Introduction
Depressive and anxiety disorders occur across all age ranges and are associated with significant disability (Ferrari et al., Reference Ferrari, Charlson, Norman, Patten, Freedman, Murray and Whiteford2013; Whiteford et al., Reference Whiteford, Degenhardt, Rehm, Baxter, Ferrari, Erskine and Vos2013). Yet, how exactly internalizing disorders differ across age and sex is a subject of debate and few studies have been able to study their patterns over lifetime in detail. More insight into these patterns can be used to identify target populations for public health interventions (Twenge, Cooper, Joiner, Duffy, & Binau, Reference Twenge, Cooper, Joiner, Duffy and Binau2019). Furthermore, this insight could inform hypotheses on specific risk factors for internalizing disorders over the course of life. For example, it has been suggested that changes in women's reproductive hormones during the menopause increase their risk for internalizing disorders, but results are inconclusive (Bryant, Judd, & Hickey, Reference Bryant, Judd and Hickey2012; Judd, Hickey, & Bryant, Reference Judd, Hickey and Bryant2012; Kuehner, Reference Kuehner2017; Rössler, Ajdacic-Gross, Riecher-Rössler, Angst, & Hengartner, Reference Rössler, Ajdacic-Gross, Riecher-Rössler, Angst and Hengartner2016; Vivian-Taylor & Hickey, Reference Vivian-Taylor and Hickey2014). Different developments in prevalence in men and women around the age of menopause could support this hypothesis.
The first question concerns the exact development of different internalizing disorders over lifetime. Most studies in the general population find a decrease of internalizing disorders in older age (de Graaf, ten Have, van Gool, & van Dorsselaer, Reference de Graaf, ten Have, van Gool and van Dorsselaer2012; Jorm, Reference Jorm2000; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Birnbaum, Shahly, Bromet, Hwang, McLaughlin and Stein2010b; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Von Korff, Alonso, Angermeyer, Bromet, Bruffaerts and Williams2008; Trollor, Sachdev, Anderson, Andrews, & Brodaty, Reference Trollor, Sachdev, Anderson, Andrews and Brodaty2007; Wells et al., Reference Wells, Oakley Browne, Scott, McGee, Baxter and Kokaua2006). However, it remains unclear whether this decrease in prevalence is linear or nonlinear, and even though it is possible that there are multiple peaks and valleys over the lifetime, most studies use models that cannot identify patterns more complex than a U-curve (Jorm, Reference Jorm2000).
Second, there is a clear gap in the prevalence of depression and anxiety disorders between men and women, with women being affected roughly twice as often as men (Baxter et al., Reference Baxter, Scott, Ferrari, Norman, Vos and Whiteford2014; Kuehner, Reference Kuehner2017; McLean, Asnaani, Litz, & Hofmann, Reference McLean, Asnaani, Litz and Hofmann2011; Wittchen et al., Reference Wittchen, Jacobi, Rehm, Gustavsson, Svensson, Jönsson and Steinhausen2011). However, is this true over the entire lifespan? Some studies suggest that the gap between the sexes remains the same across the lifespan (Baxter et al., Reference Baxter, Scott, Ferrari, Norman, Vos and Whiteford2014; Cairney & Wade, Reference Cairney and Wade2002; Ferrari et al., Reference Ferrari, Charlson, Norman, Patten, Freedman, Murray and Whiteford2013; Jorm, Reference Jorm2000; Kessler, McGonagle, Swartz, Blazer, & Nelson, Reference Kessler, McGonagle, Swartz, Blazer and Nelson1993), but other studies found a decreased (Bebbington et al., Reference Bebbington, Dunn, Jenkins, Lewis, Brugha, Farrell and Meltzer1998; Jorm, Reference Jorm2000; Wittchen & Jacobi, Reference Wittchen and Jacobi2005) as well as an increased gap (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Birnbaum, Bromet, Hwang, Sampson and Shahly2010a) in older ages.
Lastly, it is unclear whether there are significant differences in trajectories across these various highly comorbid internalizing disorders, and how these trajectories of internalizing disorders compare with the trajectories of internalizing symptoms and traits, such as depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, negative affect (NA), and neuroticism (Jorm, Reference Jorm2000; Keyes et al., Reference Keyes, Nicholson, Kinley, Raposo, Stein, Goldner and Sareen2014; Twenge et al., Reference Twenge, Cooper, Joiner, Duffy and Binau2019; Wells et al., Reference Wells, Oakley Browne, Scott, McGee, Baxter and Kokaua2006). Insight in the difference between the trajectories of internalizing disorders, symptoms, and traits can inform discussions on classification, such as whether internalizing disorders and symptoms are sufficiently similar constructs so that the latter could serve as the measures of internalizing disorders for research and clinical purposes (Cai et al., Reference Cai, Revez, Adams, Andlauer, Breen, Byrne and Flint2020; Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Waszczuk, Krueger, Forbes, Watson, Clark and Zimmerman2017).
The study of these questions requires large general population samples with well-measured phenotypes, and statistical methods that are able to identify potentially complex nonlinear developments. Yet to date, no studies have used advanced nonlinear statistical methods to investigate the point prevalence of different internalizing disorders, symptoms, and traits as a function of age and compared these across sex.
Our aim is to investigate the prevalence of different internalizing disorders across age and sex, and compare the results of internalizing disorders with internalizing symptoms and traits. We investigate the point prevalence of major depression (MD), dysthymia (DYS), generalized anxiety disorder (GAD), panic disorder (PD), and social phobia (SPH) diagnosed at interview by DSM-IV criteria in a sample of 146 315 participants aged 18–80 years from Lifelines, a study in the Dutch general population. We also study the rates of depressive and anxiety symptoms, NA, and neuroticism. Generalized additive models (GAMs) allow us to model nonlinear patterns and test for significant differences in the development of the different internalizing disorders, symptoms and traits, and compare results for men and women.
Methods
Sample
The Lifelines Cohort Study is a multidisciplinary prospective population-based cohort study of 167 729 subjects in the north of the Netherlands. It was established as a resource for research on complex interactions between environmental, phenotypic, and genomic factors in the development of chronic diseases and healthy aging. It employs a broad range of investigative procedures in assessing the biomedical, socio-demographic, behavioral, physical, and psychological factors contributing to health and disease, with a special focus on multimorbidity and complex genetics (Scholtens et al., Reference Scholtens, Smidt, Swertz, Bakker, Dotinga, Vonk and Stolk2015; Stolk et al., Reference Stolk, Rosmalen, Postma, de Boer, Navis, Slaets and Wolffenbuttel2008). Between 2006 and 2013, an index population aged 25–49 years was recruited via participating general practitioners. Subsequently, older and younger family members were invited to participate in Lifelines. In addition, adults could self-register via the Lifelines website. In total, 49% of the included participants were invited through their GP, 38% were recruited via participating family members, and 13% self-registered (Scholtens et al., Reference Scholtens, Smidt, Swertz, Bakker, Dotinga, Vonk and Stolk2015). Most participants (57%) were included in 2012–2013 (Klijs et al., Reference Klijs, Scholtens, Mandemakers, Snieder, Stolk and Smidt2015). Baseline data were collected for 167 729 participants.
The Lifelines adult study population is broadly representative of the adult population of the north of the Netherlands. Demographic, socioeconomic, and general health characteristics of the Lifelines cohort are described elsewhere (Klijs et al., Reference Klijs, Scholtens, Mandemakers, Snieder, Stolk and Smidt2015). All participants provided written informed consent. The Lifelines Cohort Study was approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of the University Medical Center Groningen, The Netherlands. In the current study, we included all baseline participants aged 18–80 years (n = 146 315) who had available data on one or more of the internalizing disorders or symptoms. We excluded 299 participants over 80 years because of the low sample size for the statistical analyses.
Measurements
Internalizing disorders
Current MD, DYS, SPH, PD, and GAD were assessed according to DSM-IV-TR criteria with a standardized diagnostic interview based on the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) (Sheehan et al., Reference Sheehan, Lecrubier, Sheehan, Amorim, Janavs, Weiller and Dunbar1998). Trained medical assistants administered sections of the MINI to all participants during their visit to the research facilities and entered the responses into the computer. Conform DSM-IV-TR duration criteria, MD, DYS, GAD, and PD were rated as present if the subject reported the required symptoms in the past 2 weeks, 2 years, 6 months, and 1 month, respectively (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). SPH was assessed during the past month. We selected all 146 315 participants aged 18–80 with present data on the MINI questionnaire. For further details, see Supplementary Methods.
Internalizing symptoms and neuroticism
Depressive and anxiety symptoms: Using the symptoms of MD and GAD assessed with the MINI, we created two sum scores for depressive (range 0–9) and anxiety symptoms (range 0–7). As stated above, MD symptoms were assessed in the past 2 weeks, and GAD symptoms in the past 6 months. Due to changes in the design of the interview, only part of the sample (n = 73 805) had data on additional symptoms of MD and GAD if the core criteria were absent. This subsample with complete data was used for the analyses of depressive and anxiety symptoms (Supplementary Methods).
Negative affect: NA was assessed with the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) using 10 items including feeling irritable, ashamed, upset, nervous, guilty, scared, hostile, jittery, afraid, and distressed (Cronbach's α = 0.84–0.87) (Crawford & Henry, Reference Crawford and Henry2004; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, Reference Watson, Clark and Tellegen1988). Subjects were asked to rate how often they experienced each item in the past 4 weeks on a five-point Likert scale resulting in a score ranging from 10 to 50.
Neuroticism: Current neuroticism was assessed with the Revised NEO Personality Inventory (Costa & McCrae, Reference Costa and McCrae1992; Hoekstra, Ormel, & De Fruyt, Reference Hoekstra, Ormel and De Fruyt2007). The NEO PI-R Neuroticism subscale (Cronbach's α = 0.91) consists of 48 items covering the facets of anxiety, angry/hostility, depression, self-consciousness, impulsiveness, and vulnerability (Kurtz, Lee, & Sherker, Reference Kurtz, Lee and Sherker1999). Items were answered on a five-point Likert scale resulting in a sum score ranging from 48 to 240. The initial questionnaire excluded the depression and anxiety facets to limit the total length of the questionnaires, but these were added later. Here we only studied participants for whom complete data on all subscales on the NEO were available (n = 42 658) (see Supplementary Methods for details).
Statistical analysis
Weighted point prevalence
Because women and certain age groups were overrepresented in Lifelines (see Supplementary Methods), we used a person weighting factor based on age and sex to estimate the point prevalence of internalizing disorders, symptoms, and traits for the Dutch general population. Data on the sex and age distribution of the Dutch population in 2011 were derived from the CBS Statline data [Centraal Bureau voor de Statistiek (CBS), 2020].
Generalized additive models
GAMs were used to assess the prevalence of internalizing disorders, symptoms, and traits as a function of age. GAMs are regression models that can identify and characterize complex nonlinear regression effects (Hastie, Tibshirani, & Friedman, Reference Hastie, Tibshirani and Friedman2011), by automatically determining the optimal combination of nonlinear basis functions (e.g. linear terms, polynomial terms, cubic terms, etc.) (Wieling, Reference Wieling2018; Wood, Reference Wood2017). Overfitting is prevented by minimizing a combination of the error and a non-linearity penalty (Wieling, Reference Wieling2018). All analyses were performed in R_3.5.2 using the packages mgcv_1.9.29 (Marra & Wood, Reference Marra and Wood2011; Wood, Reference Wood2017) and itsadug_2.3 (van Rij, Wieling, Baayen, & van Rijn, Reference van Rij, Wieling, Baayen and van Rijn2016). We modeled the prevalence of each internalizing disorder, and the means of the symptom scores and neuroticism score as a (potentially) nonlinear function of age, and tested if there was a significant interaction effect between sex and age, i.e. if the patterns across age varied depending on sex. Subsequently, we modelled the patterns of the five internalizing disorders to investigate if the intercept and the pattern across age varied depending on the disorder type. For these models, the prevalence of any disorder served as the dependent variable, and the type of disorder was used as the independent variable. The reference classes were varied to make sure the results were robust.
Sensitivity analyses
Internalizing disorders are highly comorbid (Bijl, van Zessen, Ravelli, de Rijk, & Langendoen, Reference Bijl, van Zessen, Ravelli, de Rijk and Langendoen1998b; Kessler, Chiu, Demler, & Walters, Reference Kessler, Chiu, Demler and Walters2005). Therefore, we performed a sensitivity analysis by including a random intercept for each subject in the GAM. This random intercept accounted for individual variation in vulnerability for internalizing disorders, irrespective of age, so that the fixed effect of age on internalizing disorders on a group level could be estimated. As the current software was not able to run a GAM with random effects for the full sample, we divided the sample into 10 random subsamples of 14 624 individuals each. These subsamples were matched to the full sample based on age and sex distributions. Then, we performed the GAM without and with random intercepts for these 10 subsamples, and compared the results.
Because family history is an important risk for developing internalizing disorders, we also performed a sensitivity analysis by including a random intercept in the GAMs for individual disorders in the full sample. This random intercept accounted for family variation in vulnerability for internalizing disorders.
Results
Point prevalence
The included 146 315 participants had a mean age of 44.2 years (s.d. 12.7) and 58.6% were women (Table 1). The age and sex weighted point prevalence rates showed that current GAD was reported most frequently (3.7%), followed by MD (2.0%), DYS (1.0%), and SPH (0.8%). PD in the past month was rare (0.21%). The point prevalence rates differed significantly between all disorders as indicated by the parametric terms for each of the disorders compared to the reference class (online Supplementary Table S2). The unweighted prevalence rates were somewhat higher for all disorders because of the sex and age composition of Lifelines participants, including a higher percentage of women than the general Dutch population (online Supplementary Table S1).
DYS, dysthymia; GAD, generalized anxiety disorder; MD, major depression; PD, panic disorder; s.d., standard deviation; s.e., standard error; SPH, social phobia.
a Highest completed education: ‘Low’ is completed junior general secondary education (mavo/vmbo-t) or lower, or no education; ‘Intermediate’ is completed secondary vocational education (mbo), senior general secondary education (havo, vwo, hbs, mms); ‘High’ is completed higher vocational education (hbo) or university.
b Age and sex weighted estimates to the average Dutch population in 2011. For unweighted estimates, see online Supplementary Table S1.
Lifetime patterns of internalizing disorders
All internalizing disorders showed significant nonlinear patterns over the lifespan (Fig. 1, online Supplementary Table S2). The general trend was that their prevalence increased from the age of 18 until the age of 30, stabilized until the age of 50, and then decreased. However, there were also differences between the disorders, as indicated by their significantly different curves. The prevalence of SPH and PD decreased relatively early in life, whereas the prevalence of MD peaked at two ages, around 30 and 50 years, a pattern not seen with other disorders. Additionally, the prevalence of GAD and DYS dropped more steeply after the age of 50 than did the other disorders. The curves for GAD-DYS and for PD-SPH were not significantly different when changing the reference class, indicating no robust difference in their curves.
Sex differences and similarities
As expected, women reported more internalizing disorders than men across the entire age range. The intercepts for each disorder were all significantly different for each disorder (Fig. 2, online Supplementary Table S3). However, the curves showing the increase and decrease of prevalence over age were not significantly different between the sexes, and this was true for each internalizing disorder. This implied that the odds ratio and the relative risk (i.e. prevalence women/prevalence men) were stable across the different age groups: about 1.7 for MD, DYS, GAD, and PD, and 1.2 for SPH (online Supplementary Table S4).
Comparison with internalizing symptoms and neuroticism
Internalizing symptoms and traits showed different patterns across age than did internalizing disorders (Fig. 3, online Supplementary Table S3). Depressive symptoms decreased slightly from age 18 until the age of 35, increased until the age of 50, and then decreased again until the age of 65, after which symptoms increased again. Anxiety symptoms increased until the age of 40, and then decreased, with a stabilization after age 70. Neuroticism and NA decreased largely linearly from the age of 18 years. NA diminished linearly except from an increase from the age of 45 until the age of 55, but this increase was minor (<0.5 point on a scale from 10 to 50), and neuroticism stabilized from the age of 50.
Comparable to the internalizing disorders, women scored higher on neuroticism and all internalizing symptoms than men, especially in depressive and anxiety symptoms (ratio W/M~1.6) and less for NA and neuroticism (ratio W/M~1.1)(online Supplementary Table S4). The curves for depressive symptoms were similar in men and women, meaning that the absolute difference in the number of depressive symptoms remained constant over lifetime. The curves for generalized anxiety symptoms, neuroticism, and NA were significantly different across sex, although Fig. 3 shows that these differences were modest.
Sensitivity analyses
To investigate the potential impact of comorbidity on the different trajectories of the internalizing disorders over lifetime, we performed a sensitivity analysis comparing models excluding and including random intercepts for each subject in 10 random subsamples each including about 10% of the sample. The estimated trajectories of the prevalence of internalizing disorders over lifetime were similar in all models including and excluding random intercepts (online Supplementary Table S5). To investigate the potential impact of the family structure of the Lifelines sample on our results, we performed another sensitivity analysis comparing models excluding and including random intercept for family structure. The estimated trajectories were again similar in all models including and excluding random intercepts (online Supplementary Table S6).
Discussion
Main findings
In this study of 146 315 subjects from the Dutch general population aged 18–80 years, we investigated the patterns of the point prevalences of MD, DYS, GAD, SPH, and PD across different ages and sex. In general, our modeling indicated an increase in the prevalence of internalizing disorders from the age of 18 years, a plateau phase between 30 and 50 years of age, and a decrease after age 50. There were differences in the nonlinear patterns over lifetime between most disorders. Internalizing symptoms and neuroticism showed a distinctly different pattern over the lifetime compared with internalizing disorders. Although women reported more internalizing disorders and symptoms and higher neuroticism than men, the relative risk over the life course was remarkably similar.
Comparison to previous studies
To our knowledge, no previous studies used GAM to investigate the development of different internalizing disorders and symptoms and neuroticism over lifetime and across sex. Thus, we cannot directly compare the nonlinear patterns and statistical differences between the internalizing disorders and symptoms and neuroticism with the results of previous studies. However, we can compare some key findings with previous findings.
First, our estimated point prevalences of the internalizing disorders are close to the estimates of point prevalence in previous studies. Our prevalence estimate of GAD was in the range of other studies (3.7% in Lifelines v. 1.7–4.1%) (de Graaf et al., Reference de Graaf, ten Have, van Gool and van Dorsselaer2012; McLean et al., Reference McLean, Asnaani, Litz and Hofmann2011; Wittchen & Jacobi, Reference Wittchen and Jacobi2005), which was also true for our prevalence estimate of DYS (1.0% in Lifelines v. 0.9–2.3%) (Bijl, Ravelli, & van Zessen, Reference Bijl, Ravelli and van Zessen1998a; Charlson, Ferrari, Flaxman, & Whiteford, Reference Charlson, Ferrari, Flaxman and Whiteford2013; de Graaf et al., Reference de Graaf, ten Have, van Gool and van Dorsselaer2012). Also the overall point prevalence of any anxiety disorder was comparable to other studies (4.3% in Lifelines v. 4.0–9.7%) (Baxter, Scott, Vos, & Whiteford, Reference Baxter, Scott, Vos and Whiteford2013, Reference Baxter, Scott, Ferrari, Norman, Vos and Whiteford2014; Bijl et al., Reference Bijl, Ravelli and van Zessen1998a). Our past month estimates of PD (0.21%) and SPH (0.84%) were lower than in a smaller Dutch study in the general population (PD 1.5%; SPH 3.7%) (Bijl et al., Reference Bijl, Ravelli and van Zessen1998a). This may be due to slightly different criteria in DSM-III-R and DSM-IV-TR for PD (American Psychiatric Association, 1987, 2000), or the use of other assessment methods. Our estimate of MD was slightly lower than in other population studies (2.0% in Lifelines v. 2.7–4.4%) (Bijl et al., Reference Bijl, Ravelli and van Zessen1998a; de Graaf et al., Reference de Graaf, ten Have, van Gool and van Dorsselaer2012; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Birnbaum, Bromet, Hwang, Sampson and Shahly2010a), which may have to do with a different time frame for assessment (past 2 weeks in Lifelines v. past month in previous studies). The relative differences in point prevalence for men and women are also as expected (Baxter et al., Reference Baxter, Scott, Ferrari, Norman, Vos and Whiteford2014; Kuehner, Reference Kuehner2003, Reference Kuehner2017; McLean et al., Reference McLean, Asnaani, Litz and Hofmann2011; Wittchen et al., Reference Wittchen, Jacobi, Rehm, Gustavsson, Svensson, Jönsson and Steinhausen2011).
Second, similar to this study, two reviews found that the point prevalence of internalizing disorders followed a nonlinear development over lifetime following an inversed U-shape (Baxter et al., Reference Baxter, Scott, Ferrari, Norman, Vos and Whiteford2014; Charlson et al., Reference Charlson, Ferrari, Flaxman and Whiteford2013). Anxiety disorders manifested an initial rise in prevalence until age 30, followed by a decrease which was more pronounced after age 50, similar to our findings (Baxter et al., Reference Baxter, Scott, Ferrari, Norman, Vos and Whiteford2014). The pattern for MD was slightly different – a rise in the prevalence of MD until age 50, followed by a decrease, and a second rise after age 75. This review also suggested similar curves for men and women across the lifespan (Baxter et al., Reference Baxter, Scott, Ferrari, Norman, Vos and Whiteford2014). Another review described an increase in the prevalence of DYS at early ages with a peak around 50 years (Charlson et al., Reference Charlson, Ferrari, Flaxman and Whiteford2013). Unlike our study, these reviews included studies with substantial heterogeneity, used relatively few data points [e.g. 141 (Charlson et al., Reference Charlson, Ferrari, Flaxman and Whiteford2013)], and did not formally test for complex nonlinearity or sex differences in their results.
Implication of findings
Since this is the first study that used advanced nonlinear models to investigate the prevalence of internalizing disorders, symptoms, and traits across age and sex, we should be careful in drawing definitive conclusions. But if the results prove to be robust, they may have several implications.
First, the fact that the relative gap between the sexes remains stable over the lifetime has implications for hypotheses about risk factors for internalizing disorders. Women clearly report more internalizing disorders than men. Previous studies showed that the gap in MD prevalence between the sexes arises in puberty, due to higher incidence rates in women (Altemus, Sarvaiya, & Neill Epperson, Reference Altemus, Sarvaiya and Neill Epperson2014; Kessler, Reference Kessler2003; Kuehner, Reference Kuehner2017). One of the hypotheses for this gap between the sexes are changes in female sex hormones during lifetime, for instance around the menopause. There are suggestions that estrogens are neuroprotective, and a decrease in estrogens in menopause would increase women's risk of MD (Georgakis et al., Reference Georgakis, Thomopoulos, Diamantaras, Kalogirou, Skalkidou, Daskalopoulou and Petridou2016). Several cross-sectional and longitudinal studies have studied the prevalence of MD and anxiety disorders around the menopause in women, but results are inconsistent (Bryant et al., Reference Bryant, Judd and Hickey2012; Judd et al., Reference Judd, Hickey and Bryant2012; Kuehner, Reference Kuehner2017; Rössler et al., Reference Rössler, Ajdacic-Gross, Riecher-Rössler, Angst and Hengartner2016; Vivian-Taylor & Hickey, Reference Vivian-Taylor and Hickey2014). Our study shows that around the age of the menopause, women indeed report more MD and depressive symptoms. However, there is a similar rise in MD and depressive symptoms in men in this age group. This implies that perimenopausal changes in female sex hormones probably do not significantly influence women's risk of depression, unless male hormonal changes or other male-specific risk factors exist that explain the similar increase in depression prevalence in middle-aged men. It is more likely that shared risk factors – e.g. psychosocial distress (Rössler et al., Reference Rössler, Ajdacic-Gross, Riecher-Rössler, Angst and Hengartner2016) – explain the similar rise in depression prevalence in both sexes during midlife. A similar argument can be made for anxiety disorders, in which the relative gap between the sexes is also stable across age.
Second, the prevalence of most internalizing disorders showed different patterns over lifetime, which suggests that these disorders are not entirely identical constructs, but may have meaningful differences in etiology. At the same time, the similarity of the general pattern among the internalizing disorders suggests that there are likely shared risk factors (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Aggen, Knudsen, Roysamb, Neale and Reichborn-Kjennerud2011; Schoevers, Beekman, Deeg, Jonker, & Van Tilburg, Reference Schoevers, Beekman, Deeg, Jonker and Van Tilburg2003; Vink, Aartsen, & Schoevers, Reference Vink, Aartsen and Schoevers2008).
Third, the lifetime patterns of internalizing disorders differed from those of the internalizing symptoms and neuroticism, suggesting that the relationship between these is complex, or at least not stable across the lifespan. For instance, the prevalence of depressive symptoms, but not MD, was rising after the age of 65. This may be due to the fact that older subjects report somatic symptoms of depression more often without having episodes of MD (Balsis & Cully, Reference Balsis and Cully2008; Hegeman, Kok, Van Der Mast, & Giltay, Reference Hegeman, Kok, Van Der Mast and Giltay2012). In any case, the fact that internalizing disorders show different patterns across age and sex than internalizing symptoms and neuroticism is relevant for the debate on the nature and classification of internalizing disorders. In this debate, psychopathology is assumed to exist on a continuum instead of there being clear boundaries between health and disease (Kendell & Jablensky, Reference Kendell and Jablensky2003; Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Waszczuk, Krueger, Forbes, Watson, Clark and Zimmerman2017). Although we only investigated differences in prevalence rates, our data show that there may be important differences between internalizing disorders and symptoms and traits. This difference implies that we should be cautious in reducing internalizing disorders to high scores on symptom dimensions (Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Waszczuk, Krueger, Forbes, Watson, Clark and Zimmerman2017; Schoevers et al., Reference Schoevers, Beekman, Deeg, Jonker and Van Tilburg2003). This concern is supported by genetic studies showing that depressive symptoms are not always good proxies for MD (Cai et al., Reference Cai, Revez, Adams, Andlauer, Breen, Byrne and Flint2020; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Gardner, Neale, Aggen, Heath, Colodro-Conde and Gillespie2019).
Strengths and limitations
This is the first study that used advanced nonlinear models to investigate the development of internalizing disorders and symptoms and neuroticism over lifetime in a large sample from the general population. The disorders were assessed with structured interviews by trained research assistants, and focused on current psychopathology to minimize recall bias (Kessler, Petukhova, Sampson, Zaslavsky, & Wittchen, Reference Kessler, Petukhova, Sampson, Zaslavsky and Wittchen2012; Kruijshaar et al., Reference Kruijshaar, Barendregt, Vos, De Graaf, Spijker and Andrews2005).
We also note a number of limitations. Our study uses cross-sectional data, and therefore cannot exclude period or cohort effects as an explanation for the change in point prevalence estimates across different ages. It is unlikely, however, that our findings are exclusively based on period and cohort effects. A recent study in 611 880 subjects from the US population controlling for period and cohort effects showed that the prevalence of depressive episodes followed an inverse U-shaped curve with increasing prevalence from the age of 18 and decreased after age 32, and that psychological distress declined with age (Twenge et al., Reference Twenge, Cooper, Joiner, Duffy and Binau2019). Also population studies that were performed two decades apart indicate that the reduction of internalizing disorders is associated with older age (Baxter et al., Reference Baxter, Scott, Ferrari, Norman, Vos and Whiteford2014; Bijl et al., Reference Bijl, Ravelli and van Zessen1998a; de Graaf et al., Reference de Graaf, ten Have, van Gool and van Dorsselaer2012). Future assessment waves of Lifelines would allow an investigation of age, period, and cohort effects.
Similar to these population studies, we observed a reduction in the prevalence of internalizing disorders at older age. There are two types of explanations for the decline of internalizing disorders; (1) age is protective against internalizing disorders, (2) age is not protective, but internalizing disorders are less frequently measured in older participants due to biases. Selection bias occurs when older individuals with MD are relatively less often participating in population studies than younger individuals with MD, for example, when there is increased difficulty in establishing contact or increased refusals (Beekman et al., Reference Beekman, Geerlings, Deeg, Smit, Schoevers, De Beurs and Van Tilburg2002; Holwerda et al., Reference Holwerda, Schoevers, Dekker, Deeg, Jonker and Beekman2007; Schoevers et al., Reference Schoevers, Geerlings, Beekman, Penninx, Deeg, Jonker and Van Tilburg2000). Reporting bias might be a result of older people being less likely to report symptoms of depression (Knäuper & Wittchen, Reference Knäuper and Wittchen1994; Lyness et al., Reference Lyness, Cox, Curry, Conwell, King and Caine1995). It is also possible that the prevalence of depression at older age is lower because individuals suffering from depression are more likely to have died earlier due to related causes such as heart problems (i.e. survivor bias) (White, Schulz, Klein, & von Klitzing, Reference White, Schulz, Klein and von Klitzing2019; Wray et al., Reference Wray, Ripke, Mattheisen, Trzaskowski, Byrne, Abdellaoui and Sullivan2018). However, in Lifelines, we found no interaction effect between age and the presence of an internalizing disorder at baseline when predicting participation at follow up (2014–2017) (data not shown). This means that the impact of having a disorder on attrition for any reason was not different for older as compared to younger subjects, which makes selection bias a less likely explanation for the reduction in prevalence after age 50. Follow-up studies are needed to investigate explanations for the decline of internalizing disorders, symptoms, and traits in older participants.
Third, we assessed current internalizing disorders based on structured interviews with trained research assistants, which can be considered a strength. However, there were two limitations in the assessments. Disability was not assessed for MD and GAD, and DYS was not assessed in subjects who satisfied the criteria for MD, which could have biased prevalence rates upwards and downwards, respectively. It is most likely that these biases have been minor given that our estimates of MD, GAD, and DYS are comparable to previous estimates (Baxter et al., Reference Baxter, Scott, Ferrari, Norman, Vos and Whiteford2014; Bijl et al., Reference Bijl, Ravelli and van Zessen1998a; Charlson et al., Reference Charlson, Ferrari, Flaxman and Whiteford2013; de Graaf et al., Reference de Graaf, ten Have, van Gool and van Dorsselaer2012).
Fourth, the presence of internalizing symptoms may influence subjects' reports on internalizing traits like neuroticism, which could complicate disentangling between states and traits. Previous studies showed that subjects with depressive symptoms may temporarily score higher on neuroticism (Jeronimus, Ormel, Aleman, Penninx, & Riese, Reference Jeronimus, Ormel, Aleman, Penninx and Riese2013; Kotov, Gamez, Schmidt, & Watson, Reference Kotov, Gamez, Schmidt and Watson2010). If internalizing symptoms indeed have a strong effect on neuroticism, then we would have expected to see a similarity between the patterns of internalizing symptoms and neuroticism across age. However, in our study, neuroticism scores were not showing the same patterns as depressive symptoms, generalized anxiety symptoms, or NA. For example, neuroticism scores were lower in subjects aged 30–50 years than in younger subjects, whereas depressive symptoms were higher in this age group. Although these findings do not fully exclude that internalizing symptoms may have influenced neuroticism scores, it shows that the influence in our study is probably modest.
Conclusion
This study identified different patterns in point prevalence for most internalizing disorders, symptoms, and traits over lifetime. The overall prevalence of internalizing disorders, symptoms, and traits in women was higher than in men, but the patterns across age were remarkably similar in both sexes. These results indicate that certain hypotheses for the gap between the sexes, e.g. the changes in female sex hormones during menopause, are unlikely explanations. Future studies are needed to investigate the causes for the initial rise in internalizing disorders and their decline at older age, taking into account the sex similarities.
Supplementary material
The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721001148.
Author contributions
All authors contributed to the design of the study. HMvL, KSK, and TRdJ were involved in data collection. LB, HMvL, and MW performed the statistical analyses. HMvL and LB drafted the manuscript; all other authors provided feedback on drafts of the manuscript.
Financial support
HMvL was supported by a NARSAD Young Investigator Grant from the Brain & Behavior Research Foundation and a VENI grant from the Talent Programme of the Netherlands Organization of Scientific Research (NWO-ZonMW 09150161810021). The funding organizations had no impact on study design, data collection, analysis, or interpretation, or decision to submit the manuscript.
Conflict of interest
None.