely normalises reward processing signals in individuals with depressive symptoms. Lurasidone's antidepressant effects may arise from reducing responses to penalty outcomes in individuals with depressive symptoms.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Introduction

Multiple studies implicate reward and dopaminergic system dysfunction in the pathogenesis of major depressive disorder (MDD). Yet, only few studies use experimentally controlled designs to probe the role of these systems in MDD. Here, we examine the acute effects of lurasidone, a novel D2 antagonist with adjunctive antidepressant properties, on neural responding to reward in depression using two functional imaging modalities.

Depressed patients display alterations across several key phases of reward processing. Blunting of neural responses when anticipating or obtaining rewards (Knutson et al., Reference Knutson, Bhanji, Cooney, Atlas and Gotlib2008; Pizzagalli et al., Reference Pizzagalli, Holmes, Dillon, Goetz, Birk, Bogdan, Dougherty, Iosifescu, Rauch and Fava2009; Keren et al., Reference Keren, O'Callaghan, Vidal-Ribas, Buzzell, Brotman, Pan, Meffert, Kaisera, Wolke, Pine and Stringaris2018) is associated with anhedonia, while increased reactivity to losses may underlie the behavioural avoidance that is characteristic of depression (Stringaris et al., Reference Stringaris, Vidal-Ribas Belil, Artiges, Lemaitre, Gollier-Briant, Wolke, Vulser, Miranda, Penttila, Struve, Fadai, Kappel, Grimmer, Goodman, Poustka, Conrod, Cattrell, Banaschewski, Bokde, Bromberg, Buchel, Flor, Frouin, Gallinat, Garavan, Gowland, Heinz, Ittermann, Nees, Papadopoulos, Paus, Smolka, Walter, Whelan, Martinot, Schumann and Paillere-Martinot2015; Luking et al., Reference Luking, Pagliaccio, Luby and Barch2016; Engelmann et al., Reference Engelmann, Berns and Dunlop2017; Hevey et al., Reference Hevey, Thomas, Laureano-Schelten, Looney and Booth2017). Recently, a direct link has been found between reduced mid-brain dopamine transporter density and neural activity during reward processing within the mesolimbic pathway in healthy and depressed human participants (Dubol et al., Reference Dubol, Trichard, Leroy, Sandu, Rahim, Granger, Tzavara, Karila, Martinot and Artiges2018).

These findings make reward processing an attractive treatment target. Dopaminergic compounds provide a promising way to manipulate fronto-striatal reward pathways (Pessiglione et al., Reference Pessiglione, Seymour, Flandin, Dolan and Frith2006; Jocham et al., Reference Jocham, Klein and Ullsperger2011, Reference Jocham, Klein and Ullsperger2014; Chowdhury et al., Reference Chowdhury, Guitart-Masip, Lambert, Dayan, Huys, Düzel and Dolan2013; Dean et al., Reference Dean, Horndasch, Giannopoulos and McCabe2016; Harmer et al., Reference Harmer, Duman and Cowen2017). Surprisingly, however, very few studies have used dopaminergic drugs to probe the association between neural reward signalling and depression. Recently, Admon et al. (Reference Admon, Kaiser, Dillon, Beltzer, Goer, Olson, Vitaliano and Pizzagalli2017) showed that a single-dose of the dopamine receptor antagonist amisulpride normalised reward processing by increasing reward-related striatal activation and corticostriatal connectivity in depressed individuals. This effect is thought to result from transient increases in dopamine signalling at low amisulpride doses (Schoemaker et al., Reference Schoemaker, Claustre, Fage, Rouquier, Chergui, Curet, Oblin, Gonon, Carter, Benavides and Scatton1997; Admon et al., Reference Admon, Kaiser, Dillon, Beltzer, Goer, Olson, Vitaliano and Pizzagalli2017). Strengthening of striatal functioning through dopamine antagonists has been shown before in healthy volunteers (Mehta et al., Reference Mehta, McGowan, Lawrence, Aitken, Montgomery and Grasby2003; Handley et al., Reference Handley, Zelaya, Reinders, Marques, Mehta, O'Gorman, Alsop, Taylor, Johnston, Williams, McGuire, Pariante, Kapur and Dazzan2013) and is presumed to occur through presynaptic D2/D3 autoreceptor blockade (Fernandez-Seara et al., Reference Fernandez-Seara, Aznarez-Sanado, Mengual, Irigoyen, Heukamp and Pastor2011, Goozee et al., Reference Goozee, Handley, Kempton and Dazzan2014).

It may seem counterintuitive that some antipsychotics are antidepressant in augmentation treatment for bipolar and MDD, given that D2 antagonism (a central feature of all antipsychotics) is known to suppress reward-related striatal activation, for example, with haloperidol (Pessiglione et al., Reference Pessiglione, Seymour, Flandin, Dolan and Frith2006; Pleger et al., Reference Pleger, Ruff, Blankenburg, Kloppel, Driver and Dolan2009; Oei et al., Reference Oei, Rombouts, Soeter, van Gerven and Both2012). However, olanzapine, quetiapine and lurasidone, which are efficacious adjunctive antidepressants [olanzapine (Tohen et al., Reference Tohen, Vieta, Calabrese, Ketter, Sachs, Bowden, Mitchell, Centorrino, Risser, Baker, Evans, Beymer, Dube, Tollefson and Breier2003, Reference Tohen, Kanba, McIntyre, Fujikoshi and Katagiri2014), quetiapine (Suppes et al., Reference Suppes, Datto, Minkwitz, Nordenhem, Walker and Darko2014; Suttajit et al., Reference Suttajit, Srisurapanont, Maneeton and Maneeton2014), lurasidone (Loebel et al., Reference Loebel, Cucchiaro, Silva, Kroger, Hsu, Sarma and Sachs2014a, Reference Loebel, Cucchiaro, Silva, Kroger, Sarma, Xu and Calabrese2014b; Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Suppes, Tsai, Mao, Pikalov and Loebel2015; Suppes et al., Reference Suppes, Kroger, Pikalov and Loebel2016a, Reference Suppes, Silva, Cucchiaro, Mao, Targum, Streicher, Pikalov and Loebel2016b)] differ from haloperidol in their broader profile, including greater serotonergic action. Indeed, blockade of serotonergic 5-HT receptors (5-HT1A, 5-HT2A, 5-HT7) stimulates striatal dopamine release and in addition to this, serotonergic neurons directly impact upon reward (and predominantly aversive) processing (Boureau and Dayan, Reference Boureau and Dayan2011; Huang et al., Reference Huang, Horiguchi, Felix and Meltzer2012; Inaba et al., Reference Inaba, Mizuhiki, Setogawa, Toda, Richmond and Shidara2013; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Zhou, Li, Hu, Lu, Ma, Feng, Zhang, Wang, Zeng, Bao, Kim, Chen, El Mestikawy and Luo2014; Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Amoroso and Uchida2015; Hayashi et al., Reference Hayashi, Nakao and Nakamura2015; Li et al., Reference Li, Zhong, Wang, Feng, Liu, Zhou, Jia, Hu, Zeng, Guo, Fu and Luo2016). However, there are few studies that have assessed modulation of loss anticipation and feedback with antidepressant drugs. The evidence thus far points to a pattern of blunting of aversive events with acute administration of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) (McCabe et al., Reference McCabe, Mishor, Cowen and Harmer2010; Macoveanu et al., Reference Macoveanu, Rowe, Hornboll, Elliott, Paulson, Knudsen and Siebner2013, Reference Macoveanu, Fisher, Haahr, Frokjaer, Knudsen and Siebner2014; Macoveanu, Reference Macoveanu2014), but crucially also with D2 antagonists that have anti-depressant properties [amisulpride (Admon et al., Reference Admon, Kaiser, Dillon, Beltzer, Goer, Olson, Vitaliano and Pizzagalli2017) and aripiprazole (Bolstad et al., Reference Bolstad, Andreassen, Groote, Server, Sjaastad, Kapur and Jensen2015)]. These findings raise the intriguing possibility that dopamine antagonists with adjunctive antidepressant properties may exert their effects via reward and/or penalty signal normalisation.

In this paper, we test whether an acute dose of 20 mg lurasidone, a D2 receptor antagonist (Loebel and Citrome, Reference Loebel and Citrome2015) with demonstrated antidepressant properties in monotherapy and in combination treatment (Loebel et al., Reference Loebel, Cucchiaro, Silva, Kroger, Hsu, Sarma and Sachs2014a, Reference Loebel, Cucchiaro, Silva, Kroger, Sarma, Xu and Calabrese2014b; Suppes et al., Reference Suppes, Kroger, Pikalov and Loebel2016a; Goldberg et al., Reference Goldberg, Ng-Mak, Siu, Chuang, Rajagopalan and Loebel2017), influences reward and penalty signal in depression. Lurasidone was selected because it is the most recently licensed dopamine antagonist with antidepressant properties and there is no information with regards to its effects on brain reward and penalty signalling (Loebel et al., Reference Loebel, Cucchiaro, Silva, Kroger, Hsu, Sarma and Sachs2014a, Reference Loebel, Cucchiaro, Silva, Kroger, Sarma, Xu and Calabrese2014b; Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Suppes, Tsai, Mao, Pikalov and Loebel2015; Nierenberg et al., Reference Nierenberg, Tsai, Mao, Pikalov, Suppes and Loebel2015; Suppes et al., Reference Suppes, Kroger, Pikalov and Loebel2016a, Reference Suppes, Silva, Cucchiaro, Mao, Targum, Streicher, Pikalov and Loebel2016b; Goldberg et al., Reference Goldberg, Ng-Mak, Siu, Chuang, Rajagopalan and Loebel2017). We employ a randomised, placebo-controlled cross-over design with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and arterial spin labelling (ASL) imaging acquired on two separate occasions per individual. This design overcomes the limitations of correlational studies through randomisation and experimental manipulation. Since symptoms of MDD fall on a continuous dimension (Angst et al., Reference Angst, Sellar and Merikangas2000; Ayuso-Mateos et al., Reference Ayuso-Mateos, Nuevo, Verdes, Naidoo and Chatterji2010), we recruited medication-naïve subjects across the range of depression severity, including healthy volunteers, as well as people meeting full-criteria for MDD. This research approach is in line with the Research Domain Criteria framework (Morris and Cuthbert, Reference Morris and Cuthbert2012) [e.g. as in Stringaris et al. (Reference Stringaris, Vidal-Ribas Belil, Artiges, Lemaitre, Gollier-Briant, Wolke, Vulser, Miranda, Penttila, Struve, Fadai, Kappel, Grimmer, Goodman, Poustka, Conrod, Cattrell, Banaschewski, Bokde, Bromberg, Buchel, Flor, Frouin, Gallinat, Garavan, Gowland, Heinz, Ittermann, Nees, Papadopoulos, Paus, Smolka, Walter, Whelan, Martinot, Schumann and Paillere-Martinot2015) where symptom levels are related to the brain measurements]. It also does justice to findings concerning the genetic underpinnings of common mental illness (Plomin et al., Reference Plomin, Haworth and Davis2009) as well as current approaches to understanding neural system perturbation in a dimensional way (Matthews and Hampshire, Reference Matthews and Hampshire2016).

Depression is characterised by hyporeactivity to reward (Knutson et al., Reference Knutson, Bhanji, Cooney, Atlas and Gotlib2008; Forbes et al., Reference Forbes, Hariri, Martin, Silk, Moyles, Fisher, Brown, Ryan, Birmaher, Axelson and Dahl2009; Pizzagalli et al., Reference Pizzagalli, Holmes, Dillon, Goetz, Birk, Bogdan, Dougherty, Iosifescu, Rauch and Fava2009; Gotlib et al., Reference Gotlib, Hamilton, Cooney, Singh, Henry and Joormann2010; Admon et al., Reference Admon, Nickerson, Dillon, Holmes, Bogdan, Kumar, Dougherty, Iosifescu, Mischoulon, Fava and Pizzagalli2015; Luking et al., Reference Luking, Pagliaccio, Luby and Barch2016; Keren et al., Reference Keren, O'Callaghan, Vidal-Ribas, Buzzell, Brotman, Pan, Meffert, Kaisera, Wolke, Pine and Stringaris2018) and hyperactivity to aversive stimuli (Gotlib et al., Reference Gotlib, Hamilton, Cooney, Singh, Henry and Joormann2010; Admon et al., Reference Admon, Nickerson, Dillon, Holmes, Bogdan, Kumar, Dougherty, Iosifescu, Mischoulon, Fava and Pizzagalli2015; Luking et al., Reference Luking, Pagliaccio, Luby and Barch2016; Engelmann et al., Reference Engelmann, Berns and Dunlop2017), and thus an antidepressant effect could be brought about by increasing reward, decreasing salience to negative events, or, both simultaneously. Given the relative paucity of literature on processing of losses (Keren et al., Reference Keren, O'Callaghan, Vidal-Ribas, Buzzell, Brotman, Pan, Meffert, Kaisera, Wolke, Pine and Stringaris2018), our study is designed to interrogate both anticipation and feedback of rewards and penalties. We hypothesise a normalisation of fronto-striatal reward and/or penalty function following acute-dose administration in depression. More specifically, we anticipate that subjects scoring high on depression will show a baseline difference in fronto-striatal activity which will be reverted by acute-dose lurasidone. We first explore the expectation that the dopamine antagonist lurasidone will show striatal blunting during the anticipation phase, in line with numerous findings with D2 antagonist drugs (Pessiglione et al., Reference Pessiglione, Seymour, Flandin, Dolan and Frith2006; Pleger et al., Reference Pleger, Ruff, Blankenburg, Kloppel, Driver and Dolan2009). Although, we note that a structurally similar drug, amisulpride has shown opposite effects (Admon et al., Reference Admon, Kaiser, Dillon, Beltzer, Goer, Olson, Vitaliano and Pizzagalli2017). An intriguing question is whether any blunting in reward processing that occurs with these drugs could have beneficial effects when dealing with loss. This is important given findings from serotonergic drugs that show on the one hand blunting of reward processing and on the other, amelioration of negative feedback (McCabe et al., Reference McCabe, Mishor, Cowen and Harmer2010; Macoveanu et al., Reference Macoveanu, Rowe, Hornboll, Elliott, Paulson, Knudsen and Siebner2013, Reference Macoveanu, Fisher, Haahr, Frokjaer, Knudsen and Siebner2014; Macoveanu, Reference Macoveanu2014), which could underlie its antidepressant effects. In addition, we seek to address a key concern in pharmacoimaging studies, namely that shifts in global or regional cerebral blood flow (CBF) could underlie changes observed in a blood oxygenated level dependent (BOLD) fMRI signal. We therefore also use ASL, an imaging modality that allows the quantification of CBF at rest, to disentangle global and regional CBF changes from a BOLD fMRI signal.

Materials and methods

Participants

Forty-three participants (28 female, 15 male) were recruited using the research volunteer recruitment webpage at King's College London, social media and posters at university counselling services across London.

We recruited young people across a range of depression and anhedonia scores in the community as symptoms of MDD are known to fall on a continuum (Angst et al., Reference Angst, Sellar and Merikangas2000; Ayuso-Mateos et al., Reference Ayuso-Mateos, Nuevo, Verdes, Naidoo and Chatterji2010), allowing us to assess the role of symptom level in reward processing on and off lurasidone (see text and online Figs S2–S4 in the Supplementary Methods). Inclusion criteria restricted recruitment to right-handed individuals 18–25 years of age with no contraindications to MRI, no serious medical conditions and no lifetime substance dependence. Please refer to the online Supplementary Methods for full details of inclusion and exclusion criteria. Table 1 provides demographic and clinical information for the entire sample (n = 43). Online Table S1 in the Supplementary Methods provides demographic and clinical characteristics of recruited participants according to depression severity cut-off scores from the Beck's Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II). Participants received £230 in compensation for attending the assessment appointment and both scanning visits, in addition to their winnings from the fMRI task. All participants provided written informed consent, as approved by the Ethics Subcommittee of Psychiatry, Nursing & Midwifery Research (RESC reference number: PNM/13/14-122).

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of participants in a study investigating the effect of lurasidone on reward and penalty processing

Design and procedure

Depression and anhedonia scores were assessed using the BDI-II (Beck et al., Reference Beck, Steer, Ball and Ranieri1996) and the Snaith–Hamilton Pleasure Scale (SHAPS) (Snaith et al., Reference Snaith, Hamilton, Morley, Humayan, Hargreaves and Trigwell1995). On the basis of BDI-II scores, participants who were eligible following this screening procedure were invited to the assessment appointment.

Figure 1 illustrates the procedure and timeline of the study. At the assessment appointment, participants first completed a pre-MRI safety screening. Participants then completed questionnaires to assess handedness (Edinburgh Handedness Inventory) and IQ (National Adult Reading Test) (Nelson and Willison, Reference Nelson and Willison1991). This was followed by the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview version 6.0.0 (M.I.N.I.) (Sheehan et al., Reference Sheehan, Lecrubier, Sheehan, Amorim, Janavs, Weiller, Hergueta, Baker and Dunbar1998) which assessed past and present mental health disorders. Participants’ height, weight, heart rate, blood pressure and electrocardiogram (ECG) were measured by the experimenter and blood samples (for Full Blood Count and Liver Function Tests) were taken by a study physician. Participants provided a urine sample for drug testing and for pregnancy testing in female participants. Participants were guided through the scanning procedure in a mock scanner and completed training for the Monetary Incentive Delay (MID) task.

Fig. 1. Procedure and timeline for a study investigating the effect of lurasidone on reward and penalty processing.

If participants fulfilled the inclusion criteria after the assessment appointment, they were invited to take part in two scan days. There was a 97% retention rate in the study and this is illustrated in online Fig. S1 in the Supplementary Methods. Participants were randomised into one of two drug administration orders: placebo-lurasidone (placebo at visit one and lurasidone at visit two), or lurasidone-placebo. Both scan days followed the same schedule. On arrival at the imaging centre, participants had their heart rate and blood pressure measured and filled in two brief questionnaires to measure sedation [Visual Analogue Scale (VAS) (Herbert et al., Reference Herbert, Johns and Doré1976) and state-anxiety (State Trait Anxiety Inventory; STAI) (Spielberger et al., Reference Spielberger, Gorsuch and Lushene1970)]. Next, the experimenter administered a capsule of either lurasidone (20 mg) or placebo. This dose was selected to minimise post-synaptic D2 blockade (la Fougere et al., Reference la Fougere, Meisenzahl, Schmitt, Stauss, Frodl, Tatsch, Hahn, Moller and Dresel2005), as in similar studies of related medications (Admon et al., Reference Admon, Kaiser, Dillon, Beltzer, Goer, Olson, Vitaliano and Pizzagalli2017). Given the pharmacokinetic profile of lurasidone, the pill was consumed, followed by a 350 calorie meal (Greenberg and Citrome, Reference Greenberg and Citrome2017). Peak plasma levels of lurasidone are reached at approximately 3 h after tablet ingestion and the plasma half-life is 18 h (Greenberg and Citrome, Reference Greenberg and Citrome2017). In order to align the study assessments with peak plasma levels the MRI scan took place 3 h after tablet consumption (Fig. 1). Prior to the MRI scan, 2 h 45 min after drug administration, the experimenter measured participants’ heart rate and blood pressure again, and participants completed the VAS and STAI questionnaires. The scan lasted approximately 1.5 h and included structural scans, ASL and a functional scan acquisition while completing the MID task. After the scan, and approximately 4.5 h after drug administration, the experimenter assessed the participants’ heart rate and blood pressure, the VAS/STAI questionnaires were completed and ECG was collected. Participants were paid in cash for their winnings from the MID task and were discharged.

fMRI task

The MID task used in the current study was an adaptation of the task from, for example Knutson and colleagues (Knutson et al., Reference Knutson, Adams, Fong and Hommer2001). The task involves anticipation and receipt of monetary rewards and penalties. The task elicits robust fronto-striatal responses in healthy individuals and has high scan-rescan reliability (Plichta et al., Reference Plichta, Schwarz, Grimm, Morgen, Mier, Haddad, Gerdes, Sauer, Tost, Esslinger, Colman, Wilson, Kirsch and Meyer-Lindenberg2012; Wu et al., Reference Wu, Samanez-Larkin, Katovich and Knutson2014). During the anticipation and receipt of monetary reward and penalties, several studies using this task have demonstrated altered fronto-striatal activation in depressed individuals compared with healthy controls (Knutson et al., Reference Knutson, Bhanji, Cooney, Atlas and Gotlib2008; Pizzagalli et al., Reference Pizzagalli, Holmes, Dillon, Goetz, Birk, Bogdan, Dougherty, Iosifescu, Rauch and Fava2009; Carl et al., Reference Carl, Walsh, Eisenlohr-Moul, Minkel, Crowther, Moore, Gibbs, Petty, Bizzell, Dichter and Smoski2016). This makes the MID task well-suited for the current study and further details are provided in the online Supplementary Methods.

MRI acquisition parameters

The MRI acquisition parameters are described in the online Supplementary Methods.

fMRI data analysis

ASL pre-processing

Spatial normalisation of the CBF maps was achieved using Automated Software for ASL Processing (ASAP; Mato Abad et al., Reference Mato Abad, Garcia-Polo, O'Daly, Antonio Hernandez-Tamames and Zelaya2016). This pipeline employs the Statistical Parametric Mapping suite (SPM, Functional Imaging Laboratory, University College London, London, UK, version 12 – https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). Full details are provided in the online Supplementary Methods.

fMRI pre-processing

fMRI data were preprocessed and quality assured using SPM12 in Matlab version (R2016b). This consisted of reorientation to the AC-PC line, slice timing correction, motion correction (Friston et al., Reference Friston, Williams, Howard, Frackowiak and Turner1996), multi-channel segmentation and co-registration to each participant's structural image. The normalise estimate & write function within SPM12 was used, with the Montreal Neurological Institute template (MNI152). Smoothing was completed using a Gaussian kernel of 4 mm full-width half-maximum.

ASL statistical analysis

To test for statistical significant changes in resting CBF we carried out a paired-sample t test, which compared the CBF maps collected after administration of lurasidone against those acquired after placebo. Quantitative measures of global CBF and striatal CBF were extracted for each participant after placebo and lurasidone. The striatal region-of-interest (ROI) was formed by combining anatomically defined binary masks of the caudate, putamen and nucleus accumbens (NAcc) (see online Fig. S7 in the Supplement) (O’Doherty et al., Reference O'Doherty, Dayan, Schultz, Deichmann, Friston and Dolan2004). A repeated-measures analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was performed for global and striatal CBF with the following factors: Medication (placebo, lurasidone) as the within-subject variable, Medication Order (placebo-lurasidone, lurasidone-placebo) as the between-subject factor and Depression Severity (total BDI-II score) as the covariate of interest. To test if changes in baseline CBF were related to the BOLD findings, the change in CBF between the two sessions was entered as covariates in all subsequent analyses. Specifically, the change in CBF values for a given region was used as covariates for the same region in the fMRI analyses.

fMRI first-level model

The BOLD signal was modelled with a canonical haemodynamic response function that was convolved with the onset times of task regressors to compute parameter estimates using the general linear model (GLM) at the single-subject level. The GLM included nine task-related regressors: passive condition, three cues (neutral, win, loss) and five outcomes [with (win outcome following win cue), missed win (no-change outcome following a win cue), loss (penalty outcome following a loss cue), avoided loss (no-change outcome following a loss cue) and neutral outcome (no-change outcome following a neutral/no-incentive cue)]. High-pass temporal filtering (128 s cut-off) was used to remove low-frequency artefacts. Estimated movement parameters were added to the design matrix. These included six rigid-body movement parameters, a regressor accounting for frame-wise displacement (i.e. the 3D movement from volume 1–2, 2–3 etc.), and additional binary regressors to indicate image volumes with spikes greater than 1 mm, and images either side of the spike (i.e. motion scrubbing and padding). Movement analyses are described in the online Supplementary Methods.

fMRI statistical analysis

Anticipation and outcome

Following previous findings that depression is associated with differential fronto-striatal abnormalities in response to anticipation v. receipt of monetary outcomes (Pizzagalli et al., Reference Pizzagalli, Holmes, Dillon, Goetz, Birk, Bogdan, Dougherty, Iosifescu, Rauch and Fava2009) statistical analyses were separately conducted for the cue and outcome phases of the task.

To test a priori hypotheses regarding fronto-striatal responses to the anticipation and outcome of reward and penalty, we conducted a ROI analysis. Mean activations were extracted from seven bilateral anatomical masks of the caudate, putamen, NAcc, orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), insula and amygdala for each participant for the following contrasts of interest: (i) anticipation neutral > baseline, (ii) anticipation win > baseline, (iii) anticipation loss > baseline, (iv) Reward Outcome: feedback win > missed win and (v) Penalty Outcome: feedback loss > avoided loss. This analytic approach has been used previously (Admon et al., Reference Admon, Kaiser, Dillon, Beltzer, Goer, Olson, Vitaliano and Pizzagalli2017) and mitigates possible spillover effects of cue type on the neural responses to outcomes. Masks were collapsed across hemispheres because hemispheric effects on task activation were non-significant and because of the high correlation between hemispheric ROIs. To avoid circular analysis (Kriegeskorte et al., Reference Kriegeskorte, Simmons, Bellgowan and Baker2009), whole regions from atlas toolboxes in SPM12 were used (see online Fig. S7 in the supplementary data). These ROIs were chosen in accordance with meta-analytical findings of the neural correlates of reward and penalty processing (Diekhof et al., Reference Diekhof, Kaps, Falkai and Gruber2012; Bartra et al., Reference Bartra, McGuire and Kable2013; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Chang, Guo, Zhang and Wang2013).

For the anticipation phase of the task, a repeated-measures ANCOVA was performed for each ROI with the following factors: Medication (placebo, lurasidone) and Anticipation Cue (neutral, win, loss) as within-subject variables, Medication Order as the between-subject factor, and Depression Severity (total BDI-II score) as the covariate of interest.

To test our hypothesis regarding normalisation of reward and/or penalty responses, we conducted a repeated measures ANCOVA for each ROI. This included the factors: Medication (placebo, lurasidone) and Outcome Type (reward, penalty) as within-subject variables, Medication Order as the between-subject factor, and Depression Severity (total BDI-II score) as the covariate of interest. We predicted that normalisation responses in depressed individuals on lurasidone would be captured by a Medication-by-Depression Severity-by-Outcome Type interaction. We expected to find no effect of Medication Order.

In order to examine further the drug effects on the neural signal, we examined how the difference in neural activity (Δ neural activity) between placebo and lurasidone in each ROI varied across depression scores. For this, Pearson correlation coefficients were estimated.

To complement our primary dimensional analyses (using a continuous measure of depression), we also examined our hypothesis regarding normalisation of responses using categorical groups in a repeated measures ANOVA model (Fig. 4). We used severity cut-off scores for the BDI-II (Beck et al., Reference Beck, Steer, Ball and Ranieri1996, Krefetz et al., Reference Krefetz, Steer, Gulab and Beck2002, Kumar et al., Reference Kumar, Steer, Teitelman and Villacis2002) to compare individuals with low depressive symptoms [total BDI-II score: 0–16 (normal-mild mood disturbance), n = 24] to individuals with high depressive symptoms [total BDI-II score: 17–43 (borderline-severe depression), n = 18] on placebo and lurasidone (Strober et al., Reference Strober, Green and Carlson1981; Barrera and Garrisonjones, Reference Barrera and Garrisonjones1988; Whitaker et al., Reference Whitaker, Johnson, Shaffer, Rapoport, Kalikow, Walsh, Davies, Braiman and Dolinsky1990; Ambrosini et al., Reference Ambrosini, Metz, Bianchi, Rabinovich and Undie1991; Marton et al., Reference Marton, Churchard, Kutcher and Korenblum1991; Canals et al., Reference Canals, Blade, Carbajo and Domenech-Llaberia2001).

For all of the above ROI analyses, the threshold for statistical significance was set at (p < 0.007) following Bonferroni adjustment for seven multiple ROI comparisons. We also tested the association between dimensional anxiety scores and brain activation in an ANCOVA, with Anxiety Severity (total score on the anxiety subscale of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale) as the covariate of interest.

In order to model the effects of lurasidone and depression status beyond the fronto-striatal network targeted in the ROI analyses, exploratory whole brain analyses were also conducted (see the online Supplementary Methods and Results).

Results

Behavioural results

A repeated measures ANCOVA with Medication (placebo or lurasidone) and Cue Type (reward, penalty, neutral) as the within-subject variables, Medication Order (placebo-lurasidone, lurasidone-placebo) as the between-subject variable and Depression Severity (total BDI-II score) as the covariate of interest was completed for (i) Total Winnings, (ii) Mean Reaction Time (RT) and (iii) Accuracy. Performance data are presented in online Table S2 in the Supplementary Results. In all analyses, there were no effects of Medication Order or interactions with Medication Order (all p values > 0.050). In all analyses there were no significant three-way interactions between either (i) Total Winnings, (ii) Mean RT or (iii) Accuracy and Medication and Depression Severity. Significant two way interactions between Cue Type and Mean RT, and Cue Type and Accuracy are presented in the online Supplementary Results. We also examined the effect of Medication, Medication Order and Depression Severity on the change in Sedation ratings (total VAS scores) and State-anxiety ratings (total STAI score) from pre-drug administration (Measure 1) to peak-of-drug (Measure 2). There were no significant main effects or interactions (all p values > 0.050) (please refer to the online Supplementary Results).

Reward processing (blood-oxygen-level dependent signal) results

Response to outcomes

Primary analyses

These primary analyses are conducted with depression measured as a continuous variable. In order to test the hypothesis that lurasidone would increase activation to reward outcomes and decrease responses to penalties in depressed individuals, we conducted a repeated-measures ANCOVA. Medication (placebo, lurasidone) and Outcome Type (Reward Outcome v. Penalty Outcome) were the within-subject variables, Medication Order was the between-subject factor and Depression Severity (total BDI-II score) was the covariate of interest (n = 40). Three participants were excluded from the analyses (please refer to the online Supplementary Results). The repeated measures ANCOVA revealed a significant Medication-by-Depression Severity-by-Outcome Type interaction in the ACC (F = 8.10, df = 1, 37, p = 0.007), after passing Bonferroni adjustment for seven multiple ROI comparisons. The interaction fell short of Bonferroni-adjusted significance in the OFC (F = 4.47, df = 1, 37, p = 0.041) and insula (F = 4.90, df = 1, 37, p = 0.033). There were no significant interactions with Medication Order (all p values >0.050).

To understand the significant three-way interaction, we conducted two repeated-measures ANCOVAs for Reward Outcome (n = 41) and Penalty Outcome separately (n = 41 after excluding outliers, please refer to online Supplementary Results).

This revealed a significant Medication-by-Depression Severity-by-Penalty Outcome interaction in the ACC (F = 11.98, df = 1, 38, p = 0.001). Figure 2 demonstrates that under placebo, individuals with higher depressive symptoms had greater ACC activity during penalty outcomes. However, this trend was not found under lurasidone. Put simply, brain activity to penalties in the ACC in individuals with elevated depression scores under lurasidone, but not placebo, resembles brain activity of individuals with low depressive symptoms. In keeping with this result, we found that ΔACC (the difference between neural activity under lurasidone and placebo) was negatively correlated with depression severity. Figure 3 illustrates the finding that the absolute difference in neural activity between lurasidone and placebo increased as a function of depression scores.

Fig. 2. Facet plot illustrating ACC response during Penalty Outcome across continuous depression scores under lurasidone and placebo. Dashed vertical line denotes depression severity cut-off score on the BDI-II.

Fig. 3. Intra-individual change in penalty related ACC activity (the difference between neural activity under lurasidone and placebo) as a function of continuous depression scores.

A similar pattern of results, namely a signal normalisation, was found in the OFC (F = 4.94, df = 1, 37, p = 0.032), but the interaction fell short of significance after Bonferroni adjustment (see the online Supplementary Results).

We then examined the Medication-by-Depression Severity-by-Reward Outcome interaction across the seven ROIs. This also displayed a pattern of signal normalisation, although in an opposite direction to Penalty Outcome, as lurasidone had its strongest effect of increasing responses to reward outcomes in individuals with high depression severity. This trend fell short of significance in the NAcc (F = 4.87, df = 1, 38, p = 0.033) and ACC (F = 5.92, df = 1, 37, p = 0.020) following Bonferroni correction.

Secondary analyses

Complementing the primary (continuous variable) analyses, we sought to replicate our results using categorical analyses. A repeated-measures ANOVA with Medication (placebo, lurasidone) and Outcome Type (Reward Outcome v. Penalty Outcome) as the within-subject variables and Depression Group (low v. high depressive symptoms) and Medication Order as the between-subject factors (n = 40), revealed a significant Medication-by-Depression Group-by-Outcome Type interaction in the ACC (F = 8.68, df = 1, 38, p = 0.005).

Figure 4 illustrates these findings using BDI-II cut-off scores, with individuals with low depressive symptoms (total BDI-II score: 0–16, n = 24) v. high depressive symptoms (total BDI-II score: 17–43, n = 18). Post-hoc t tests showed that participants with high depressive symptoms receiving placebo had significantly greater ACC activation to Penalty Outcomes than participants with high depressive symptoms receiving lurasidone (T = 2.17, df = 19, p = 0.043), and participants with low depressive symptoms receiving placebo (T = 2.32, df = 37, p = 0.026). There was no significant difference between individuals with high BDI-II scores on lurasidone and individuals with low BDI-II scores on placebo (T = 0.48, df = 37, p = 0.634). Together, these findings indicate that brain activity to penalties in the ACC in individuals with elevated depression scores under lurasidone, but not placebo, resembles brain activity of healthy volunteers.

Fig. 4. Box plot illustrating ACC Response to Penalty Outcomes (loss > avoided loss). Depression severity cut-off scores from the BDI-II, with individuals with low depressive symptoms (total BDI-II score: 0–16, n = 24) v. high depressive symptoms (total BDI-II score: 17–43, n = 18).

To summarise, across reward and penalty outcomes, lurasidone had its strongest effect of increasing responses to reward outcomes and decreasing responses to penalty outcomes in individuals with high depression severity (Figs 2–4). The pattern and significance of the results remained when the outliers were included in the analysis (see the online Supplementary Results).

Response to cues

In contrast to the outcome results, there were no significant interactions with depression in the anticipation phase of the task. Instead, the repeated measures ANCOVA revealed a significant Medication-by-Anticipation Cue interaction in the ACC (F = 8.16, df = 2, 72, p = 0.001) and caudate (F = 7.78, df = 2, 72, p = 0.001). Post-hoc tests show that lurasidone reduced responses to win and loss cues v. placebo, and increased responses for neutral cues in the ACC and caudate. This fell short of significance in the OFC (F = 3.94, df = 2, 72, p = 0.024) and amygdala (F = 3.85, df = 2, 72, p = 0.026).

Anxiety severity analyses and exploratory whole-brain findings for the anticipatory and outcome phases of the task are presented in the online Results Section of the Supplementary data.

Cerebral blood flow (CBF)

In order to ensure that the BOLD results in the ACC were independent of changes in underlying CBF, we tested the effects of acute lurasidone administration on global and regional blood flow. As shown in Fig. 5, a paired-samples t test across the whole-brain showed that lurasidone increased CBF in bilateral putamen relative to placebo during rest in the whole sample (n = 43). Significant increases in blood flow were not observed in the ACC. The repeated measures ANCOVA revealed that the extracted global and striatal CBF values were not related to Depression Severity (F = 0.02, df = 1, 40, p = 0.903), Medication Order (F = 0.44, df = 1, 40, p = 0.903), or any three-way interactions with these respective factors (F = 0.01, df = 1, 40, p = 0.952); (F = 1.10, df = 1, 40, p = 0.300). The change in CBF values for each of the seven ROIs were extracted and used as covariates for the same region in the fMRI BOLD analyses. This did not lead to any changes in the results: non-significant results remained non-significant and significant results remained significant. In particular, the Medication-by-Depression Severity-by-Outcome Type interaction in the ACC (F = 8.13, df = 1, 36, p = 0.007).

Fig. 5. Increased CBF in bilateral putamen for lurasidone relative to placebo during rest in the whole sample (n = 43). Significant at the peak level whole-brain analyses, family-wise error-corrected (left putamen x = −26, y = −4, z = 2, t = 6.15: p = 0.002, right putamen x = 28, y = −2, z = 2, t = 5.50: p = 0.015). Bar represents T-value.

Discussion

In this study, we compared the effects of lurasidone and placebo on neural responding to reward and penalties in medication-naïve young-adult subjects across the range of depression severity. During the anticipation phase of the task, we found that lurasidone reduced responses to win and loss cues v. placebo, and increased responses for neutral cues in the ACC and caudate across the entire sample (i.e. regardless of depression severity). We found that brain activity in the ACC to Penalty Outcomes in individuals with high symptoms of depression under lurasidone, but not placebo, resembled brain activity of individuals with low symptoms of depression. Specifically, lurasidone reduced ACC signalling to negative feedback in young people with elevated depressive symptoms. Increased regional and global blood flow under lurasidone did not drive the BOLD findings. These results provide evidence for abnormalities in neural reward-penalty systems in depression and highlight the potential of targeted pharmacological treatments (dopaminergic agents) to normalise penalty related processing in depression.

Our findings are consistent with the notion that acute dose of drugs with antidepressant properties, either as used in monotherapy or combination treatment, can have an effect on brain processes implicated in depression (Harmer et al., Reference Harmer, Duman and Cowen2017). For example, SSRIs reduce negative bias and amygdala response to negative emotional stimuli (Harmer et al., Reference Harmer, O'Sullivan, Favaron, Massey-Chase, Ayres, Reinecke, Goodwin and Cowen2009; Murphy et al., Reference Murphy, Norbury, O'Sullivan, Cowen and Harmer2009). In our study, the effects of normalisation where localised to the ACC, a region that integrates diverse striatal and prefrontal functions (Haber and Knutson, Reference Haber and Knutson2010). For example, the ACC and ventral striatum (VS) show functional connectivity at rest (Pan et al., Reference Pan, Sato, Salum, Rohde, Gadelha, Zugman, Mari, Jackowski, Picon, Miguel, Pine, Leibenluft, Bressan and Stringaris2017) and input from the ACC to the VS allows for flexible deployment and adaptation of behaviour to changing circumstances (Holroyd and Coles, Reference Holroyd and Coles2002; Walton et al., Reference Walton, Croxson, Behrens, Kennerley and Rushworth2007; Alexander and Brown, Reference Alexander and Brown2011; Holroyd and Yeung, Reference Holroyd and Yeung2012; Walsh and Anderson, Reference Walsh and Anderson2012; Holroyd and Umemoto, Reference Holroyd and Umemoto2016; Umemoto and Holroyd, Reference Umemoto and Holroyd2016; Shahnazian and Holroyd, Reference Shahnazian and Holroyd2017). Electrophysiological (electroencephalogram) studies have shown that the feedback negativity (FRN), an event-related potential which indicates the early appraisal of feedback and appears larger following the presentation of negative feedback, has its origins in the ACC (Gehring and Willoughby, Reference Gehring and Willoughby2002; Holroyd and Coles, Reference Holroyd and Coles2002; Holroyd et al., Reference Holroyd, Larsen and Cohen2004; Hajcak et al., Reference Hajcak, Holroyd, Moser and Simons2005; Yeung et al., Reference Yeung, Holroyd and Cohen2005). Specifically, an FRN signal may be generated as ACC neurons shift from encoding expected to actual outcomes (i.e. a prediction error signal) (Hyman et al., Reference Hyman, Holroyd and Seamans2017). In our study, participants with higher depression severity on placebo showed greater ACC response to negative feedback. This is congruent with evidence of heightened sensitivity to negative outcomes in depression and its association with elevated loss-related signals in the ACC, and connected regions such as the anterior insula and striatum.

It has been postulated that increased ACC activity in depressed individuals to loss outcomes reflects biased stimuli representations that mediate choice behaviour, including preferential attention, planning and self-referential processing towards losses (Sylvester et al., Reference Sylvester, Wager, Lacey, Hernandez, Nichols, Smith and Jonides2003; Grimm et al., Reference Grimm, Ernst, Boesiger, Schuepbach, Hell, Boeker and Northoff2009; Gotlib et al., Reference Gotlib, Hamilton, Cooney, Singh, Henry and Joormann2010). A normalisation of ACC response in depressed individuals on lurasidone therefore suggests that lurasidone may act to decrease salience and processing of loss events.

Inter-individual differences between low and high depression severity subjects could account for the findings that lurasidone attenuated response to penalty outcomes in individuals with high depression severity only. Indeed, depression is associated with baseline differences in availability and function of 5-HT and/or D2 receptors and reductions in binding relative to healthy volunteers (Suhara et al., Reference Suhara, Nakayama, Inoue, Fukuda, Shimizu, Mori and Tateno1992; Yatham et al., Reference Yatham, Liddle, Dennie, Shiah, Adam, Lane, Lam and Ruth1999; Sheline et al., Reference Sheline, Mintun, Barch, Wilkins, Snyder and Moerlein2004; Yatham et al., Reference Yatham, Goldstein, Vieta, Bowden, Grunze, Post, Suppes and Calabrese2005). Thus, in accordance with previous findings that more divergent patterns of reward/penalty processing at baseline are associated with greater post-intervention change (Vrieze et al., Reference Vrieze, Pizzagalli, Demyttenaere, Hompes, Sienaert, de Boer, Schmidt and Claes2013; Rice et al., Reference Rice, Rawal, Riglin, Lewis and Dunsmuir2015; Burkhouse et al., Reference Burkhouse, Kujawa, Kennedy, Shankman, Langenecker, Phan and Klumpp2016; Walsh et al., Reference Walsh, Carl, Eisenlohr-Moul, Minkel, Crowther, Moore, Gibbs, Petty, Bizzell, Smoski and Dichter2016), it could be that subjects with more severe depressive symptoms have more ‘room for improvement’ following acute lurasidone administration.

In addition to attenuating penalty outcome responses, lurasidone reduced neural responses in the ACC and caudate during the anticipation of loss and reward cues across the entire sample (i.e. regardless of depression severity). This is in line with studies showing attenuated reward-related striatal activation during reward anticipation and decision making with D2 antagonist haloperidol (Pessiglione et al., Reference Pessiglione, Seymour, Flandin, Dolan and Frith2006; Pleger et al., Reference Pleger, Ruff, Blankenburg, Kloppel, Driver and Dolan2009). However, no effect has been reported for prediction of losses (Pessiglione et al., Reference Pessiglione, Seymour, Flandin, Dolan and Frith2006). There are various mechanisms which could account for the finding in this study, all of which are speculative at the moment. First, lurasidone may modulate tonic and phasic dopamine firing either directly by D2 antagonism or indirectly via antagonism at serotonergic 5-HT receptors (5-HT2A, 5-HT7). Antagonism at D2 receptors could act to block and reduce dopamine release, thereby also attenuating the BOLD signal. Alternatively, lurasidone may, at low doses, like amisulpride increase striatal dopamine release by preferentially blocking presynaptic dopamine auto-receptors. Increased dopamine availability may act to increase tonic levels of dopamine, in turn decreasing the phasic firing of dopamine neurons and the sensitivity of the dopamine reward system (Grace, Reference Grace1991), thereby potentially reducing BOLD signal to anticipation cues. Although it must be noted that ascribing the changes seen to one or more receptor systems is highly speculative as the precise mechanism by which BOLD signal is modulated cannot be determined with fMRI alone.

It is notable, that in line with previous studies utilising dopamine antagonists (Lahti et al., Reference Lahti, Holcomb, Weiler, Medoff and Tamminga2003; Lahti et al., Reference Lahti, Weiler, Medoff, Tamminga and Holcomb2005; Handley et al., Reference Handley, Zelaya, Reinders, Marques, Mehta, O'Gorman, Alsop, Taylor, Johnston, Williams, McGuire, Pariante, Kapur and Dazzan2013; Goozee et al., Reference Goozee, Handley, Kempton and Dazzan2014), we show here that lurasidone increased striatal CBF at rest. Increases in blood flow following antipsychotic lurasidone administration may be related to increased neuronal metabolism in striatal areas due to the large density of D2 receptors (Goozee et al., Reference Goozee, Handley, Kempton and Dazzan2014), with blockade of D2 receptors in the striatum potentially resulting in disinhibition of D2 receptor-containing medium spiny neurons (Fernandez-Seara et al., Reference Fernandez-Seara, Aznarez-Sanado, Mengual, Irigoyen, Heukamp and Pastor2011). Our results showed that the penalty and reward-related findings were unchanged after controlling for baseline shifts in global and striatal CBF, and highlight the utility of multi-modal fMRI in identifying if the effects of the drug administered are indeed neuronal.

Our study also showed a pattern in which lurasidone potentiated striatal (NAcc) activity to reward outcomes in young adults with elevated depressive symptoms. These findings did not survive stringent correction for multiple comparisons and should therefore be interpreted with appropriate caution. We note, that these results are in keeping with recent findings by Admon et al. (Reference Admon, Kaiser, Dillon, Beltzer, Goer, Olson, Vitaliano and Pizzagalli2017) who showed that a single-dose of the dopamine receptor antagonist amisulpride normalises reward processing by increasing reward-related striatal activation and connectivity between the striatum and mid-cingulate cortex in depressed individuals.

This study has several strengths. First, we tested the association between reward processing and depression using randomisation and experimental manipulation, thereby overcoming several of the limitations of correlational studies in drawing causal inferences. Second, the cross-over, within-subject design affords higher statistical power than a parallel design by minimising subject variance as each individual acts as their own control, and increasing the drug variance. Third, we recruited medication-naïve subjects across the range of depression severity, thus avoiding the confound of medication (Pessiglione et al., Reference Pessiglione, Seymour, Flandin, Dolan and Frith2006; Abler et al., Reference Abler, Erk and Walter2007).

This study also has limitations. Caution should be exercised with the interpretation of our results as ‘normalising’. In the absence of any behavioural effect there is no evidence for better performance of the task on lurasidone, there is no clear main effect of depressive symptoms on the task (i.e. no deficit to improve) and no other triangulating measure of response to negative outcomes which can be linked to function. Nevertheless, it could be argued that lurasidone changes an activity in the ACC that might be beneficial. Further studies need to address both the behavioural deficit and the neural changes in parallel. This has proven challenging as it requires alignment between different levels of explanation including task, neural, clinical and behavioural (Keren et al., Reference Keren, O'Callaghan, Vidal-Ribas, Buzzell, Brotman, Pan, Meffert, Kaisera, Wolke, Pine and Stringaris2018).

Our study was not designed to capture changes in depressive symptoms following lurasidone and therefore it is unclear how these would correlate with brain responses. However, our strategy of searching for the signal of an intervention in the first place is consistent with current recommendations to boost drug discovery (Krystal and State, Reference Krystal and State2014). The next piece of information which would be needed to infer causality, is whether lurasidone-induced neural changes (reduced penalty related ACC signalling and increased reward-related NAcc signalling) predict a decline in depressive and anhedonic symptoms (Shiroma et al., Reference Shiroma, Thuras, Johns and Lim2014; Godlewska et al., Reference Godlewska, Browning, Norbury, Cowen and Harmer2016). This would require longer-term lurasidone treatment in longitudinal studies with assessment of pre-post changes in behavioural and neural responses. Antidepressants seem to exacerbate reward deficits early in treatment (Kumar et al., Reference Kumar, Waiter, Ahearn, Milders, Reid and Steele2008; McCabe et al., Reference McCabe, Mishor, Cowen and Harmer2010; Marutani et al., Reference Marutani, Yahata, Ikeda, Ito, Yamamoto, Matsuura, Matsushima, Okubo, Suzuki and Matsuda2011) prior to normalisation following longer-term (2–6 week) treatment (Stoy et al., Reference Stoy, Schlagenhauf, Sterzer, Bermpohl, Haegele, Suchotzki, Schmack, Wrase, Ricken, Knutson, Adli, Bauer, Heinz and Stroehle2012; Scholl et al., Reference Scholl, Kolling, Nelissen, Browning, Rushworth and Harmer2017; Walsh et al., Reference Walsh, Browning, Drevets, Furey and Harmer2017). Thus, in line with longer-term dosing studies, repeated dosing with lurasidone could lead to increasing anticipation of rewards with more chronic exposure to the drug. Although speculative, one could predict a behavioural activation model of the antidepressant mechanism of action of lurasidone, with normalisation of responses to outcomes (consummation), prior to a normalisation of neural anticipatory signals with longer-term treatment (Dimidjian et al., Reference Dimidjian, Barrera, Martell, Munoz and Lewinsohn2011).

We note that we used two contrasts for the outcome type: reward and penalty outcome. Whilst this is standard in the literature in similarly designed studies (Admon et al., Reference Admon, Kaiser, Dillon, Beltzer, Goer, Olson, Vitaliano and Pizzagalli2017), an alternative modelling could be four levels: reward, missed reward, penalty and avoided penalty outcomes relative to no incentive outcomes. In addition, the recruitment was designed for analysis of depressive symptoms as a continuum, and as such any analysis of those with higher scores contrasted with lower scores may be underpowered. Interestingly we were able to replicate the results of Admon et al. (Reference Admon, Kaiser, Dillon, Beltzer, Goer, Olson, Vitaliano and Pizzagalli2017), as lurasidone potentiated striatal (NAcc) activity to reward outcomes using such categories but this did not survive correction for multiple comparisons.

In conclusion, our study shows that an acute dose of dopaminergic agent, lurasidone, transiently decreased penalty related ACC activity in individuals with high symptoms of depression. These findings suggest that modulation of dopamine transmission may help to normalise processing of negative outcomes in depressed individuals through the alteration of ACC signalling. Thus, ACC signalling may provide a new target for engagement in future drug development studies. Using an experimental medicine design such as the one used in this study, could help identify relevant compounds which could then be tested further in using longer-term follow-up.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718003306.

Acknowledgements

We thank all participants who have generously given their time to taking part in this study. We thank the Wellcome Trust (093909/Z/10/A) and National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre (BRC) at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King's College London for funding this independent research project. We also thank the NIHR-BRC for funding Selina Wolke's Ph.D. The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health. We thank Miss Hazel Deacon for her assistance in administration and data entry. We also thank Brian Knutson for providing the images used in the MID task. Preliminary results from this study were presented (via poster) at the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry (AACAP) 63rd Annual Meeting, New York, NY, USA, 24–29 October 2016 and the International Society for Bipolar Disorders Annual Conference, Washington DC, USA, 4–7 May 2017.

Funding

This study was funded by the Wellcome Trust (093909/Z/10/A) and National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre (BRC) at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College London.

Disclosure

Selina Wolke’s Ph.D. is funded through a 3-year National Institutes of Health Research (NIHR) Maudsley Biomedical Research Centre (BRC) Ph.D. Studentship based at the Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London. Dr Argyris Stringaris has received funding from the Wellcome Trust and the UK National Institutes of Health Research, funds from University College London for a joint project with Johnson & Johnson, and royalties from Cambridge University Press and Oxford University Press. Prof. Mitul Mehta has received research funding from Eli Lilly, Roche and Takeda and has acted as a consultant for Cambridge Cognition and Lundbeck. Prof. Allan Young is employed by King’s College London and is an Honorary Consultant SLaM (NHS UK) and has received grant funding from the Medical Research Council and Wellcome Trust (UK). Prof. Young is part of advisory boards for all major pharmaceutical companies with drugs used in affective and related disorders and has no shareholdings in pharmaceutical companies. Prof. Young has initiated studies for Embolden Study (AZ), BCI Neuroplasticity study, Aripiprazole Mania Study, Eli Lilly, Lundbeck and Wyeth. Dr Owen O’Daly and Dr Fernando Zelaya are with the Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London and report no potential conflicts of interest. Dr Nada Zahreddine is based at the Department of Psychiatry, Saint-Joseph University, Beirut, Lebanon and has nothing to declare. Drs Stringaris, Leibenluft, Pine, Keren and O’Callaghan are with the National Institute of Mental Health and have nothing to declare.

Author ORCIDs

Selina A. Wolke, 0000-0001-6762-0456; Allan H. Young, 0000-0003-2291-6952.

References

Abler, B, Erk, S and Walter, H (2007) Human reward system activation is modulated by a single dose of olanzapine in healthy subjects in an event-related, double-blind, placebo-controlled fMRI study. Psychopharmacology 191, 823833.Google Scholar
Admon, R, Nickerson, LD, Dillon, DG, Holmes, AJ, Bogdan, R, Kumar, P, Dougherty, DD, Iosifescu, DV, Mischoulon, D, Fava, M and Pizzagalli, DA (2015) Dissociable cortico-striatal connectivity abnormalities in major depression in response to monetary gains and penalties. Psychological Medicine 45, 121131.Google Scholar
Admon, R, Kaiser, RH, Dillon, DG, Beltzer, M, Goer, F, Olson, DP, Vitaliano, G and Pizzagalli, DA (2017) Dopaminergic enhancement of striatal response to reward in major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry 174, 378386.Google Scholar
Alexander, WH and Brown, JW (2011) Medial prefrontal cortex as an action-outcome predictor. Nature Neuroscience 14, 13381344.Google Scholar
Ambrosini, PJ, Metz, C, Bianchi, MD, Rabinovich, H and Undie, A (1991) Concurrent validity and psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory in outpatient adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 30, 5157.Google Scholar
Angst, J, Sellar, R and Merikangas, KR (2000) Depressive spectrum diagnoses. Comprehensive Psychiatry 41, 3947.Google Scholar
Ayuso-Mateos, JL, Nuevo, R, Verdes, E, Naidoo, N and Chatterji, S (2010) From depressive symptoms to depressive disorders: the relevance of thresholds. The British Journal of Psychiatry 196, 365371.Google Scholar
Barrera, M and Garrisonjones, CV (1988) Properties of the Beck depression inventory as a screening instrument for adolescent depression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 16, 263273.Google Scholar
Bartra, O, McGuire, JT and Kable, JW (2013) The valuation system: a coordinate-based meta-analysis of BOLD fMRI experiments examining neural correlates of subjective value. Neuroimage 76, 412427.Google Scholar
Beck, AT, Steer, RA, Ball, R and Ranieri, WF (1996) Comparison of Beck Depression Inventories-IA and -II in psychiatric outpatients. Journal of Personality Assessment 67, 588597.Google Scholar
Bolstad, I, Andreassen, OA, Groote, I, Server, A, Sjaastad, I, Kapur, S and Jensen, J (2015) Effects of haloperidol and aripiprazole on the human mesolimbic motivational system: a pharmacological fMRI study. European Neuropsychopharmacology 25, 22522261.Google Scholar
Boureau, YL and Dayan, P (2011) Opponency revisited: competition and cooperation between dopamine and serotonin. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 7497.Google Scholar
Burkhouse, KL, Kujawa, A, Kennedy, AE, Shankman, SA, Langenecker, SA, Phan, KL and Klumpp, H (2016) Neural reactivity to reward as a predictor of cognitive behavioral therapy response in anxiety and depression. Depression and Anxiety 33, 281288.Google Scholar
Canals, J, Blade, J, Carbajo, G and Domenech-Llaberia, ED (2001) The Beck Depression Inventory: psychometric characteristics and usefulness in nonclinical adolescents. European Journal of Psychological Assessment 17, 6368.Google Scholar
Carl, H, Walsh, E, Eisenlohr-Moul, T, Minkel, J, Crowther, A, Moore, T, Gibbs, D, Petty, C, Bizzell, J, Dichter, GS and Smoski, MJ (2016) Sustained anterior cingulate cortex activation during reward processing predicts response to psychotherapy in major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders 203, 204212.Google Scholar
Chowdhury, R, Guitart-Masip, M, Lambert, C, Dayan, P, Huys, Q, Düzel, E and Dolan, RJ (2013) Dopamine restores reward prediction errors in old age. Nature Neuroscience 16, 648653.Google Scholar
Cohen, JY, Amoroso, MW and Uchida, N (2015) Serotonergic neurons signal reward and punishment on multiple timescales. Elife 4, 125.Google Scholar
Dean, Z, Horndasch, S, Giannopoulos, P and McCabe, C (2016) Enhanced neural response to anticipation, effort and consummation of reward and aversion during bupropion treatment. Psychological Medicine 46, 22632274.Google Scholar
Diekhof, EK, Kaps, L, Falkai, P and Gruber, O (2012) The role of the human ventral striatum and the medial orbitofrontal cortex in the representation of reward magnitude – an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of passive reward expectancy and outcome processing. Neuropsychologia 50, 12521266.Google Scholar
Dimidjian, S, Barrera, M, Martell, C, Munoz, RF and Lewinsohn, PM (2011) The origins and current status of behavioral activation treatments for depression. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 7, 138.Google Scholar
Dubol, M, Trichard, C, Leroy, C, Sandu, AL, Rahim, M, Granger, B, Tzavara, ET, Karila, L, Martinot, JL and Artiges, E (2018) Dopamine transporter and reward anticipation in a dimensional perspective: a multimodal brain imaging study. Neuropsychopharmacology 43, 820827.Google Scholar
Engelmann, JB, Berns, GS and Dunlop, BW (2017) Hyper-responsivity to losses in the anterior insula during economic choice scales with depression severity. Psychological Medicine 47, 113.Google Scholar
Fernandez-Seara, MA, Aznarez-Sanado, M, Mengual, E, Irigoyen, J, Heukamp, F and Pastor, MA (2011) Effects on resting cerebral blood flow and functional connectivity induced by metoclopramide: a perfusion MRI study in healthy volunteers. British Journal of Pharmacology 163, 16391652.Google Scholar
Forbes, EE, Hariri, AR, Martin, SL, Silk, JS, Moyles, DL, Fisher, PM, Brown, SM, Ryan, ND, Birmaher, B, Axelson, DA and Dahl, RE (2009) Altered striatal activation predicting real-world positive affect in adolescent major depressive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 166, 6473.Google Scholar
Friston, KJ, Williams, S, Howard, R, Frackowiak, RSJ and Turner, R (1996) Movement-related effects in fMRI time-series. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine 35, 346355.Google Scholar
Gehring, WJ and Willoughby, AR (2002) The medial frontal cortex and the rapid processing of monetary gains and losses. Science 295, 22792282.Google Scholar
Godlewska, BR, Browning, M, Norbury, R, Cowen, PJ and Harmer, CJ (2016) Early changes in emotional processing as a marker of clinical response to SSRI treatment in depression. Translational Psychiatry 6, 17.Google Scholar
Goldberg, JF, Ng-Mak, D, Siu, C, Chuang, CC, Rajagopalan, K and Loebel, A (2017) Remission and recovery associated with lurasidone in the treatment of major depressive disorder with subthreshold hypomanic symptoms (mixed features): post-hoc analysis of a randomized, placebo-controlled study with longer-term extension. CNS Spectrums 22, 373373.Google Scholar
Goozee, R, Handley, R, Kempton, MJ and Dazzan, P (2014) A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effects of antipsychotic medications on regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) in schizophrenia: association with response to treatment. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 43, 118136.Google Scholar
Gotlib, IH, Hamilton, JP, Cooney, RE, Singh, MK, Henry, ML and Joormann, J (2010) Neural processing of reward and loss in girls at risk for major depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 67, 380387.Google Scholar
Grace, AA (1991) Phasic versus tonic dopamine release and the modulation of dopamine system responsivity – a hypothesis for the etiology of schizophrenia. Neuroscience 41, 124.Google Scholar
Greenberg, WM and Citrome, L (2017) Pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of lurasidone hydrochloride, a second-generation antipsychotic: a systematic review of the published literature. Clinical Pharmacokinetics 56, 493503.Google Scholar
Grimm, S, Ernst, J, Boesiger, P, Schuepbach, D, Hell, D, Boeker, H and Northoff, G (2009) Increased self-focus in major depressive disorder is related to neural abnormalities in subcortical-cortical midline structures. Human Brain Mapping 30, 26172627.Google Scholar
Haber, SN and Knutson, B (2010) The reward circuit: linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 426.Google Scholar
Hajcak, G, Holroyd, CB, Moser, JS and Simons, RF (2005) Brain potentials associated with expected and unexpected good and bad outcomes. Psychophysiology 42, 161170.Google Scholar
Handley, R, Zelaya, FO, Reinders, AATS, Marques, TR, Mehta, MA, O'Gorman, R, Alsop, DC, Taylor, H, Johnston, A, Williams, S, McGuire, P, Pariante, CM, Kapur, S and Dazzan, P (2013) Acute effects of single-dose aripiprazole and haloperidol on resting cerebral blood flow (rCBF) in the human brain. Human Brain Mapping 34, 272282.Google Scholar
Harmer, CJ, O'Sullivan, U, Favaron, E, Massey-Chase, R, Ayres, R, Reinecke, A, Goodwin, GM and Cowen, PJ (2009) Effect of acute antidepressant administration on negative affective bias in depressed patients. American Journal of Psychiatry 166, 11781184.Google Scholar
Harmer, CJ, Duman, RS and Cowen, PJ (2017) How do antidepressants work? New perspectives for refining future treatment approaches. The Lancet Psychiatry 4, 409418.Google Scholar
Hayashi, K, Nakao, K and Nakamura, K (2015) Appetitive and aversive information coding in the primate dorsal raphe nucleus. Journal of Neuroscience 35, 61956208.Google Scholar
Herbert, M, Johns, MW and Doré, C (1976) Factor analysis of analogue scales measuring subjective feelings before and after sleep. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice 49, 373379.Google Scholar
Hevey, D, Thomas, K, Laureano-Schelten, S, Looney, K and Booth, R (2017) Clinical depression and punishment sensitivity on the BART. Frontiers in Psychology 8, 17.Google Scholar
Holroyd, CB and Coles, MGH (2002) The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review 109, 679709.Google Scholar
Holroyd, CB and Umemoto, A (2016) The research domain criteria framework: the case for anterior cingulate cortex. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 71, 418443.Google Scholar
Holroyd, CB and Yeung, N (2012) Motivation of extended behaviors by anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16, 122128.Google Scholar
Holroyd, CB, Larsen, JT and Cohen, JD (2004) Context dependence of the event-related brain potential associated with reward and punishment. Psychophysiology 41, 245253.Google Scholar
Huang, M, Horiguchi, M, Felix, AR and Meltzer, HY (2012) 5-HT1A and 5-HT7 receptors contribute to lurasidone-induced dopamine efflux. Neuroreport 23, 436440.Google Scholar
Hyman, JM, Holroyd, CB and Seamans, JK (2017) A novel neural prediction error found in anterior cingulate cortex ensembles. Neuron 95, 447456.Google Scholar
Inaba, K, Mizuhiki, T, Setogawa, T, Toda, K, Richmond, BJ and Shidara, M (2013) Neurons in monkey dorsal raphe nucleus code beginning and progress of step-by-step schedule, reward expectation, and amount of reward outcome in the reward schedule task. Journal of Neuroscience 33, 34773491.Google Scholar
Jocham, G, Klein, TA and Ullsperger, M (2011) Dopamine-mediated reinforcement learning signals in the striatum and ventromedial prefrontal cortex underlie value-based choices. Journal of Neuroscience 31, 16061613.Google Scholar
Jocham, G, Klein, TA and Ullsperger, M (2014) Differential modulation of reinforcement learning by d2 dopamine and NMDA glutamate receptor antagonism. Journal of Neuroscience 34, 1315113162.Google Scholar
Keren, H, O'Callaghan, G, Vidal-Ribas, P, Buzzell, G, Brotman, MEL, Pan, P, Meffert, L, Kaisera, A, Wolke, S, Pine, D and Stringaris, A (2018) Reward processing in depression: a conceptual and meta-analytic review across electrophysiological and fMRI studies. American Journal of Psychiatry 175, 11111120.Google Scholar
Knutson, B, Adams, CM, Fong, GW and Hommer, D (2001) Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens. Journal of Neuroscience 21, 15.Google Scholar
Knutson, B, Bhanji, JP, Cooney, RE, Atlas, LY and Gotlib, IH (2008) Neural responses to monetary incentives in major depression. Biological Psychiatry 63, 686692.Google Scholar
Krefetz, DG, Steer, RA, Gulab, NA and Beck, AT (2002) Convergent validity of the Beck Depression Inventory-II with the Reynolds Adolescent Depression Scale in psychiatric inpatients. Journal of Personality Assessment 78, 451460.Google Scholar
Kriegeskorte, N, Simmons, WK, Bellgowan, PS and Baker, CI (2009) Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: the dangers of double dipping. Nature Neuroscience 12, 535540.Google Scholar
Krystal, JH and State, MW (2014) Psychiatric disorders: diagnosis to therapy. Cell 157, 201214.Google Scholar
Kumar, G, Steer, RA, Teitelman, KB and Villacis, L (2002) Effectiveness of Beck Depression Inventory-II subscales in screening for major depressive disorders in adolescent psychiatric inpatients. Assessment 9, 164170.Google Scholar
Kumar, P, Waiter, G, Ahearn, T, Milders, M, Reid, I and Steele, JD (2008) Abnormal temporal difference reward-learning signals in major depression. Brain 131, 20842093.Google Scholar
la Fougere, C, Meisenzahl, E, Schmitt, G, Stauss, J, Frodl, T, Tatsch, K, Hahn, K, Moller, HJ and Dresel, S (2005) D-2 receptor occupancy during high- and low-dose therapy with the atypical antipsychotic amisulpride: a I-123-iodobenzamide SPECT study. Journal of Nuclear Medicine 46, 10281033.Google Scholar
Lahti, AC, Holcomb, HH, Weiler, MA, Medoff, DR and Tamminga, CA (2003) Functional effects of antipsychotic drugs: comparing clozapine with haloperidol. Biological Psychiatry 53, 601608.Google Scholar
Lahti, AC, Weiler, MA, Medoff, DR, Tamminga, CA and Holcomb, HH (2005) Functional effects of single dose first- and second-generation antipsychotic administration in subjects with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 139, 1930.Google Scholar
Li, Y, Zhong, W, Wang, D, Feng, Q, Liu, Z, Zhou, J, Jia, C, Hu, F, Zeng, J, Guo, Q, Fu, L and Luo, M (2016) Serotonin neurons in the dorsal raphe nucleus encode reward signals. Nature Communications 7, 115.Google Scholar
Liu, Z, Zhou, J, Li, Y, Hu, F, Lu, Y, Ma, M, Feng, Q, Zhang, JE, Wang, D, Zeng, J, Bao, J, Kim, JY, Chen, ZF, El Mestikawy, S and Luo, M (2014) Dorsal raphe neurons signal reward through 5-HT and glutamate. Neuron 81, 13601374.Google Scholar
Loebel, A and Citrome, L (2015) Lurasidone: a novel antipsychotic agent for the treatment of schizophrenia and bipolar depression. BJPsych Bulletin 39, 237241.Google Scholar
Loebel, A, Cucchiaro, J, Silva, R, Kroger, H, Hsu, J, Sarma, K and Sachs, G (2014 a) Lurasidone monotherapy in the treatment of bipolar I depression: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. American Journal of Psychiatry 171, 160168.Google Scholar
Loebel, A, Cucchiaro, J, Silva, R, Kroger, H, Sarma, K, Xu, J and Calabrese, JR (2014 b) Lurasidone as adjunctive therapy with lithium or valproate for the treatment of bipolar i depression: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. American Journal of Psychiatry 171, 169177.Google Scholar
Luking, KR, Pagliaccio, D, Luby, JL and Barch, DM (2016) Depression risk predicts blunted neural responses to gains and enhanced responses to losses in healthy children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 55, 328337.Google Scholar
Macoveanu, J (2014) Serotonergic modulation of reward and punishment: evidence from pharmacological fMRI studies. Brain Research 1556, 1927.Google Scholar
Macoveanu, J, Rowe, JB, Hornboll, B, Elliott, R, Paulson, OB, Knudsen, GM and Siebner, HR (2013) Playing it safe but losing anyway-serotonergic signaling of negative outcomes in dorsomedial prefrontal cortex in the context of risk-aversion. European Neuropsychopharmacology 23, 919930.Google Scholar
Macoveanu, J, Fisher, PM, Haahr, ME, Frokjaer, VG, Knudsen, GM and Siebner, HR (2014) Effects of selective serotonin reuptake inhibition on neural activity related to risky decisions and monetary rewards in healthy males. Neuroimage 99, 434442.Google Scholar
Marton, P, Churchard, M, Kutcher, S and Korenblum, M (1991) Diagnostic utility of the Beck depression inventory with adolescent psychiatric outpatients and inpatients. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry-Revue Canadienne De Psychiatrie 36, 428431.Google Scholar
Marutani, T, Yahata, N, Ikeda, Y, Ito, T, Yamamoto, M, Matsuura, M, Matsushima, E, Okubo, Y, Suzuki, H and Matsuda, T (2011) Functional magnetic resonance imaging study on the effects of acute single administration of paroxetine on motivation-related brain activity. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 65, 191198.Google Scholar
Mato Abad, V, Garcia-Polo, P, O'Daly, O, Antonio Hernandez-Tamames, J and Zelaya, F (2016) ASAP (automatic Software for ASL processing): a toolbox for processing Arterial Spin Labeling images. Magnetic Resonance Imaging 34, 334344.Google Scholar
Matthews, PM and Hampshire, A (2016) Clinical concepts emerging from fMRI functional connectomics. Neuron 91, 511528.Google Scholar
McCabe, C, Mishor, Z, Cowen, PJ and Harmer, CJ (2010) Diminished neural processing of aversive and rewarding stimuli during selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor treatment. Biological Psychiatry 67, 439445.Google Scholar
Mehta, MA, McGowan, SW, Lawrence, AD, Aitken, MRF, Montgomery, AJ and Grasby, PM (2003) Systemic sulpiride modulates striatal blood flow: relationships to spatial working memory and planning. Neuroimage 20, 19821994.Google Scholar
Morris, SE and Cuthbert, BN (2012) Research Domain Criteria: cognitive systems, neural circuits, and dimensions of behavior. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 14, 2937.Google Scholar
Murphy, SE, Norbury, R, O'Sullivan, U, Cowen, PJ and Harmer, CJ (2009) Effect of a single dose of citalopram on amygdala response to emotional faces. British Journal of Psychiatry 194, 535540.Google Scholar
Nelson, C and Willison, J (1991) The Revised National Adult Reading Test–Test Manual. Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson 991, pp. 16.Google Scholar
Nelson, C, Suppes, T, Tsai, J, Mao, Y, Pikalov, A and Loebel, A (2015) Lurasidone for major depressive disorder with mixed features: effect of baseline depression severity on clinical outcome. Neuropsychopharmacology 40, 330331.Google Scholar
Nierenberg, A, Tsai, J, Mao, Y, Pikalov, A, Suppes, T and Loebel, A (2015) Efficacy of lurasidone in major depression with mixed features: pattern of improvement in depressive and manic symptoms. Neuropsychopharmacology 40, 141142.Google Scholar
O'Doherty, J, Dayan, P, Schultz, J, Deichmann, R, Friston, KJ and Dolan, RJ (2004) Dissociable roles of ventral and dorsal striatum in instrumental conditioning. Science 304, 452454.Google Scholar
Oei, NYL, Rombouts, S, Soeter, RP, van Gerven, JM and Both, S (2012) Dopamine modulates reward system activity during subconscious processing of sexual stimuli. Neuropsychopharmacology 37, 17291737.Google Scholar
Pan, PM, Sato, JR, Salum, GA, Rohde, LA, Gadelha, A, Zugman, A, Mari, J, Jackowski, A, Picon, F, Miguel, EC, Pine, DS, Leibenluft, E, Bressan, RA and Stringaris, A (2017) Ventral striatum functional connectivity as a predictor of adolescent depressive disorder in a longitudinal community-based sample. American Journal of Psychiatry 174, 11121119.Google Scholar
Pessiglione, M., Seymour, B., Flandin, G., Dolan, R. J. and Frith, C. D. (2006) Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behaviour in humans. Nature 442, 10421045.Google Scholar
Pizzagalli, DA, Holmes, AJ, Dillon, DG, Goetz, EL, Birk, JL, Bogdan, R, Dougherty, DD, Iosifescu, DV, Rauch, SL and Fava, M (2009) Reduced caudate and nucleus accumbens response to rewards in unmedicated individuals with major depressive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 166, 702710.Google Scholar
Pleger, B, Ruff, CC, Blankenburg, F, Kloppel, S, Driver, J and Dolan, RJ (2009) Influence of dopaminergically mediated reward on somatosensory decision-making. PLoS Biology 7, 110.Google Scholar
Plichta, MM, Schwarz, AJ, Grimm, O, Morgen, K, Mier, D, Haddad, L, Gerdes, ABM, Sauer, C, Tost, H, Esslinger, C, Colman, P, Wilson, F, Kirsch, P and Meyer-Lindenberg, A (2012) Test-retest reliability of evoked BOLD signals from a cognitive-emotive fMRI test battery. Neuroimage 60, 17461758.Google Scholar
Plomin, R, Haworth, CMA and Davis, OSP (2009) Common disorders are quantitative traits. Nature Reviews Genetics 10, 872878.Google Scholar
Rice, F, Rawal, A, Riglin, L, Lewis, G and Dunsmuir, S (2015) Examining reward-seeking, negative self-beliefs and over-general autobiographical memory as mechanisms of change in classroom prevention programs for adolescent depression. Journal of Affective Disorders 186, 320327.Google Scholar
Schoemaker, H, Claustre, Y, Fage, D, Rouquier, L, Chergui, K, Curet, O, Oblin, A, Gonon, F, Carter, C, Benavides, J and Scatton, B (1997) Neurochemical characteristics of amisulpride, an atypical dopamine D-2/D-3 receptor antagonist with both presynaptic and limbic selectivity. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics 280, 8397.Google Scholar
Scholl, J, Kolling, N, Nelissen, N, Browning, M, Rushworth, MFS and Harmer, CJ (2017) Beyond negative valence: 2-week administration of a serotonergic antidepressant enhances both reward and effort learning signals. PLoS Biology 15, 130.Google Scholar
Shahnazian, D and Holroyd, CB (2017) Distributed representations of action sequences in anterior cingulate cortex: a recurrent neural network approach. Psychonomic Bulletin, Review 25, 302321.Google Scholar
Sheehan, DV, Lecrubier, Y, Sheehan, KH, Amorim, P, Janavs, J, Weiller, E, Hergueta, T, Baker, R and Dunbar, GC (1998) The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 59, 2233.Google Scholar
Sheline, YI, Mintun, MA, Barch, DM, Wilkins, C, Snyder, AZ and Moerlein, SM (2004) Decreased hippocampal 5-HT2A receptor binding in older depressed patients using F-18 altanserin positron emission tomography. Neuropsychopharmacology 29, 22352241.Google Scholar
Shiroma, PR, Thuras, P, Johns, B and Lim, KO (2014) Emotion recognition processing as early predictor of response to 8-week citalopram treatment in late-life depression. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 29, 11321139.Google Scholar
Snaith, RP, Hamilton, M, Morley, S, Humayan, A, Hargreaves, D and Trigwell, P (1995) A scale for the assessment of hedonic tone – the Snaith–Hamilton Pleasure Scale. British Journal of Psychiatry 167, 99103.Google Scholar
Spielberger, CD, Gorsuch, RL and Lushene, RE (1970) Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Self-Evaluation Questionnaire 124.Google Scholar
Stoy, M, Schlagenhauf, F, Sterzer, P, Bermpohl, F, Haegele, C, Suchotzki, K, Schmack, K, Wrase, J, Ricken, R, Knutson, B, Adli, M, Bauer, M, Heinz, A and Stroehle, A (2012) Hyporeactivity of ventral striatum towards incentive stimuli in unmedicated depressed patients normalizes after treatment with escitalopram. Journal of Psychopharmacology 26, 677688.Google Scholar
Stringaris, A, Vidal-Ribas Belil, P, Artiges, E, Lemaitre, H, Gollier-Briant, F, Wolke, S, Vulser, H, Miranda, R, Penttila, J, Struve, M, Fadai, T, Kappel, V, Grimmer, Y, Goodman, R, Poustka, L, Conrod, P, Cattrell, A, Banaschewski, T, Bokde, AL, Bromberg, U, Buchel, C, Flor, H, Frouin, V, Gallinat, J, Garavan, H, Gowland, P, Heinz, A, Ittermann, B, Nees, F, Papadopoulos, D, Paus, T, Smolka, MN, Walter, H, Whelan, R, Martinot, JL, Schumann, G and Paillere-Martinot, ML (2015) The brain's response to reward anticipation and depression in adolescence: dimensionality, specificity, and longitudinal predictions in a community-based sample. American Journal of Psychiatry 172, 12151223.Google Scholar
Strober, M, Green, J and Carlson, G (1981) Utility of the Beck depression inventory with psychiatrically hospitalized adolescents. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 49, 482483.Google Scholar
Suhara, T, Nakayama, K, Inoue, O, Fukuda, H, Shimizu, M, Mori, A and Tateno, Y (1992) D1 dopamine receptor-binding in mood disorders measured by positron emission tomography. Psychopharmacology 106, 1418.Google Scholar
Suppes, T, Datto, C, Minkwitz, M, Nordenhem, A, Walker, C and Darko, D (2014) Effectiveness of the extended release formulation of quetiapine as monotherapy for the treatment of acute bipolar depression. Journal of Affective Disorders 168, 485493.Google Scholar
Suppes, T, Kroger, H, Pikalov, A and Loebel, A (2016 a). Lurasidone adjunctive with lithium or valproate for bipolar depression: a placebo-controlled trial utilizing prospective and retrospective enrolment cohorts. Journal of Psychiatric Research 78, 8693.Google Scholar
Suppes, T, Silva, R, Cucchiaro, J, Mao, Y, Targum, S, Streicher, C, Pikalov, A and Loebel, A (2016 b). Lurasidone for the treatment of major depressive disorder with mixed features: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. American Journal of Psychiatry 173, 400407.Google Scholar
Suttajit, S, Srisurapanont, M, Maneeton, N and Maneeton, B (2014) Quetiapine for acute bipolar depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Drug Design Development and Therapy 8, 827838.Google Scholar
Sylvester, CYC, Wager, TD, Lacey, SC, Hernandez, L, Nichols, TE, Smith, EE and Jonides, J (2003) Switching attention and resolving interference: fMRI measures of executive functions. Neuropsychologia 41, 357370.Google Scholar
Tohen, M, Vieta, E, Calabrese, J, Ketter, TA, Sachs, G, Bowden, C, Mitchell, PB, Centorrino, F, Risser, R, Baker, RW, Evans, AR, Beymer, K, Dube, S, Tollefson, GD and Breier, A (2003) Efficacy of olanzapine and olanzapine-fluoxetine combination in the treatment of bipolar I depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 60, 10791088.Google Scholar
Tohen, M, Kanba, S, McIntyre, RS, Fujikoshi, S and Katagiri, H (2014) Efficacy of olanzapine monotherapy in the treatment of bipolar depression with mixed features. Journal of Affective Disorders 164, 5762.Google Scholar
Umemoto, A and Holroyd, CB (2016) Exploring individual differences in task switching: persistence and other personality traits related to anterior cingulate cortex function. Progress in Brain Research 229, 189212.Google Scholar
Vrieze, E, Pizzagalli, DA, Demyttenaere, K, Hompes, T, Sienaert, P, de Boer, P, Schmidt, M and Claes, S (2013) Reduced reward learning predicts outcome in major depressive disorder. Biological Psychiatry 73, 639645.Google Scholar
Walsh, MM and Anderson, JR (2012) Learning from experience: event-related potential correlates of reward processing, neural adaptation, and behavioral choice. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36, 18701884.Google Scholar
Walsh, E, Carl, H, Eisenlohr-Moul, T, Minkel, J, Crowther, A, Moore, T, Gibbs, D, Petty, C, Bizzell, J, Smoski, MJ and Dichter, GS (2016) Attenuation of frontostriatal connectivity during reward processing predicts response to psychotherapy in major depressive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 42, 831843.Google Scholar
Walsh, A, Browning, M, Drevets, W, Furey, M and Harmer, C (2017) Dissociable temporal effects of bupropion on behavioural measures of emotional and reward processing in major depressive disorder. Biological Psychiatry 81, 353354.Google Scholar
Walton, ME, Croxson, PL, Behrens, TEJ, Kennerley, SW and Rushworth, MFS (2007) Adaptive decision making and value in the anterior cingulate cortex. Neuroimage 36, 142154.Google Scholar
Whitaker, A, Johnson, J, Shaffer, D, Rapoport, JL, Kalikow, K, Walsh, BT, Davies, M, Braiman, S and Dolinsky, A (1990) Uncommon troubles in young-people – prevalence estimates of selected psychiatric-disorders in a nonreferred adolescent population. Archives of General Psychiatry 47, 487496.Google Scholar
Wu, CC, Samanez-Larkin, GR, Katovich, K and Knutson, B (2014) Affective traits link to reliable neural markers of incentive anticipation. Neuroimage 84, 279289.Google Scholar
Yatham, LN, Liddle, PF, Dennie, J, Shiah, IS, Adam, MJ, Lane, CJ, Lam, RW and Ruth, TJ (1999) Decrease in brain serotonin 2 receptor binding in patients with major depression following desipramine treatment – a positron emission tomography study with fluorine-18-labeled setoperone. Archives of General Psychiatry 56, 705711.Google Scholar
Yatham, LN, Goldstein, JM, Vieta, E, Bowden, CL, Grunze, H, Post, RM, Suppes, T and Calabrese, JR (2005) Atypical antipsychotics in bipolar depression: potential mechanisms of action. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 66, 4048.Google Scholar
Yeung, N, Holroyd, CB and Cohen, JD (2005) ERP correlates of feedback and reward processing in the presence and absence of response choice. Cerebral Cortex 15, 535544.Google Scholar
Zhang, WN, Chang, SH, Guo, LY, Zhang, KL and Wang, J (2013) The neural correlates of reward-related processing in major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Journal of Affective Disorders 151, 531539.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of participants in a study investigating the effect of lurasidone on reward and penalty processing

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Procedure and timeline for a study investigating the effect of lurasidone on reward and penalty processing.

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Facet plot illustrating ACC response during Penalty Outcome across continuous depression scores under lurasidone and placebo. Dashed vertical line denotes depression severity cut-off score on the BDI-II.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Intra-individual change in penalty related ACC activity (the difference between neural activity under lurasidone and placebo) as a function of continuous depression scores.

Figure 4

Fig. 4. Box plot illustrating ACC Response to Penalty Outcomes (loss > avoided loss). Depression severity cut-off scores from the BDI-II, with individuals with low depressive symptoms (total BDI-II score: 0–16, n = 24) v. high depressive symptoms (total BDI-II score: 17–43, n = 18).

Figure 5

Fig. 5. Increased CBF in bilateral putamen for lurasidone relative to placebo during rest in the whole sample (n = 43). Significant at the peak level whole-brain analyses, family-wise error-corrected (left putamen x = −26, y = −4, z = 2, t = 6.15: p = 0.002, right putamen x = 28, y = −2, z = 2, t = 5.50: p = 0.015). Bar represents T-value.

Supplementary material: File

Wolke et al. supplementary material

Wolke et al. supplementary material 1

Download Wolke et al. supplementary material(File)
File 6 MB