Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T06:01:29.448Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

No relationship between male pubertal timing and depression – new insights from epidemiology and Mendelian randomization

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 March 2024

Raphael Hirtz*
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics, Division of Rare Diseases, and CeSER, Ruhr-University Bochum, Alexandrinenstr. 5, 44791 Bochum, Germany Division of Pediatric Endocrinology and Diabetology, Department of Pediatrics II, University Hospital Essen, University of Duisburg-Essen, Hufelandstr. 55, 40211 Essen, Germany Helios University Medical Centre Wuppertal – Children's Hospital, Witten/Herdecke University, Wuppertal, Germany
Corinna Grasemann
Affiliation:
Division of Pediatric Endocrinology and Diabetology, Department of Pediatrics II, University Hospital Essen, University of Duisburg-Essen, Hufelandstr. 55, 40211 Essen, Germany
Heike Hölling
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology and Health Monitoring, Robert Koch Institute, Berlin, Germany
Björn-Hergen von Holt
Affiliation:
Institut für Medizinische Biometrie und Statistik, Universität zu Lübeck, Universitätsklinikum Schleswig-Holstein, 23562 Lübeck, Germany
Nicola Albers
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Essen, University of Duisburg-Essen, Virchowstr. 174, 45147 Essen, Germany Center for Translational Neuro- and Behavioral Sciences, University Hospital Essen, University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany
Anke Hinney
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Essen, University of Duisburg-Essen, Virchowstr. 174, 45147 Essen, Germany Center for Translational Neuro- and Behavioral Sciences, University Hospital Essen, University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany
Johannes Hebebrand
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Essen, University of Duisburg-Essen, Virchowstr. 174, 45147 Essen, Germany Center for Translational Neuro- and Behavioral Sciences, University Hospital Essen, University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany
Triinu Peters
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Essen, University of Duisburg-Essen, Virchowstr. 174, 45147 Essen, Germany Center for Translational Neuro- and Behavioral Sciences, University Hospital Essen, University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany
*
Corresponding author: Raphael Hirtz; Email: raphael.hirtz@outlook.de
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

In males, the relationship between pubertal timing and depression is understudied and less consistent than in females, likely for reasons of unmeasured confounding. To clarify this relationship, a combined epidemiological and genetic approach was chosen to exploit the methodological advantages of both approaches.

Methods

Data from 2026 males from a nationwide, representative study were used to investigate the non-/linear relationship between pubertal timing defined by the age at voice break and depression, considering a multitude of potential confounders and their interactions with pubertal timing. This analysis was complemented by Mendelian randomization (MR), which is robust to inferential problems inherent to epidemiological studies. We used 71 single nucleotide polymorphisms related to pubertal timing in males as instrumental variable to clarify its causal relationship with depression based on data from 807 553 individuals (246 363 cases and 561 190 controls) by univariable and multivariable MR, including BMI as pleiotropic phenotype.

Results

Univariable MR indicated a causal effect of pubertal timing on depression risk (inverse-variance weighted: OR 0.93, 95%-CI [0.87–0.99)], p = 0.03). However, this was not confirmed by multivariable MR (inverse-variance weighted: OR 0.95, 95%-CI [0.88–1.02)], p = 0.13), consistent with the epidemiological approach (OR 1.01, 95%-CI [0.81–1.26], p = 0.93). Instead, the multivariable MR study indicated a causal relationship of BMI with depression by two of three methods.

Conclusions

Pubertal timing is not related to MDD risk in males.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Major depressive disorder (MDD) is a debilitating psychiatric disorder and is expected to be the leading cause of disease burden worldwide in 2030 by the WHO (Doherty, Egan, & Dinneen, Reference Doherty, Egan and Dinneen2021). Childhood and adolescent psychopathology, including MDD, relates to poor mental health and social functioning in later life (Fergusson, Boden, & Horwood, Reference Fergusson, Boden and Horwood2007), implicating the need to identify children and adolescents at risk for early prevention and intervention (Bevan Jones et al., Reference Bevan Jones, Thapar, Stone, Thapar, Jones, Smith and Simpson2018). While the prevalence of MDD rises during puberty and is higher in adolescent girls than boys (Mojtabai, Olfson, & Han, Reference Mojtabai, Olfson and Han2016), also the 12-month prevalence of MDD in adolescent boys in western countries ranges between 5.4% (Lu, Reference Lu2019) and 5.7% (Mojtabai et al., Reference Mojtabai, Olfson and Han2016) and cumulatively amounts to 13.6% between 12 and 17 years of age (Breslau et al., Reference Breslau, Gilman, Stein, Ruder, Gmelin and Miller2017).

However, the relationship between puberty and MDD is less consistent in boys than in girls (Hamlat, Stange, Abramson, & Alloy, Reference Hamlat, Stange, Abramson and Alloy2014; Wang, Lin, Leung, & Schooling, Reference Wang, Lin, Leung and Schooling2016), likely for reasons of unmeasured confounding (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Lin, Leung and Schooling2016). Recently, Stumper and Alloy (Reference Stumper and Alloy2023) summarized a large number of potential confounders in this relationship. In particular, aspects associated with body weight, including body mass index (BMI) and body image perception, have been identified as confounding factors. Alongside, stressful and adverse life events, parental and social support, and socioeconomic status (SES) have been recognized as confounders (Lorant et al., Reference Lorant, Deliege, Eaton, Robert, Philippot and Ansseau2003; Oelkers et al., Reference Oelkers, Vogel, Kalenda, Surup, Korner, Kratzsch and Kiess2020; Stumper & Alloy, Reference Stumper and Alloy2023). Unlike the thorough analysis of the relationship between puberty and MDD in girls, some studies in boys did not account for confounders (Angold, Costello, & Worthman, Reference Angold, Costello and Worthman1998; Hamlat, McCormick, Young, & Hankin, Reference Hamlat, McCormick, Young and Hankin2020), and many only considered a limited subset.

Physical changes linked to puberty have been thoroughly analyzed, particularly in relation to pubertal timing (Hirtz et al., Reference Hirtz, Libuda, Hinney, Focker, Buhlmeier, Holterhus and Grasemann2022a). Pubertal timing refers to the time of onset of pubertal development or the occurrence of important maturational events, including the age at voice break in males (Hirtz et al., Reference Hirtz, Libuda, Hinney, Focker, Buhlmeier, Holterhus and Grasemann2022a). In women, we could recently show that an earlier age at menarche, a commonly used milestone to define pubertal timing, is related to an increased MDD risk in the general population (Hirtz et al., Reference Hirtz, Hars, Naaresh, Laabs, Antel, Grasemann and Peters2022b) as well as the severity of depressive symptoms in a clinical cohort of adolescent females (Hirtz et al., Reference Hirtz, Libuda, Hinney, Focker, Buhlmeier, Holterhus and Grasemann2022a).

To consider the methodological limitations of epidemiological studies in the analysis of the relationship between male pubertal timing (MPT) and MDD, the present study utilizes Mendelian randomization (MR). As previously discussed (Hirtz et al., Reference Hirtz, Hars, Naaresh, Laabs, Antel, Grasemann and Peters2022b), MR uses genetic markers to draw causal conclusions on the association between an exposure (e.g. MPT) and an outcome (e.g. MDD) of interest by exploiting that genotypes are not generally associated with confounders in the population and are randomly assigned at conception, analogous to randomization in clinical trials. Moreover, since the individual genotype is determined upon conception and cannot be modified by the outcome of interest, MR is robust to reverse causation. Usually, single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) derived from large-scale genome-wide association studies (GWASs) are used as instrumental variable (IV) to study the relationship between exposure and outcome. So-called horizontal pleiotropy poses a risk to the validity of MR results. This phenomenon denotes effects of the IV on the outcome (depression) via pathways other than the exposure (MPT). Previously, BMI has been identified as a potential pathway to horizontal pleiotropy when using MPT as an exposure: SNPs associated with pubertal timing are related to BMI (Busch, Hojgaard, Hagen, & Teilmann, Reference Busch, Hojgaard, Hagen and Teilmann2020; Day et al., Reference Day, Thompson, Helgason, Chasman, Finucane, Sulem and Perry2017), and a higher BMI is associated with an increased risk for depression (Casanova et al., Reference Casanova, O'Loughlin, Martin, Beaumont, Wood, Watkins and Tyrrell2021). Therefore, the analysis of the relationship between MPT and MDD needs to address pleiotropy by multivariable MR incorporating BMI.

However, two-sample MR studies that rely on summary statistics from independent GWAS on the exposure and the outcome (Burgess & Thompson, Reference Burgess and Thompson2021) are often limited by studying the relationship of interest based on the statistical, usually linear model used in the GWAS to define the IV. Thus, it is not possible to study interactions between the exposure and other phenotypes in multivariable MR. Moreover, MR studies are not suitable for estimating effect sizes for specific developmental episodes, as significant findings represent the average effect of an exposure on the outcome over the life course (Burgess & Thompson, Reference Burgess and Thompson2021). Thus, this approach is complemented by a large-scale, representative study of adolescents and young adults, also including information on the above-mentioned confounders in the relationship between pubertal timing and MDD.

Using this methodological approach, combing the methodological advantages of each strategy, the results of the present study may have implications at the population level to guide prevention measures that address the timing of puberty in boys. At the individual level, MPT may be considered part of a prospective risk score to determine the likelihood of MDD if identified as an important contributing factor.

Epidemiological analysis

Participants

As previously described (Hirtz, Holling, & Grasemann, Reference Hirtz, Holling and Grasemann2022c), ‘The German Health Interview and Examination Survey for Children and Adolescents' (KiGGS) is a nationally representative longitudinal study on the health status of children and young people in Germany. Since the baseline study (2003–2006, N = 17,640, age range 0 to 17 years), two follow-ups have been completed: KiGGS wave 1 between 2009 and 2012 (N = 11,992, age range 11 to 24 years) and KiGGS wave 2 between 2014 and 2017 (N = 10,853, age range 18 to 31 years); 3,775 respondents did not take part in any follow-up assessment). Further details on the study design, sampling strategy, and study protocol have been described in detail elsewhere (Mauz et al., Reference Mauz, Lange, Houben, Hoffmann, Allen, Gosswald and Ki2020) and are summarized in the online Supplemental Material 1 (SM1 – Supplementary Methods 1).

The KiGGS study was approved by the ethics committee of the Charité Berlin (baseline and wave 1) and the ethics committee of the Hannover Medical School (wave 2). Written informed consent was obtained from parents as well as from children aged 14 years and older.

For the present study, data from the baseline study and wave 2 were used. Male participants who were queried on MDD history were considered for analysis (N = 2,732). Participants with incomplete information on MDD history, a history of MDD before 9 years of age (i.e. before pubertal onset is expected in boys), precocious or delayed puberty, and missing information on pubertal development or any confounder of interest were excluded (Fig. 1).

Figure 1. Study design flowchart. MDD, major depressive disorder.

Note: Some participants had missing information on more than more variable.

Questionnaires and interviews

Participants aged 11 years and older and parents of minor participants completed self-administered, standardized questionnaires. Among others, the questionnaires addressed physical and mental health as well as their determinants, including information on SES, social support, self-efficacy, and childhood trauma by standardized questionnaires (details on the questionnaires are provided in the online Supplemental Methods 1). Moreover, participants were queried on their subjective body image (much too thin, slightly too thin, exactly the right weight, too obese, or much too obese) and on adverse childhood experiences (own severe disease or accident and death; severe disease or accident of a close person).

Parents of minors and young adults (⩾ 18 years) additionally completed a computer-assisted personal interview (CAPI) conducted by a specially trained study physician (Mauz et al., Reference Mauz, Lange, Houben, Hoffmann, Allen, Gosswald and Ki2020). Beginning with wave 2, the CAPI also covered the participants' mental health history, including the age of onset of mental health conditions (in total years), regarding selected disorders diagnosed by a physician (general practitioner, psychiatrist, neurologist) or psychologist.

Male pubertal timing and physical examination

MPT was defined by age at voice break, considering its high correlation with other maturational pubertal events (Busch et al., Reference Busch, Hollis, Day, Sorensen, Aksglaede, Perry and Hagen2019) and for consistency with the GWAS underlying the MR analysis. MPT was determined at wave 2 by retrospectively asking participants about the age at which their voice started to break (in total years) (Mauz et al., Reference Mauz, Lange, Houben, Hoffmann, Allen, Gosswald and Ki2020). Based on this information, the total sample of male participants (N = 3,747) was used to define precocious (mean age at voice break ⩽ −2.5 SDS [0.4th percentile]) and delayed puberty (⩾ +2.5 SDS [99.6th percentile]) (Pathomvanich, Merke, & Chrousos, Reference Pathomvanich, Merke and Chrousos2000).

Further details regarding anthropometric measures in the psychiatric sample and KiGGS participants are outlined in the online SM1 – Supplementary Methods 2.

Statistical analysis – significance, effect size, and power

The results regarding the relationship between MPT and MDD were assessed by two-tailed testing and considered significant at p < 0.05. The results regarding the analysis of covariates, including those investigating interactions with MPT, were FDR-corrected for multiple comparisons at q < 0.05. The analysis of sociodemographic data and the pattern of missingness, including results from multiple imputation (see online SM1 – Supplementary Methods 3), were considered exploratory and not corrected for multiple testing.

The effect size of significant findings is reported as Cohen's d (d [small 0.31 ⩽ d ⩽ 0.49, medium 0.50 ⩽ d ⩽ 0.79, large ⩾ 0.8]). Post hoc power analyses were conducted with GPower 3.1.9.7 (Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, Reference Faul, Erdfelder, Lang and Buchner2007), assuming a prevalence of MDD of 5%, α = 0.05, borderline small effect size (d = 0.2) per change in 1 s.d. of the predictor, and R 2 of the predictor with other variables = 0.04.

Logistic regression analysis

Two logistic regression models assessed the relationship between MPT (age at voice break in total years – independent variable) and the risk for MDD (dependent variable). The first model was unadjusted, the second model accounted for information on confounders related to MPT and MDD outlined in the introduction (BMI, SES, subjective body image, social support, self-efficacy, CTQ subscales emotional and physical abuse, and the sum score of childhood adverse events; details on the assessment of confounders are provided in online SM1 – Supplementary Methods 4) recorded at wave 2 (for details on testing the assumptions of logistic regression, see online SM1 – Supplementary Methods 5).

Confounders – moderation and mediation analysis

Since confounders in the relationship between pubertal timing and MDD have the potential to act as moderators and/or mediators (Stumper & Alloy, Reference Stumper and Alloy2023), a third logistic regression model was specified. This model, otherwise identical to model 2, additionally considered interaction terms between MPT and the above outlined confounders.

In case of a significant relationship between MPT and MDD demonstrated by either of the three models, a subsequent mediation analysis was intended to determine whether the observed relationship was driven by one the aforementioned confounders.

Mendelian randomization

Univariate MR analysis

In an MR study, three main assumptions must be met (Haycock et al., Reference Haycock, Burgess, Wade, Bowden, Relton and Davey Smith2016): (1) the genetic instrument must have a strong association with the exposure, (2) the genetic instrument is independent of potential confounders in the relationship between the exposure and the outcome, and (3) the outcome is associated with the genetic instrument only through the effect of the exposure (Haycock et al., Reference Haycock, Burgess, Wade, Bowden, Relton and Davey Smith2016).

The first assumption can be tested by the F statistic, calculated as (Beta/s.e.)2 for each SNP defining the IV for MPT. The F statistic for the total IV including all SNPs was calculated considering the proportion of the explained variance by the IV (h 2SNP) and the sample size of the outcome (Burgess & Thompson, Reference Burgess and Thompson2017). An F > 10 indicates that no bias due to a weak IV is present (Haycock et al., Reference Haycock, Burgess, Wade, Bowden, Relton and Davey Smith2016). The second assumption is unlikely to be violated in the MR context (Greco, Minelli, Sheehan, & Thompson, Reference Greco, Minelli, Sheehan and Thompson2015), as genetic variants are fixed at conception and cannot be influenced by confounding factors of the risk factor-outcome associations (Haycock et al., Reference Haycock, Burgess, Wade, Bowden, Relton and Davey Smith2016).

To test the validity of the third assumption, multiple methods were employed. First, we calculated Cochran's Q-statistic to test for IV heterogeneity, which is indicated by a significant finding (p < 0.05). Heterogeneity can have several causes, of which horizontal pleiotropy is the most likely (Greco et al., Reference Greco, Minelli, Sheehan and Thompson2015). To specifically address directional unbalanced pleiotropy, we performed MR‒Egger regression and MR-PRESSO (Mendelian Randomization Pleiotropy RESidual Sum and Outlier) analysis (Verbanck, Chen, Neale, & Do, Reference Verbanck, Chen, Neale and Do2018). Importantly, a previous simulation study showed that MR-PRESSO is more sensitive to horizontal pleiotropy than Egger's intercept (Verbanck et al., Reference Verbanck, Chen, Neale and Do2018).

Considering it unlikely that all IVs in MR fulfill the above outlined assumptions, several robust MR methods have been developed that differ in their robustness to violations of the MR assumptions (for details on the different methods, see the online SM1 – Supplementary Methods 6). Since no single method provides infallible proof of causality, using different methods has been recommended (Burgess et al., Reference Burgess, Davey Smith, Davies, Dudbridge, Gill, Glymour and Theodoratou2019) and is implemented in the present study. If Cochran's Q statistic suggested heterogeneity of the IV, the contamination mixture method and the MR-Lasso method were applied in addition to the penalized weighted median method.

For visualization of the results, Forest, scatter, funnel, and leave-one-out plots were created (see online SM1 – Supplementary Methods 6 and Figs S2–S4).

Power analysis to estimate the probability of finding a true effect was implemented using the sample size of outcome GWAS, the proportion of cases in the study, and the proportion of variance explained (h 2SNP) by SNPs in the IV. Because SNP heritability was not reported by Hollis et al. (Reference Hollis, Day, Busch, Thompson, Soares, Timmers and Perry2020) for MPT, we estimated the explained variance in MPT by the 71 SNPs constituting our IV using the formula by Shim et al. (Reference Shim, Chasman, Smith, Mora, Ridker, Nickerson and Stephens2015).

Multivariable MR analysis – assessing the effect of BMI and other causes of pleiotropy

To consider BMI as a confounder, we performed multivariable MR with four different methods (for details on the different methods, see the online SM1 – Supplementary Methods 6), including multivariable MR-PRESSO. If there was no evidence of pleiotropy by multivariable MR-PRESSO, this implied that no further confounders needed consideration. Uncorrected effect sizes from MR-PRESSO correspond to those from the IVW method and will therefore not be reported. We further calculated the Q a-statistic to test for heterogeneity. Consistent with the univariate approach, a p value < 0.05 indicates heterogeneity in the multivariable MR model (Sanderson, Spiller, & Bowden, Reference Sanderson, Spiller and Bowden2020). As individual genetic data were not available, we could not consider correlations between the exposure variables.

Male pubertal timing

To construct the instrument variable, we used the genome-wide significant independent SNPs (n = 76) from the GWAS by Hollis et al. (Reference Hollis, Day, Busch, Thompson, Soares, Timmers and Perry2020). An effective GWAS meta-analysis sample size of 205,354 men of European ancestry was achieved by a multi-trait analysis of GWAS (MTAG) for continuous age at voice by data obtained from the UK Biobank and 23andMe study. MPT was constructed from the questions ‘When did your voice break?’ and ‘When did you start to grow facial hair’ (younger than average, about average, older than average, do not know, prefer not to answer). In the 23andMe study, the ‘age at voice breaking’ phenotype was determined by response to the question ‘How old were you when your voice began to crack/deepen?’. Participants could choose from seven predefined age bins, which were rescaled to one-year age bins (for further details on the GWAS, see the online SM1 – Supplementary Methods 7).

Major depressive disorder

The data source for the outcome variable MDD was the recent GWAS by Howard et al. (Reference Howard, Adams, Clarke, Hafferty, Gibson, Shirali and McIntosh2019) including 807,553 individuals (246, 363 cases and 561,190 controls). This meta-analysis was based on data from the three largest GWASs of depression. These studies used different measures for depression: (a) self-reported clinical diagnosis of MDD by Hyde et al. (Reference Hyde, Nagle, Tian, Chen, Paciga, Wendland and Winslow2016); (b) MDD obtained from a structured clinical interview or based on broader criteria by Wray et al. (Reference Wray, Ripke, Mattheisen, Trzaskowski, Byrne and Abdellaoui2018); (c) self-reported help-seeking for problems with nerves, anxiety, tension or depression (broad depression) by Howard et al. (Reference Howard, Adams, Clarke, Hafferty, Gibson, Shirali and McIntosh2019). Overlapping samples were excluded. The meta-analysis identified 102 independent genome-wide significant variants. The genome-wide SNP-based heritability (h 2SNP) was 8.9%. The proportion of females was 48% in Hyde et al. (Reference Hyde, Nagle, Tian, Chen, Paciga, Wendland and Winslow2016) and 54% in Howard et al. (Reference Howard, Adams, Clarke, Hafferty, Gibson, Shirali and McIntosh2019). The proportion of males and females was not reported by Wray et al. (Reference Wray, Ripke, Mattheisen, Trzaskowski, Byrne and Abdellaoui2018) (for further details on the GWAS, see the online SM1 – Supplementary Methods 6).

BMI

We used the most recent GWAS on BMI with sex-specific analyses (Pulit et al., Reference Pulit, Stoneman, Morris, Wood, Glastonbury, Tyrrell and Lindgren2019). A total of 806 ,834 individuals of European ancestry were included in the analysis, 374,756 of whom were males. The SNP-based heritability (h 2SNP) for males was 35% for all SNPs (Pulit et al., Reference Pulit, Stoneman, Morris, Wood, Glastonbury, Tyrrell and Lindgren2019).

Reporting and software

In reporting our studies, we followed the STROBE and STROBE-MR recommendations (Skrivankova et al., Reference Skrivankova, Richmond, Woolf, Yarmolinsky, Davies, Swanson and Richards2021). Data handling and analyses were either performed with SPSS 28.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY) and its Complex Samples® procedures to account for the sample design, design-related effects, and attrition related to sociodemographic characteristics (Mauz et al., Reference Mauz, Lange, Houben, Hoffmann, Allen, Gosswald and Ki2020) or the software ‘R’ (3.5.1 and 4.1.1) with the R-packages ‘TwoSampleMR’ (0.4.26, https://github.com/MRCIEU/TwoSampleMR) (Hemani et al., Reference Hemani, Zheng, Elsworth, Wade, Haberland, Baird and Haycock2018), ‘Mendelian Randomization’ (0.5.1; https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=MendelianRandomization) (Broadbent et al., Reference Broadbent, Foley, Grant, Mason, Staley and Burgess2020), ‘MVMR’ (0.3; https://github.com/WSpiller/MVMR) (Sanderson et al., Reference Sanderson, Spiller and Bowden2020), and ‘MRPracticals’ (0.0.1; https://github.com/WSpiller/MRPracticals). MR-PRESSO was performed with R-package (1.0; https://github.com/rondolab/MR-PRESSO) (Verbanck et al., Reference Verbanck, Chen, Neale and Do2018).

Results

Epidemiological study – results

Of the 17,640 children and adolescents who took part in the baseline assessment of the KiGGS study, 2,732 were male and were queried on MDD history at wave 2. Of these, 2,026 participants were considered for further analyses regarding the cross-sectional analysis, as 706 were excluded for reasons outlined in Fig. 1.

Missingness

Only regarding the history of MDD (unweighted: 3.1%, weighted 5.2%), MPT (weighted 13.7%, unweighted: 10.6%), and adverse life events (weighted 9.9%, unweighted: 9.0%) was there a significant (weighted) proportion (> 5%) of participants with missing information. Further analyses showed a random pattern of missingness.

Excluded participants were found to have a lower SES (p < 0.001), lower levels of social support (p = 0.004), lower self-efficacy (p = 0.02), and a history of more emotional childhood trauma (p = 0.04). However, absolute differences were minor (online SM1 – Supplementary Table S1), and effect sizes were either borderline small (SES: d = 0.22) or negligible (d < 0.20).

Descriptives

The mean age of participants at wave 2 was 23.93 (s.d. 3.35) years, and the mean age at voice break was 13.9 (s.d. 4.5) years (Table 1, Fig. 2A). The mean difference between the age at wave 2 and the age at voice break was 9.6 (s.d. 3.7) years. Considering a significant (p = 0.01) but trivial effect (d = 0.14) regarding the relationship between age at wave 2 and the reported age at voice break, no recall bias was evident.

Table 1. Descriptives

MDD, major depressive disorder; SES, socioeconomic status; CTQ, childhood trauma questionnaire; ISCED, International Standard Classification of Education (1: primary education; 2: lower secondary education; 3: upper secondary education; 4: post-secondary non-tertiary education; 5: short-cycle tertiary education; 6: Bachelor's or equivalent level; 7: Master's or equivalent level; 8: Doctoral's or equivalent level).

Mean, standard deviation (in round brackets), and range (in square brackets) for interval scaled variables, absolute numbers and percentages otherwise.

Note: ISCED level (N missing = 45) and migration background (N missing = 3) were not part of the analyses but included for a more complete picture of demographic characteristics. Percentages are adjusted for the sampling plan, standard deviations are based on normalized weights. a = BMI is based on self-reported height and weight.

Figure 2. Cumulative probability of voice break (Panel A) and major depressive disorder (MDD, Panel B) dependent on age.

Over a median follow-up period of 11 years (IQR: 10–14), 112 cases of MDD were reported, of which only 25.9% were diagnosed before the age of 18 (Fig. 2B).

Logistic regression results

Neither in the unadjusted (b = −0.05, t = −0.46, OR 0.95, 95%-CI [0.76–1.19], p = 0.65) nor the adjusted (b = 0.01, t = 0.06, OR 1.01, 95%-CI [0.81–1.26], p = 0.93) model considering important confounders in the relationship between MPT and MDD risk was there a significant finding in this regard (online Supplementary Material 2 (SM2) – Supplementary Table S2 for detailed results). This did not change when additionally considering interactions between MPT and confounders (b = 1.12, t = 1.08, OR 3.07, 95%-CI [0.40–23.74], p = 0.28). Power (1-β) to detect a borderline small effect size (d ⩾ 0.2) was 0.95. These findings were confirmed by the pooled analysis of imputed datasets (online SM2 – Supplementary Table 3). Since there was no evidence of a relationship between MPT and MDD, no mediation analysis was performed.

There was no evidence of a curvilinear relationship between MPT and MDD by the Box-Tidwell approach in either model.

Mendelian randomization – results

For the univariable MR on the effect of MPT on MDD risk, 71 SNPs could be considered for the IV (online SM2 – Supplementary Table S4). The F statistic for the IV was 16 685.2. The F statistic for each SNP individually indicated that all SNPs were sufficiently strong instruments (lowest F-value = 29.9, online SM2 – Supplementary Table S5).

Cochran's Q test indicated heterogeneity of the IV (MR-Egger – Q(df = 69) = 186.4, p = 9.5 × 10−13; IVW – Q(df = 70) = 187.0, p = 1.3 × 10−12). Consistently, MR-PRESSO indicated pleiotropy even though Egger's intercept did not ( = −0.001, s.e. = 0.003, p = 0.66), likely for the reasons outlined above.

Univariable MR showed mixed results: IVW and MR-Egger with bootstrapping showed a significant causal effect of MBT on MDD. The methods robust to pleiotropy (MR RAPS, outlier corrected MR-PRESSO) and two of three methods robust to heterogeneity (contamination mixture method, MR Lasso) also showed a significant effect (Fig. 3, online SM2 – Supplementary Table S6 and Supplementary Figs S2–S4). However, the multivariable MR of the effect of MPT on MDD risk (n = 71; online SM2 – Supplementary Table 7), adjusted for BMI, showed that the effect observed in the univariable MR could be due to BMI-related pleiotropy (online SM1 – Supplementary Table S8 and Fig. S4), as none of the three multivariable methods showed an effect of MPT on MDD risk. Multivariate MR-PRESSO did not identify pleiotropic outliers. The conditional F test showed that the IV was sufficiently strong for both MPT (F TS = 39.3) and BMI (F TS = 27.0). The Q a statistic indicated heterogeneity of the IV (Q[df = 68] = 180.6; p = 3.7 × 10−12).

Figure 3. Results of the multi-SNP univariate Mendelian randomization (MR) analyses regarding the effect of male pubertal timing (MPT) [Hollis et al. (Reference Hollis, Day, Busch, Thompson, Soares, Timmers and Perry2020)] on MDD risk [Howard et al. (Reference Howard, Adams, Clarke, Hafferty, Gibson, Shirali and McIntosh2019)]. OR = odds ratio, CI, confidence interval; b, unstandardized causal estimate of the change in risk for depression per one-year change in puberty time.

Power analysis

The h 2SNP for the 71 SNPs associated with MPT was 2.1% (online Supplementary Table S4). Our analysis had a power of 80% to detect an OR of 0.95 or 1.05 for MDD per one-year change in MPT and 100% power to detect an OR of 0.92 or 1.08 (online SM1 – Supplementary Fig. S1).

Discussion

Previous studies have provided inconsistent findings regarding the association of MPT with depressive symptoms and MDD (Hamlat et al., Reference Hamlat, Stange, Abramson and Alloy2014), likely for methodological reasons. However, when investigating this relationship with a large epidemiological study considering multiple potential confounders and within the MR framework robust to residual confounding and reverse causation to consider the methodological limitations of each approach, there was no evidence of a (causal) relationship in this regard in the present work (Fig. 4).

Figure 4. Results of the multivariable MR (MVMR) analyses of the causal effect of male pubertal timing (MPT) [Hollis et al. (Reference Hollis, Day, Busch, Thompson, Soares, Timmers and Perry2020)] on MDD risk [Howard et al. (Reference Howard, Adams, Clarke, Hafferty, Gibson, Shirali and McIntosh2019)] adjusted for BMI [Pulit et al. (Reference Pulit, Stoneman, Morris, Wood, Glastonbury, Tyrrell and Lindgren2019)] calculated using three different methods. OR, odds ratio; CI, confidence interval; b, unstandardized causal estimate of the change in risk for depression per one-year change in MPT.

Male pubertal timing and MDD

The relationship between MPT and depression has mainly been studied dimensionally, that is, regarding the severity of depressive symptoms on a continuous scale. These studies have provided mixed evidence (Hamlat et al., Reference Hamlat, Stange, Abramson and Alloy2014), reporting a relationship between depressive symptoms and either earlier or later pubertal timing, off-timing effects (i.e. effects of earlier and later pubertal timing), or no relationship. These inconsistencies also apply to those few studies addressing this relationship categorically, that is, the presence (or absence) of MDD according to clinical criteria, as investigated in the present study. While confounders were not considered in either study, Angold et al. (Reference Angold, Costello and Worthman1998) found no evidence of a relationship between pubertal timing in girls and boys, but Hamlat et al. (Reference Hamlat, McCormick, Young and Hankin2020) identified pubertal timing as a risk factor for incident and recurrent MDD in both sexes. However, considering the present study's findings, the latter study was likely affected by residual confounding. Moreover, this study may have also been insufficiently powered to conclusively investigate the impact of pubertal timing on MDD risk in boys, as discussed by the authors. In contrast, the present epidemiological analyses and the MR study were sufficiently powered for sound conclusions.

However, for the conclusions of the MR study to hold, the genetic architecture of depression should not change throughout the life course. As previously discussed in more detail (Hirtz et al., Reference Hirtz, Hars, Naaresh, Laabs, Antel, Grasemann and Peters2022b), a recent genome-wide association meta-analysis showed that the genetic architecture of internalizing symptoms is stable from early childhood to adolescence. This was concluded from the observation of overlapping SNP-based heritability estimates during earlier and later life and a high genetic correlation between childhood internalizing symptoms and adult depression (rg > 0.7) (Jami et al., Reference Jami, Hammerschlag, Ip, Allegrini, Benyamin, Border and Middeldorp2022).

Closely related but yet distinct – pubertal timing in girls and boys

The genetic SNP-based underpinnings of pubertal timing in females and males largely overlap (Hollis et al., Reference Hollis, Day, Busch, Thompson, Soares, Timmers and Perry2020). Of the 76 SNPs related to MPT, 71 show a similar effect estimate of the same direction observed for age at menarche. This could imply that pubertal timing in both sexes shares the same underlying mechanisms to implicate sexual maturation in mental health. In females, downstream mechanisms to age at menarche have been suggested to explain its effect on MDD (Magnus et al., Reference Magnus, Guyatt, Lawn, Wyss, Trajanoska, Kupers and Fraser2020), especially as there is no conclusive evidence that upstream mechanisms, including sex steroid hormones, are important in this regard (Hirtz et al., Reference Hirtz, Hars, Naaresh, Laabs, Antel, Grasemann and Peters2022b). Downstream mechanisms discussed include, for example, younger age at first delivery, an increased risk of childhood sexual abuse, and younger age at first sexual intercourse (Magnus et al., Reference Magnus, Guyatt, Lawn, Wyss, Trajanoska, Kupers and Fraser2020). In particular, the latter is consistent with risky sexual behavior in adolescent boys with a small to medium effect size in a recent meta-analysis (Ullsperger & Nikolas, Reference Ullsperger and Nikolas2017) and a significant genetic correlation between MPT and health risk behaviors, including alcohol consumption and smoking (Hollis et al., Reference Hollis, Day, Busch, Thompson, Soares, Timmers and Perry2020). However, at the same time, early MPT is associated with favorable social traits, including educational attainment (Hollis et al., Reference Hollis, Day, Busch, Thompson, Soares, Timmers and Perry2020), which does not seem to apply to girls (Torvik et al., Reference Torvik, Flato, McAdams, Colman, Silventoinen and Stoltenberg2021). Moreover, physical changes related to pubertal maturation in boys are often considered desirable and convey social value, in contrast to adolescent girls (Rudolph & Troop-Gordon, Reference Rudolph and Troop-Gordon2010). Thus, adverse outcomes regarding mental health conferred by the genotype underlying pubertal timing might be counterbalanced by advantageous effects related to the same set of genes and their individual allelic architecture in males but not females. This explains the findings of the present study as well as the observation of adverse effects of earlier pubertal timing in girls on the mental health phenotype (Hirtz et al., Reference Hirtz, Hars, Naaresh, Laabs, Antel, Grasemann and Peters2022b).

However, the present study also provides insights into the underlying mechanisms relating pubertal timing to the etiology of MDD in boys. From a genetic perspective, BMI drives the apparent relationship between MPT and MDD due to an overlapping genetic origin of both exposure phenotypes. This is indicated by the multivariable MR analysis and the observation of no cause of horizontal pleiotropy other than via BMI. In this regard, BMI has not only been related to pubertal timing by epidemiological and MR studies (Busch et al., Reference Busch, Hojgaard, Hagen and Teilmann2020) but has also been implicated in MDD (Casanova et al., Reference Casanova, O'Loughlin, Martin, Beaumont, Wood, Watkins and Tyrrell2021). BMI may exert its effects on MDD risk via several downstream outcomes, including, for example, hormone levels (Milano et al., Reference Milano, Ambrosio, Carizzone, De Biasio, Di Munzio, Foia and Capasso2020), peer relations (Kanders, Nilsson, & Åslund, Reference Kanders, Nilsson and Åslund2021), and body image (Richard, Rohrmann, Lohse, & Eichholzer, Reference Richard, Rohrmann, Lohse and Eichholzer2016). In contrast, in the epidemiological analyses in the present study, neither BMI nor body image was related to MDD risk. However, since MR studies assess the average effect of an exposure over the life course, the direct and indirect effects of BMI may no longer be apparent or have yet to manifest in an epidemiological context.

Limitations – epidemiological study

The age at voice break was self-reported in the epidemiological (and genetic) study. However, there was no evidence of a substantial recall bias, as far as can be inferred from our analyses, and the follow-up period in the present study was notably shorter than in previous studies reporting such a bias concerning pubertal timing (Must et al., Reference Must, Phillips, Naumova, Blum, Harris, Dawson-Hughes and Rand2002). Moreover, self-assessed age at voice break has been shown to correlate closely with other important pubertal events, justifying its use in epidemiological studies (Busch et al., Reference Busch, Hollis, Day, Sorensen, Aksglaede, Perry and Hagen2019).

In neither the epidemiological analyses nor the MR study, we could consider subclinical depressive symptoms. As a consequence, our conclusions are limited to overt depression. Moreover, our study was limited to the analysis of the relationship between pubertal timing but not pubertal status and MDD. However, regarding subclinical depressive symptoms and pubertal status (Stumper & Alloy, Reference Stumper and Alloy2023), previous studies have provided conflicting findings, which are suggestive of residual confounding, such as the analysis of pubertal timing. Thus, once data are available, it would be tempting to test for this by an MR study.

Moreover, confounders other than those included in the epidemiological analyses have been suggested but were not captured at either KiGGS assessment. However, issues of residual confounding are sufficiently covered by the MR analyses, confirming the results of the epidemiological studies. Also, neither migration nor social status is related to MPT in the KiGGS sample, rendering them unlikely confounders (Kahl, Schaffrath Rosario, & Schlaud, Reference Kahl, Schaffrath Rosario and Schlaud2007).

Limitations – MR study

By the statistical model of the GWAS by Hollis et al. (Reference Hollis, Day, Busch, Thompson, Soares, Timmers and Perry2020) underlying the MR analysis, we were limited to studying a linear relationship between MPT and MDD. However, there was no evidence of a relationship other than linear by the epidemiological study.

The analysis of the effect of BMI on MPT in the multivariable MR model relied on the GWAS by Pulit et al. (Reference Pulit, Stoneman, Morris, Wood, Glastonbury, Tyrrell and Lindgren2019). While this GWAS allowed us to study sex-specific BMI-related findings pertaining only to males, it was conducted in adults. This implies a continuity of the genetic architecture of BMI over the life course regarding the present study, which is supported by a recent meta-analysis of 26 GWASs on childhood BMI. This study included data from 61 ,111 children and identified 25 childhood-specific BMI loci (Vogelezang et al., Reference Vogelezang, Bradfield, Ahluwalia, Curtin, Lakka, Grarup and Felix2020). Most of these SNPs (20 of 25) were also related to adult BMI, and there was a genetic correlation between childhood and adult BMI of rg = 0.76.

The genetic instrument for depression was not sex specific, as no such information is available. However, as previously discussed (Hirtz et al., Reference Hirtz, Hars, Naaresh, Laabs, Antel, Grasemann and Peters2022b), a recent study that evaluated between-sex genetic heterogeneity in MDD using GWAS summary statistics from 29 cohorts found a genetic correlation close to one (Trzaskowski et al., Reference Trzaskowski, Mehta, Peyrot, Hawkes, Davies and Howard2019).

Moreover, our results apply to the Caucasian population only and likely do not generalize to patients with precocious puberty, even though SNPs in genes related to precocious puberty were used as IV as well (Hollis et al., Reference Hollis, Day, Busch, Thompson, Soares, Timmers and Perry2020).

Adjusting for BMI could lead to an over-adjustment if BMI was a mediator. Thus, our approach provides a conservative estimate of the effect of pubertal timing on depression, recognizing that it might not capture the full direct effect. However, considering the KiGGS results which do not suggest a mediating effect of BMI on the relationship between MPT and MDD, our approach seems justified and highlights the advantage of the combined epidemiological and MR analysis.

Further minor methodological limitations are discussed in the online SM1 – Supplementary Discussion.

Conclusions and implications

In contrast to females, our study shows that earlier MPT is not related to an increased MDD risk. This is suggested by the epidemiological and genetic analyses, combining methodological advantages of both approaches, which allows for sound conclusions.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291724000060

Data availability

The reported results related to the KiGGS study are based on a secondary analysis of data provided by the Robert Koch Institute (RKI), Germany. Requests to access the datasets should, therefore, be directed to the RKI ().

Publicly available datasets analyzed in this study can be found here: https://www.ebi.ac.uk/gwas/. Original data generated and analyzed during this study (i.e. harmonized genetic data) can be found here: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.19518982.v4.

Acknowledgements

The authors also acknowledge support from the Open Access Publication Fund of the University of Duisburg-Essen. The funders had no role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or manuscript preparation.

Authors’ contributions

RH and TP conceptualized the present study, analyzed and interpreted the data, and wrote the manuscript. All authors participated in critically revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content. All authors approved the final version to be published.

Competing interest

The authors report no financial or other relationship relevant to the subject of this article.

References

Angold, A., Costello, E. J., & Worthman, C. M. (1998). Puberty and depression: The roles of age, pubertal status and pubertal timing. Psychological Medicine, 28(1), 5161. doi: 10.1017/s003329179700593xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bevan Jones, R., Thapar, A., Stone, Z., Thapar, A., Jones, I., Smith, D., & Simpson, S. (2018). Psychoeducational interventions in adolescent depression: A systematic review. Patient Education and Counseling, 101(5), 804816. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2017.10.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breslau, J., Gilman, S. E., Stein, B. D., Ruder, T., Gmelin, T., & Miller, E. (2017). Sex differences in recent first-onset depression in an epidemiological sample of adolescents. Translational Psychiatry, 7(5), e1139. doi: 10.1038/tp.2017.105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broadbent, J. R., Foley, C. N., Grant, A. J., Mason, A. M., Staley, J. R., & Burgess, S. (2020). MendelianRandomization v0.5.0: Updates to an R package for performing Mendelian randomization analyses using summarized data. Wellcome Open Research, 5, 252. doi: 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.16374.2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burgess, S., Davey Smith, G., Davies, N. M., Dudbridge, F., Gill, D., Glymour, M. M., … Theodoratou, E. (2019). Guidelines for performing Mendelian randomization investigations: Update for summer 2023. Wellcome Open Research, 4, 186. doi: 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.15555.3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burgess, S., & Thompson, S. G. (2017). Interpreting findings from Mendelian randomization using the MR-Egger method. European Journal of Epidemiology, 32(5), 377389. doi: 10.1007/s10654-017-0255-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burgess, S., & Thompson, S. G. (2021). Mendelian randomization: Methods for causal inference using genetic variants. New York: CRC Press.10.1201/9780429324352CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Busch, A. S., Hojgaard, B., Hagen, C. P., & Teilmann, G. (2020). Obesity is associated with earlier pubertal onset in boys. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 105(4), 16671672. 10.1210/clinem/dgz22210.1210/clinem/dgz222CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Busch, A. S., Hollis, B., Day, F. R., Sorensen, K., Aksglaede, L., Perry, J. R. B., … Hagen, C. P. (2019). Voice break in boys-temporal relations with other pubertal milestones and likely causal effects of BMI. Human Reproduction, 34(8), 15141522. doi: 10.1093/humrep/dez118CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casanova, F., O'Loughlin, J., Martin, S., Beaumont, R. N., Wood, A. R., Watkins, E. R., … Tyrrell, J. (2021). Higher adiposity and mental health: Causal inference using Mendelian randomization. Human Molecular Genetics, 30(24), 23712382. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddab204CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Day, F. R., Thompson, D. J., Helgason, H., Chasman, D. I., Finucane, H., Sulem, P., … Perry, J. R. B. (2017). Genomic analyses identify hundreds of variants associated with age at menarche and support a role for puberty timing in cancer risk. Nature Genetics, 49(6), 834841. doi: 10.1038/ng.3841CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doherty, A. M., Egan, A. M., & Dinneen, S. (2021). Mental health, diabetes and endocrinology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175191. doi: 10.3758/bf03193146CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fergusson, D. M., Boden, J. M., & Horwood, L. J. (2007). Recurrence of major depression in adolescence and early adulthood, and later mental health, educational and economic outcomes. British Journal of Psychiatry, 191(4), 335342. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.036079CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greco, M. F., Minelli, C., Sheehan, N. A., & Thompson, J. R. (2015). Detecting pleiotropy in Mendelian randomisation studies with summary data and a continuous outcome. Statistics in Medicine, 34(21), 29262940. doi: 10.1002/sim.6522CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamlat, E. J., McCormick, K. C., Young, J. F., & Hankin, B. L. (2020). Early pubertal timing predicts onset and recurrence of depressive episodes in boys and girls. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 61(11), 12661274. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13198CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamlat, E. J., Stange, J. P., Abramson, L. Y., & Alloy, L. B. (2014). Early pubertal timing as a vulnerability to depression symptoms: Differential effects of race and sex. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42(4), 527538. doi: 10.1007/s10802-013-9798-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haycock, P. C., Burgess, S., Wade, K. H., Bowden, J., Relton, C., & Davey Smith, G. (2016). Best (but oft-forgotten) practices: The design, analysis, and interpretation of Mendelian randomization studies. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 103(4), 965978. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.115.118216CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hemani, G., Zheng, J., Elsworth, B., Wade, K. H., Haberland, V., Baird, D., … Haycock, P. C. (2018). The MR-base platform supports systematic causal inference across the human phenome. Elife, 7, e34408. doi: 10.7554/eLife.34408CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirtz, R., Libuda, L., Hinney, A., Focker, M., Buhlmeier, J., Holterhus, P. M., … Grasemann, C. (2022a). Age at menarche relates to depression in adolescent girls: Comparing a clinical sample to the general pediatric population. Journal of Affective Disorders, 318, 103112. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2022.08.092CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirtz, R., Hars, C., Naaresh, R., Laabs, B. H., Antel, J., Grasemann, C., … Peters, T. (2022b). Causal effect of age at menarche on the risk for depression: Results from a Two-sample multivariable Mendelian randomization study. Frontiers in Genetics, 13, 918584. doi: 10.3389/fgene.2022.918584CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirtz, R., Holling, H., & Grasemann, C. (2022c). Subclinical hypothyroidism and incident depression in adolescents and young adults: Results from a nationwide representative prospective study. Thyroid: Official Journal of the American Thyroid Association, 32(10), 11691177. doi: 10.1089/thy.2022.0205Google Scholar
Hollis, B., Day, F. R., Busch, A. S., Thompson, D. J., Soares, A. L. G., Timmers, P., … Perry, J. R. B. (2020). Genomic analysis of male puberty timing highlights shared genetic basis with hair colour and lifespan. Nature Communications, 11(1), 1536. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-14451-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howard, D. M., Adams, M. J., Clarke, T. K., Hafferty, J. D., Gibson, J., Shirali, M., … McIntosh, A. M. (2019). Genome-wide meta-analysis of depression identifies 102 independent variants and highlights the importance of the prefrontal brain regions. Nature Neuroscience, 22(3), 343352. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0326-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyde, C. L., Nagle, M. W., Tian, C., Chen, X., Paciga, S. A., Wendland, J. R., … Winslow, A. R. (2016). Identification of 15 genetic loci associated with risk of major depression in individuals of European descent. Nature Genetics, 48(9), 10311036. doi: 10.1038/ng.3623CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jami, E. S., Hammerschlag, A. R., Ip, H. F., Allegrini, A. G., Benyamin, B., Border, R., … Middeldorp, C. M. (2022). Genome-wide association meta-analysis of childhood and adolescent internalizing symptoms. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 61(7), 934945. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2021.11.035CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahl, H., Schaffrath Rosario, A., & Schlaud, M. (2007). [Sexual maturation of children and adolescents in Germany. Results of the German Health Interview and Examination Survey for Children and Adolescents (KiGGS)]. Bundesgesundheitsblatt, Gesundheitsforschung, Gesundheitsschutz, 50(5–6), 677685. doi: 10.1007/s00103-007-0229-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanders, S. H., Nilsson, K. W., & Åslund, C. (2021). Body mass index and bullying victimization as antecedents for depressive symptoms in a Swedish youth cohort. Journal of Public Health, 30(10), 25132524. doi: 10.1007/s10389-020-01460-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorant, V., Deliege, D., Eaton, W., Robert, A., Philippot, P., & Ansseau, M. (2003). Socioeconomic inequalities in depression: A meta-analysis. American Journal of Epidemiology, 157(2), 98112. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwf182CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lu, W. (2019). Adolescent depression: National trends, risk factors, and healthcare disparities. American Journal of Health Behavior, 43(1), 181194. doi: 10.5993/AJHB.43.1.15CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magnus, M. C., Guyatt, A. L., Lawn, R. B., Wyss, A. B., Trajanoska, K., Kupers, L. K., … Fraser, A. (2020). Identifying potential causal effects of age at menarche: A Mendelian randomization phenome-wide association study. BMC Medicine, 18(1), 71. doi: 10.1186/s12916-020-01515-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mauz, E., Lange, M., Houben, R., Hoffmann, R., Allen, J., Gosswald, A., … Ki, G. G. S. C. R. T. (2020). Cohort profile: KiGGS cohort longitudinal study on the health of children, adolescents and young adults in Germany. International Journal of Epidemiology, 49(2), 375375k. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyz231CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milano, W., Ambrosio, P., Carizzone, F., De Biasio, V., Di Munzio, W., Foia, M. G., & Capasso, A. (2020). Depression and obesity: Analysis of common biomarkers. Diseases (basel, Switzerland), 8(2), 23. doi: 10.3390/diseases8020023Google ScholarPubMed
Mojtabai, R., Olfson, M., & Han, B. (2016). National trends in the prevalence and treatment of depression in adolescents and young adults. Pediatrics, 138(6), e20161878. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-1878CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Must, A., Phillips, S. M., Naumova, E. N., Blum, M., Harris, S., Dawson-Hughes, B., & Rand, W. M. (2002). Recall of early menstrual history and menarcheal body size: After 30 years, how well do women remember? American Journal of Epidemiology, 155(7), 672679. doi: 10.1093/aje/155.7.672CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oelkers, L., Vogel, M., Kalenda, A., Surup, H. C., Korner, A., Kratzsch, J., & Kiess, W. (2020). Socioeconomic status is related to pubertal development in a German cohort. Hormone Research in Paediatrics, 93(9–10), 548557. doi: 10.1159/000513787CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pathomvanich, A., Merke, D. P., & Chrousos, G. P. (2000). Early puberty: A cautionary tale. Pediatrics, 105(1 Pt 1), 115116. doi: 10.1542/peds.105.1.115CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulit, S. L., Stoneman, C., Morris, A. P., Wood, A. R., Glastonbury, C. A., Tyrrell, J., … Lindgren, C. M. (2019). Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies for body fat distribution in 694 649 individuals of European ancestry. Human Molecular Genetics, 28(1), 166174. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddy327CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richard, A., Rohrmann, S., Lohse, T., & Eichholzer, M. (2016). Is body weight dissatisfaction a predictor of depression independent of body mass index, sex and age? Results of a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health, 16(1), 863. doi: 10.1186/s12889-016-3497-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rudolph, K. D., & Troop-Gordon, W. (2010). Personal-accentuation and contextual-amplification models of pubertal timing: Predicting youth depression. Development and Psychopathology, 22(2), 433451. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000167CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanderson, E., Spiller, W., & Bowden, J. (2020). Testing and correcting for weak and pleiotropic instruments in two-sample multivariable Mendelian randomisation. Human Heredity, 84(4-5), 223223. doi: 10.1101/2020.04.02.021980Google Scholar
Shim, H., Chasman, D. I., Smith, J. D., Mora, S., Ridker, P. M., Nickerson, D. A., … Stephens, M. (2015). A multivariate genome-wide association analysis of 10 LDL subfractions, and their response to statin treatment, in 1868 Caucasians. PloS One, 10(4), e0120758. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0120758CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skrivankova, V. W., Richmond, R. C., Woolf, B. A. R., Yarmolinsky, J., Davies, N. M., Swanson, S. A., … Richards, J. B. (2021). Strengthening the reporting of observational studies in epidemiology using Mendelian randomization: The STROBE-MR statement. JAMA, 326(16), 16141621. doi: 10.1001/jama.2021.18236CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stumper, A., & Alloy, L. B. (2023). Associations between pubertal stage and depression: A systematic review of the literature. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 54(2), 312339. doi: 10.1007/s10578-021-01244-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torvik, F. A., Flato, M., McAdams, T. A., Colman, I., Silventoinen, K., & Stoltenberg, C. (2021). Early puberty is associated with higher academic achievement in boys and girls and partially explains academic sex differences. Journal of Adolescent Health, 69(3), 503510. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2021.02.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trzaskowski, M., Mehta, D., Peyrot, W. J., Hawkes, D., Davies, D., Howard, D. M., … Major Depressive Disorder Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics, C. (2019). Quantifying between-cohort and between-sex genetic heterogeneity in major depressive disorder. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 180(6), 439447. doi:10.1002/ajmg.b.32713CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ullsperger, J. M., & Nikolas, M. A. (2017). A meta-analytic review of the association between pubertal timing and psychopathology in adolescence: Are there sex differences in risk? Psychological Bulletin, 143(9), 903938. doi: 10.1037/bul0000106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verbanck, M., Chen, C. Y., Neale, B., & Do, R. (2018). Detection of widespread horizontal pleiotropy in causal relationships inferred from Mendelian randomization between complex traits and diseases. Nature Genetics, 50(5), 693698. doi: 10.1038/s41588-018-0099-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vogelezang, S., Bradfield, J. P., Ahluwalia, T. S., Curtin, J. A., Lakka, T. A., Grarup, N., … Felix, J. F. (2020). Novel loci for childhood body mass index and shared heritability with adult cardiometabolic traits. Plos Genetics, 16(10), e1008718. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1008718CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, H., Lin, S. L., Leung, G. M., & Schooling, C. M. (2016). Age at onset of puberty and adolescent depression: “Children of 1997” birth cohort. Pediatrics, 137(6), e20153231. doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-3231CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wray, N. R., Ripke, S., Mattheisen, M., Trzaskowski, M., Byrne, E. M., Abdellaoui, A., … Major Depressive Disorder Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics, C. (2018). Genome-wide association analyses identify 44 risk variants and refine the genetic architecture of major depression. Nature Genetics, 50(5), 668681. doi: 10.1038/s41588-018-0090-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Study design flowchart. MDD, major depressive disorder.Note: Some participants had missing information on more than more variable.

Figure 1

Table 1. Descriptives

Figure 2

Figure 2. Cumulative probability of voice break (Panel A) and major depressive disorder (MDD, Panel B) dependent on age.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Results of the multi-SNP univariate Mendelian randomization (MR) analyses regarding the effect of male pubertal timing (MPT) [Hollis et al. (2020)] on MDD risk [Howard et al. (2019)]. OR = odds ratio, CI, confidence interval; b, unstandardized causal estimate of the change in risk for depression per one-year change in puberty time.

Figure 4

Figure 4. Results of the multivariable MR (MVMR) analyses of the causal effect of male pubertal timing (MPT) [Hollis et al. (2020)] on MDD risk [Howard et al. (2019)] adjusted for BMI [Pulit et al. (2019)] calculated using three different methods. OR, odds ratio; CI, confidence interval; b, unstandardized causal estimate of the change in risk for depression per one-year change in MPT.

Supplementary material: File

Hirtz et al. supplementary material 1

Hirtz et al. supplementary material
Download Hirtz et al. supplementary material 1(File)
File 3.4 MB
Supplementary material: File

Hirtz et al. supplementary material 2

Hirtz et al. supplementary material
Download Hirtz et al. supplementary material 2(File)
File 39.1 KB