Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T06:16:19.253Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The predisposition to hallucinate

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 July 2009

Alan Richardson*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Western Australia
Prem Divyo
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Western Australia
*
1Address for correspondence: Dr Alan Richardson, Department of Psychology, University of Western Australia, Nedlands, 6009, Western Australia.

Synopsis

It is argued that people who are predisposed to mistake a vivid imagination image for a genuine percept (i.e. hallucinators) should show an impaired ability to make clear perceptual–conceptual distinctions (i.e. boundary confusion) and should lack familiarity with internal sources of information. A test of these hypotheses was undertaken by comparing a group of ‘alcoholic’ hallucinators with a group of ‘alcoholic’ non-hallucinators. Some support is claimed for the ‘boundary confusion’ hypothesis but not for the ‘lack of familiarity’ hypothesis. Specifically, it was found that hallucinators performed significantly worse than non-hallucinators on the Embedded Figures Test (a measure of perceptual boundary confusion) and on the Intersensory Time Judgement Test (a measure of conceptual boundary confusion). These results are discussed in relation to other aspects of the predisposition to hallucinate. Some specific research needs are described.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1980

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

As, A. (1962). A factor analytic study of some subjective personal experiences and their bearing on theories of hypnosis. Acta psychologica 20, 196209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bergman, H. & Agren, G. (1974). Cognitive style and intellectual performance in relation to the progress of alcoholism. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcoholism 35, 12421255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bownes, A. F. (1967). Field articulation in paranoid and non-paranoid schizophrenic reactions: a comparative developmental study. Unpublished doctoral thesis: University of Western Australia.Google Scholar
Brett, E. A. & Starker, S. (1977). Auditory imagery and hallucinations. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 164, 394400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
David, O. & Glicksman, M. (1976). Cognitive style and the Perky effect. Perceptual and Motor Skills 42, 432434.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, S. (1962). Body image boundaries and hallucinations. In Hallucinations (ed. West, L. J.), pp. 249257. Grune and Stratton: New York.Google Scholar
Foulkes, D., Spear, P. S. & Symonds, J. D. (1966). Individual differences in mental activity at sleep onset. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 71, 280286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freedman, S. J. & Marks, P. A. (1965). Visual imagery produced by rhythmic photic stimulation: personality correlates and phenomenology. British Journal of Psychology 56, 95112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freedman, S. J., Grunebaum, H. V., Stare, F. A. & Greenblatt, M. (1962). Imagery in sensory deprivation. In Hallucinations (ed. West, L. J.), pp. 108117. Grune and Stratton: New York.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1957). A metapsychological supplement to the theory of dreams. In The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud Vol. 14 (ed. Strachey, J.), pp. 222235. Hogarth Press: London.Google Scholar
Goldberger, L. & Holt, R. R. (1961). A comparison of isolation effects and their personality correlates in two divergent samples. ASD Technical Report 61–417. Wright-Patterson AFB: Ohio.Google Scholar
Goldstone, S. (1962). Psychophysics, reality and hallucinations. In Hallucinations (ed. West, L. J.), pp. 261272. Grune and Stratton: New York.Google Scholar
Goldstone, S. & Lhamon, W. T. (1974). Studies of auditory–visual differences in human time judgement: 1. Sounds are judged longer than lights. Perceptual and Motor Skills 39, 6382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, F. K. & Kendall, B. S. (1946). Performance of brain injured cases on a memory-for-designs test. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 41, 303314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, F. K. & Kendall, B. S. (1960). Memory-for-designs test: revised manual. Perceptual and Motor Skills 11, 147188 (Monogr. Suppl. 2-VII).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holt, R. R. & Goldberger, L. (1959). Personological correlates of reaction to perceptual isolation. WADC Technical Report 59753. Wright-Patterson AFB: Ohio.Google Scholar
Horowitz, M. J. (1970). Image Formation and Cognition. Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York.Google Scholar
Horowitz, M. J. (1975). Hallucinations, an information processing approach. In Hallucinations: Behavior, Experience and Theory (ed. West, L. J. and Siegel, R.) pp. 163195. John Wiley: New York.Google Scholar
Mintz, S. & Alpert, M. (1972). Imagery vividness, reality testing and schizophrenic hallucinations. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 79, 310316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richardson, A. (1969). Mental Imagery. Routledge & Kegan Paul: London.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied 80, 128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Segal, S. J. & Nathan, S. (1964). The Perky effect: incorporation of an external stimulus into an imagery experience under placebo and control conditions. Perceptual and Motor Skills 18, 385395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seitz, P. F. D. & Molholm, H. B. (1947). Relation of mental imagery to hallucinations. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry 57, 469480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheehan, P. W. (1967). A shortened form of Betts' Questionnaire upon mental imagery. Journal of Clinical Psychology 23, 386389.3.0.CO;2-S>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silverman, A. J., Cohen, S. I., Shmavonian, B. M. & Greenberg, G. (1961). Psychophysiological investigations in sensory deprivation. Psychosomatic Medicine 23, 4861.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, J. L. & Antrobus, J. G. (1966). Imaginal Processes Inventory. New York.Google Scholar
Singer, J. L. & Antrobus, J. G. (1972). Daydreaming, imaginal processes and personality: a normative study. In The Function and Nature of Imagery (ed. Sheehan, P. W.), pp. 175202. Academic Press: New York.Google Scholar
Slade, P. D. (1976). An investigation of psychological factors involved in the predisposition to auditory hallucinations. Psychological Medicine 6, 123132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vernon, P. E. (1972). The distinctiveness of field independence. Journal of Personality 40, 366391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
West, L. J. (1962). A general theory of hallucinations and dreams. In Hallucinations (ed. West, L. J.), pp. 275291. Grune & Stratton: New York.Google Scholar
Witkin, H. A., Lewis, H. B., Hertzman, M., Machover, K., Meissner, P. B. & Wapner, S. (1954). Personality Through Perception. Harper: New York.Google Scholar
Ziskind, E. & Augsburg, T. (1962). Hallucinations in sensory deprivation – method or madness. Science 137, 992993.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zubek, J. P., Welch, G. & Saunders, M. G. (1963). Electroencephalographic changes during and after 14 days of perceptual deprivation. Science 139, 490492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed