Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-11T04:38:34.677Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Stuck in a negative me: fMRI study on the role of disturbed self-views in social feedback processing in borderline personality disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2019

Charlotte C. van Schie*
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, Leiden, The Netherlands
Chui-De Chiu
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong S. A. R., People's Republic of China
Serge A. R. B. Rombouts
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, Leiden, The Netherlands Leiden University Medical Centre, Leiden, The Netherlands
Willem J. Heiser
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands Mathematical Institute, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands
Bernet M. Elzinga
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, Leiden, The Netherlands
*
Author for correspondence: Charlotte C. van Schie, E-mail: c.c.van.schie@fsw.leidenuniv.nl
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Interpersonal difficulties in borderline personality disorder (BPD) could be related to the disturbed self-views of BPD patients. This study investigates affective and neural responses to positive and negative social feedback (SF) of BPD patients compared with healthy (HC) and low self-esteem (LSE) controls and how this relates to individual self-views.

Methods

BPD (N = 26), HC (N = 32), and LSE (N = 22) performed a SF task in a magnetic resonance imaging scanner. Participants received 15 negative, intermediate and positive evaluative feedback words putatively given by another participant and rated their mood and applicability of the words to the self.

Results

BPD had more negative self-views than HC and felt worse after negative feedback. Applicability of feedback was a less strong determinant of mood in BPD than HC. Increased precuneus activation was observed in HC to negative compared with positive feedback, whereas in BPD, this was similarly low for both valences. HC showed increased temporoparietal junction (TPJ) activation to positive v. negative feedback, while BPD showed more TPJ activation to negative feedback. The LSE group showed a different pattern of results suggesting that LSE cannot explain these findings in BPD.

Conclusions

The negative self-views that BPD have, may obstruct critically examining negative feedback, resulting in lower mood. Moreover, where HC focus on the positive feedback (based on TPJ activation), BPD seem to focus more on negative feedback, potentially maintaining negative self-views. Better balanced self-views may make BPD better equipped to deal with potential negative feedback and more open to positive interactions.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - SA
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the same Creative Commons licence is included and the original work is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Introduction

Humans have an innate need to form relationships (Over, Reference Over2016). However, for patients with borderline personality disorder (BPD), this is a struggle as they encounter interpersonal problems on a daily basis (Lieb et al., Reference Lieb, Zanarini, Schmahl, Linehan and Bohus2004; Lazarus et al., Reference Lazarus, Cheavens, Festa and Zachary Rosenthal2014). Interpersonal difficulties are strongly interlinked with identity disturbance as the way we view ourselves shapes our interaction with others, just as our self-views are shaped by interaction with others (Jorgensen, Reference Jorgensen2010; Kerr et al., Reference Kerr, Finlayson-Short, McCutcheon, Beard and Chanen2015; van Schie et al., Reference van Schie, Chiu, Rombouts, Heiser and Elzinga2018). Since BPD patients tend to have fragmented and conflicting self-views that are predominantly negative (Wilkinson-Ryan and Westen, Reference Wilkinson-Ryan and Westen2000; Zeigler-Hill, Reference Zeigler-Hill2006), this may make them more vulnerable for interpersonal difficulties. As of yet, the role of identity disturbances of BPD in the context of interpersonal difficulties has not been addressed (Kerr et al., Reference Kerr, Finlayson-Short, McCutcheon, Beard and Chanen2015). Therefore, the current study aims to investigate the role of negative self-views of BPD on the affective and neural responses to negative as well as positive social feedback (SF).

Core features of interpersonal difficulties in BPD are a heightened sensitivity to rejection and altered social cognition (Gunderson, Reference Gunderson2007; Fertuck et al., Reference Fertuck, Jekal, Song, Wyman, Morris, Wilson, Brodsky and Stanley2009; Ha et al., Reference Ha, Sharp, Ensink, Fonagy and Cirino2013; Lis and Bohus, Reference Lis and Bohus2013; Sharp et al., Reference Sharp, Ha, Carbone, Kim, Perry, Williams and Fonagy2013). Studies have shown that patients with BPD perceive others’ faces as more untrustworthy and tend to anticipate threat rather than acceptance in social interactions (Daros et al., Reference Daros, Zakzanis and Ruocco2013; Fertuck et al., Reference Fertuck, Grinband and Stanley2013; Miano et al., Reference Miano, Fertuck, Arntz and Stanley2013; Nicol et al., Reference Nicol, Pope, Sprengelmeyer, Young and Hall2013; Deckers et al., Reference Deckers, Lobbestael, van Wingen, Kessels, Arntz and Egger2015). This lack of trust and expectation of rejection may relate to feeling less included during pleasant interactions (Staebler et al., Reference Staebler, Renneberg, Stopsack, Fiedler, Weiler and Roepke2011; Renneberg et al., Reference Renneberg, Herm, Hahn, Staebler, Lammers and Roepke2012) and showing less cooperative behavior (King-Casas et al., Reference King-Casas, Sharp, Lomax-Bream, Lohrenz, Fonagy and Montague2008; Saunders et al., Reference Saunders, Goodwin and Rogers2015). Altered responses in brain activation to negative emotional stimuli, i.e. increases in insula, posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) activation and decreases in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, indicate increased sensitivity to and difficulty in dealing with negative stimuli (Ruocco et al., Reference Ruocco, Amirthavasagam, Choi-Kain and McMain2013; Schulze et al., Reference Schulze, Schmahl and Niedtfeld2016). The tendency to mistrust others has been conceptualized as not only obstructing the building of relationships but also social learning from others about the self (Fonagy and Allison, Reference Fonagy and Allison2014; Fonagy et al., Reference Fonagy, Luyten and Allison2015; Bo et al., Reference Bo, Sharp, Fonagy and Kongerslev2017).

Accordingly, interpersonal difficulties are tightly linked to a disturbed view of the self, or say, identity disturbance (Bender and Skodol, Reference Bender and Skodol2007). Self-views are organized knowledge about various aspects of the self, which is related to but separable from the evaluative attitude toward the self (i.e. self-esteem) (Higgins, Reference Higgins1987; Campbell et al., Reference Campbell, Assanand and Di Paula2003; Swann and Brooks, Reference Swann and Brooks2012; Chiu et al., Reference Chiu, Chang and Hui2017). In interaction with others, individuals generally prefer feedback that is consistent with their self-views (Stinson et al., Reference Stinson, Logel, Holmes, Wood, Forest, Gaucher, Fitzsimons and Kath2010; van Schie et al., Reference van Schie, Chiu, Rombouts, Heiser and Elzinga2018). For example, we previously found that negative feedback that is inconsistent with someone's self-view has more detrimental impact on mood than consistent negative feedback (van Schie et al., Reference van Schie, Chiu, Rombouts, Heiser and Elzinga2018). Moreover, there are individual differences with regard to the impact of (in)consistent feedback: individuals with lower self-esteem seem to have more difficulty in dealing with (inconsistent) negative feedback (vanDellen et al., Reference vanDellen, Campbell, Hoyle and Bradfield2011) as indicated by lower mood and precuneus activity (van Schie et al., Reference van Schie, Chiu, Rombouts, Heiser and Elzinga2018). Therefore, the way we respond to feedback during social interactions is impacted by both valence and consistency of feedback with self-views as well as a global self-evaluation (Markus and Wurf, Reference Markus and Wurf1987; Chen et al., Reference Chen, English and Peng2006).

Disturbed self-views in BPD could be highly relevant in SF processing (Wilkinson-Ryan and Westen, Reference Wilkinson-Ryan and Westen2000; Zeigler-Hill, Reference Zeigler-Hill2006). On the one hand, literature on the preference for consistent information explains the ease with which BPD patients process negative information compared with positive information (Stinson et al., Reference Stinson, Logel, Holmes, Wood, Forest, Gaucher, Fitzsimons and Kath2010; Winter et al., Reference Winter, Herbert, Koplin, Schmahl, Bohus and Lis2015; Auerbach et al., Reference Auerbach, Tarlow, Bondy, Stewart, Aguirre, Kaplan, Yang and Pizzagalli2016; Korn et al., Reference Korn, La Rosee, Heekeren and Roepke2016). On the other hand, one might expect that negative feedback may yield relatively mild emotional responses in BPD, since it is consistent with their self-views, whereas positive information may not elicit much positive responses because of its inconsistency with their self-views (Higgins, Reference Higgins1987; Winter et al., Reference Winter, Herbert, Koplin, Schmahl, Bohus and Lis2015; Auerbach et al., Reference Auerbach, Tarlow, Bondy, Stewart, Aguirre, Kaplan, Yang and Pizzagalli2016; Korn et al., Reference Korn, La Rosee, Heekeren and Roepke2016). However, literature on rejection sensitivity suggests that BPD may be particularly prone to negative feedback regardless of its consistency with the self-view (Berenson et al., Reference Berenson, Downey, Rafaeli, Coifman and Paquin2011; Miano et al., Reference Miano, Fertuck, Arntz and Stanley2013).

The current study investigates how self-views of BPD influence the affective and neural responses to negative as well as positive SF. First, we aim to ascertain that self-views of BPD patients are more negative than healthy control (HC) participants. Next, we investigate how valence of the feedback and consistency with the self-views moderate the responses to SF. Importantly, as self-esteem influences responses to SF and BPD patients are characterized by low self-esteem (LSE) (Zeigler-Hill, Reference Zeigler-Hill2006; Brown, Reference Brown2010), we compare BPD patients not only with HC but also with a group of participants matched by the level of self-esteem. This LSE group allows for specifying the effect of disturbed self-views in BPD that goes beyond BPDs LSE.

Methods and materials

Participants

Participants (N = 80, all female, age M = 29.9, s.d. = 9.4, range = 18–54 years) consisted of patients with the BPD group (N = 26), HC group (N = 32), and LSE control group (N = 22). Participants included in the LSE group had a score lower than 18 on the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale [RSES, clinical cut-off for LSE (Schmitt and Allik, Reference Schmitt and Allik2005; Korrelboom, Reference Korrelboom2011)]. As expected, BPD patients (M = 11.28, s.d. = 6.1) had lower levels of self-esteem compared with HC (M = 23.75, s.d. = 3.2). The LSE (M = 12.73, s.d. = 2.9) were matched with BPD patients on the level of trait self-esteem. HC and LSE were included to resemble BPD in gender, age, education, and handedness to BPD, see Table 1. We distinguished three levels of education: high school level only, continued education on vocational level, and continued education on higher level. HC (but not LSE) reported higher education levels compared with BPD, see Table 1.

Table 1. Demographic data (N = 80)

MBO: middelbaar beroepsonderwijs (in Dutch); MDD: major depressive disorder.

a Group differences in the education level were assessed using a Kruskal–Wallis test followed by three Wilcoxon rank sum tests (Bonferroni corrected).

Exclusion criteria for all participants were incompatibility with a magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scanner, and usage of benzodiazepines (equivalent of >20 mg of oxazepam) or antipsychotics. Exclusion criteria for HC: any current Axis I or Axis II disorder. Exclusion criteria for LSE: current Axis I and II disorders were allowed, except for a diagnosis of BPD. HC and LSE participants were recruited from the general population where LSE were specifically targeted with adverts seeking insecure individuals. BPD patients were recruited from a mental health institution (GGZ Rivierduinen). Medication use, both for physical ailments and psychotropic medication, was taken into account with HC (N = 3, 9%), LSE (N = 4, 18%) and BPD (N = 11, 42%) reporting medication use (for specifications see online Supplementary Table S1). Two HC and one BPD participant were excluded from analyses because of scanner artifacts. One BPD participant was excluded because of neural abnormalities resulting in a final set of 80 participants.

Participants signed informed consent. The study was approved by the medical ethics committee of the Leiden University Medical Center (P12.249) and performed in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008 and the Dutch Medical Research Involving Human Subjects Act (WMO). Part of the HC and LSE data has been reported in van Schie et al. (Reference van Schie, Chiu, Rombouts, Heiser and Elzinga2018).

Social feedback task

Before performing the SF task in an MRI scanner, participants were introduced to another female participant who in reality was a confederate to the study. Together they were instructed that the participant would receive feedback of the confederate based on a personal interview. The interview, consisting of nine personal questions and three moral dilemmas, was held without the confederate, outside the scanner. The voice recorded interview was supposedly handed over to the confederate to provide feedback choosing from the provided list of trait words. The chosen trait words were presented on screen to the participant while in an MRI scanner. In reality, the SF was the same for each participant and consisted of 15 negative (e.g. arrogant), 15 intermediate (e.g. reserved) and 15 positive (e.g. happy) evaluative words, presented one by one (2500 ms) in random order (with no consecutive trials of the same valence). After each word, the participant was asked how she was feeling at that moment on a scale from really bad (1) to really good (4) using button boxes attached to the participants’ legs. We used an event-related design with a jittered interstimulus interval (M = 2004 ms, s.d. = 370) and self-paced responses to each trial. Outside the scanner, before debriefing, participants were asked to rate the feedback words on valence from very negative (−4) to very positive (4) with 0 indicating intermediate and on applicability to self from not at all applicable (1) to very much applicable (4). Finally, a manipulation check interview was held with indirect and direct questions to assess whether participants believed the cover story, see online Supplementary material. Most participants believed that the feedback was given by the confederate (HC: 91%, LSE: 95%, BPD: 96%) and groups did not differ in this respect, χ2(4) = 1.20, p = 0.879.

The SF task has been validated before in healthy participants and shown to affect mood, with a better mood after positive feedback and a worse mood after negative feedback especially when feedback is inapplicable. Moreover, a broad neural network is involved in the processing of SF including anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), insula, precuneus, PCC, and temporoparietal junction (TPJ) areas, with differential activation for negative, positive and more or less applicable feedback. For more details, see van Schie et al. (Reference van Schie, Chiu, Rombouts, Heiser and Elzinga2018).

Clinical measures

Axis I disorders

The MINI, a semi structured interview, was used to assess lifetime and current Axis-I disorders based on DSM-IV (First et al., Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams1997). BPD patients were assessed by a trained psychologist as part of their intake and diagnosis at the mental health institute. HC and LSE participants were assessed by a trained psychologist (C.v.S).

Axis II disorders

Axis II disorders in BPD patients were assessed using IPDE-IV (Loranger, Reference Loranger1999). Borderline symptom severity was assessed using the Borderline Personality Disorder-Severity Interview (BPD-SI) (Giesen-Bloo et al., Reference Giesen-Bloo, Wachters, Schouten and Arntz2010). HC and LSE were screened for personality disorders using the self-report standardized assessment of personality – abbreviated scale (SAPAS-SR) (Germans et al., Reference Germans, van Heck, Moran and Hodiamont2008). A score of four or greater indicates the likelihood of a personality disorder and led to exclusion in the case of HC. In the case of LSE, it led to a follow-up to exclude BPD using the SCID-II, a semi structured interview used to diagnose personality disorders (First et al., Reference First, Gibbon, Spitzer, Williams and Benjamin2000). Furthermore, the presence of borderline symptoms was assessed in both HC and LSE using the BPD items of the ‘Questionnaire for Personality traits’ [Vragenlijst voor Kenmerken van de Persoonlijkheid (VKP) (Duijsens et al., Reference Duijsens, Eurelings-Bontekoe and Diekstra1996)].

Trait self-esteem

The RSES measures the level of trait self-esteem using 10 items with a four point scale (Rosenberg, Reference Rosenberg1965). The Dutch translation has been well validated (Schmitt and Allik, Reference Schmitt and Allik2005; Frank et al., Reference Frank, De Raedt, Barbez and Rosseel2008). The reliability of the RSES was good (α = 0.92).

Procedure

Participants were screened over phone and with online questionnaires to assess compatibility with the MRI scanner (e.g. no metal objects in their body), Axis I disorders (First et al., Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams1997), Axis II disorders (First et al., Reference First, Gibbon, Spitzer, Williams and Benjamin2000; Germans et al., Reference Germans, van Heck, Moran and Hodiamont2008), BPD symptoms (Duijsens et al., Reference Duijsens, Eurelings-Bontekoe and Diekstra1996), handedness (van Strien, Reference van Strien1992) and medication use. After screening and inclusion two appointments were made. During the first appointment participants signed informed consent, filled in a demographic form and the RSES and were prepared for the MRI scan session. During the second appointment, they performed the SF task in the MRI scanner. After the experiment participants were debriefed of the set-up of the experiment and received a monetary reward of €30.

Data acquisition, preprocessing and analysis

The SF task was programmed in E-prime 2.0. Responses to the SF task were prepared for analysis using Excel 2010 and IBM SPSS statistics version 23. Preprocessing of fMRI data was performed in Feat v6.00 in FSL 5.0.7. Details on data acquisition and preprocessing can be found in the online Supplementary materials. For both affective and neural responses, the moderation of valence and applicability on group effect was tested by trial level modeling using multilevel analyses and parametric modulation. The reference category (i.e. intercept) of valence was set to ‘intermediate feedback’ enabling the contrasting of negative and positive feedback with intermediate feedback. Applicability ratings were recoded from values 1, 2, 3, 4 to values −3, −1, 1, 3, contrasting inapplicable to applicable feedback. Our main interest was to compare BPD with HC. To investigate the specificity of the results, BPD were also compared with LSE, making BPD the reference group.

Analysis of affect

Behavioral and self-report data were analyzed using R version 3.3.3 (R Core Team, 2013), packages: lme4 (linear mixed-effect modeling) and ggplot2 (for creating figures) (Wickham, Reference Wickham2009; Bates et al., Reference Bates, Maechler, Bolker and Walker2015). To test the significance of main and interaction effects, models increasing in complexity were compared using the χ2 test.

To ascertain whether self-views of BPD patients are more negative, applicability ratings for each feedback word for each participant were used as outcome, predefined valence categories were used on the first level and group membership on the second level. To investigate the impact of the SF on affect, mood ratings for each feedback word for each participant were used as outcome, while the predefined valence categories and individual applicability ratings were specified on the first level. The second level consisted of group membership. The two-way interaction effects of group by valence and group by applicability and the three-way interaction of group by valence by applicability were tested.

Analysis of fMRI

Analysis of fMRI data was performed in Feat v6.00 in FSL 5.0.7. On the individual level, an event related design was used for modeling both valence and applicability. For valence, the onset and duration of each feedback word per valence was specified, resulting in three regressors (Neg, Int and Pos valence). The parametric modulation analysis for applicability contained three regressors (one per valence) in which trials were weighted by the applicability ratings. The onset and duration of the mood question was modeled as a regressor but not of interest to the hypotheses. Six motion parameters indicating rotation and translation, and mean time series of white matter and cerebrospinal fluid were added as confound regressors (Birn et al., Reference Birn, Diamond, Smith and Bandettini2006; Mier et al., Reference Mier, Lis, Esslinger, Sauer, Hagenhoff, Ulferts, Gallhofer and Kirsch2013; Cheng and Puce, Reference Cheng and Puce2014). The bold response was convolved with the double-gamma HRF function. Six t-contrasts were formulated to contrast negative feedback to intermediate (Neg v. Int) and positive feedback (Neg v. Pos) and positive to intermediate feedback (Pos v. Int) in both directions. The general relation between applicability and neural activation was tested (Applicability), as well as the specific relation for applicability of negative, intermediate and positive feedback (interaction effects: Applicability × Neg, Applicability × Int and Applicability × Pos).

For group level inference, a mixed effects model with the FLAME1 method was used. The three groups with separate variance estimation were specified in the model. T-contrasts were formulated to compare BPD with HC and BPD with LSE in both directions on all lower level contrasts. This resulted in the test of two two-way interaction effects, i.e. group by valence and group by applicability and one three way-interaction effect, i.e. group by applicability by valence. Data were cluster corrected supported by the findings of Eklund et al. (Reference Eklund, Nichols and Knutsson2016) indicating that the FLAME1 method and cluster correction of Z > 2.3 and cluster p < 0.05 keep the amount of false positives within allowable limits.

For labeling peak voxels the following atlases were used: Harvard-Oxford structural atlas for cortical and subcortical regions (Frazier et al., Reference Frazier, Chiu, Breeze, Makris, Lange, Kennedy, Herbert, Bent, Koneru, Dieterich, Hodge, Rauch, Grant, Cohen, Seidman, Caviness and Biederman2005; Desikan et al., Reference Desikan, Segonne, Fischl, Quinn, Dickerson, Blacker, Buckner, Dale, Maguire, Hyman, Albert and Killiany2006; Makris et al., Reference Makris, Goldstein, Kennedy, Hodge, Caviness, Faraone, Tsuang and Seidman2006; Goldstein et al., Reference Goldstein, Seidman, Makris, Ahern, O'Brien, Caviness, Kennedy, Faraone and Tsuang2007), Mars connectivity-based parcellation for TPJ and inferior parietal lobe (IPL) areas (Mars et al., Reference Mars, Jbabdi, Sallet, O'Reilly, Croxson, Olivier, Noonan, Bergmann, Mitchell, Baxter, Behrens, Johansen-Berg, Tomassini, Miller and Rushworth2011, Reference Mars, Sallet, Schuffelgen, Jbabdi, Toni and Rushworth2012), the cerebellar atlas for cerebellum coordinates (Diedrichsen et al., Reference Diedrichsen, Balsters, Flavell, Cussans and Ramnani2009), and the Talairach Deamon labels for Brodmann areas (Lancaster et al., Reference Lancaster, Woldorff, Parsons, Liotti, Freitas, Rainey, Kochunov, Nickerson, Mikiten and Fox2000).

Results

Applicability of feedback to self-views

Applicability ratings were affected by a valence by group interaction [χ2(4) = 106.19, p < 0.001], see online Supplementary Tables S4 and S5 for model comparisons and parameters. Consistent with our hypothesis, BPD patients rated the intermediate (b = −0.40, s.e. = 0.16, t = −2.50) and especially negative feedback (b = −0.53, s.e. = 0.16, t = −3.36) as more applicable compared with HC, see Fig. 1a. Positive feedback was rated as less applicable by BPD compared with HC (b = 1.07, s.e. = 0.16, t = 6.74). Compared with LSE, BPD also rated negative feedback as more applicable (b = −0.43, s.e. = 0.17, t = −2.43) and positive feedback as less applicable (b = 0.63, s.e. = 0.18, t = 3.61) but did not differ in applicability of intermediate feedback (b = −0.15, s.e. = 0.18, t = −0.83). Moreover, using the valence ratings (i.e. degree of negativity or positivity), we found that all three groups rated the valence of the words in a similar way [χ2(2) = 2.4, p = 0.307], with negative and positive words being more emotional than intermediate words, see online Supplementary Tables S2 and S3. However, there was a trend for an interaction effect between valence and group [χ2(4) = 8.42, p = 0.077], which could indicate that negative feedback was rated slightly less negative by BPD than HC (b = −0.43, s.e. = 0.16, t = −2.69), see also online Supplementary Table S3 for model parameters.

Fig. 1. (a) Mean applicability ratings by group after negative, intermediate and positive feedback (error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals). (b) Illustration of mood ratings by group after negative, intermediate and positive feedback at the mean level of applicability of feedback. (c) Illustration of mean mood ratings by group after negative, intermediate and positive feedback for not to very applicable feedback. Applicability has a greater impact on mood during negative and intermediate feedback than positive feedback. Applicability has a greater impact on the mood of HC compared with BPD. Mood rating is rescaled to scores 1–4 for display purposes.

Affective responses

Mood was affected by group [χ2(2) = 11.4, p = 0.003] with BPD reporting a worse mood than HC overall (b = 0.81, s.e. = 0.19, t = 4.28), see Table 2 and online Supplementary Table S6. Valence moderated the group effect [χ2(4) = 39.89, p < 0.001]. BPD reported a worse mood after negative (b = −0.14, s.e. = 0.15, t = −0.95) and intermediate feedback (b = −0.81, s.e. = 0.19, t = 4.28) and similar mood after positive feedback (b = −0.49, s.e. = 0.13, t = −3.70) compared with HC, see Fig. 1b. Compared with LSE, BPD reported equal mood after intermediate (b = 0.19, s.e. = 0.21, t = 0.91) and positive feedback (b = 0.11, s.e. = 0.15, t = 0.75) but a better mood after negative feedback (b = −0.50, s.e. = 0.16, t = −3.10).

Table 2. Effect parameters of model predicting mood ratings by valence category (intermediate = reference), group (BPD = reference), and applicability of feedback and two-way interactions

Significance level (***<0.001, **<0.01, *<0.05, ^<0.10) based on χ2 test of model comparisons, see online Supplementary Table S6.

Applicability moderated the group effect as well [χ2(4) = 14.8, p = 0.005]. BPD mood ratings were less affected by applicability compared with HC (b = 0.07, s.e. = 0.03, t = 2.27), but did not differ in this respect from LSE (b = 0.01, s.e. = 0.03, t = 0.23), see Fig. 1c. There was no three-way interaction of valence by applicability by group [χ2(4) = 8.0, p = 0.090].

Neural responses

Groups differed in neural correlates of feedback valence, see Table 3 for clusters and peak voxelsFootnote Footnote 1. In response to negative feedback compared with positive feedback, HC showed stronger left precuneus activation, whereas BPD showed relatively low and equal precuneus activation for negative and positive feedback, see Fig. 2. In this precuneus cluster, LSE showed relatively high and equal activation for negative and positive feedback, albeit not significantly different from BPD, see Fig. 2. In response to positive compared with negative feedback, HC showed stronger right anterior TPJ activation, whereas BPD showed the reverse pattern, with stronger TPJ activation for negative feedback compared with positive feedback. Compared with LSE, BPD showed stronger left precuneus activation during negative compared with positive feedback, see Table 3 and Fig. 2. However, this cluster in the left precuneus did not overlap with the cluster found in comparison to HC. Groups did not differ in neural correlates of applicability. The three-way interaction of applicability by negative valence of BPD compared with HC in the motor cortex, superior parietal lobule and inferior parietal lobule is probably attributable to button press movements (Mars et al., Reference Mars, Jbabdi, Sallet, O'Reilly, Croxson, Olivier, Noonan, Bergmann, Mitchell, Baxter, Behrens, Johansen-Berg, Tomassini, Miller and Rushworth2011).

Fig. 2. Left: Clusters of neural activation indicating HC > BPD (blue) and BPD > LSE (orange). Right: Mean contrast values for the HC > BPD clusters (blue clusters) by group and contrast.

Table 3. Selected neural correlates for group comparisons on contrasts of valence and applicability of feedbacka, cluster corrected Z = 2.3, cluster p < 0.05

a Contrasts without any above threshold clusters are not reported in this table.

Exploratory findings

For exploratory purposes, we checked whether LSE differed in self-views from HC by rerunning the model with applicability ratings as an outcome but with HC set as a reference group instead of BPD. We found that despite lower self-esteem, LSE did not report that negative feedback was more applicable to them (b = 0.11, s.e. = 0.17, t = 0.65), neither was intermediate feedback (b = 0.26, s.e. = 0.17, t = 1.52). However, they did report that positive feedback is less applicable to them (b = −0.44, s.e. = 0.17, t = −2.64).

Confounds

To control for potential effects of whether the participant believed the SF paradigm (yes/no), medication status (on/off) and current depression comorbidity, we took this into account in additional affective and neural analyses. These confounds had no effects on the affective results.

Handedness was also taken into account in neural analyses. The stronger precuneus activation in HC compared with BPD found after negative feedback compared with positive feedback did not survive significance threshold after current depression or handedness was taken into account.

Discussion

The current study investigated how disturbed self-views related to interpersonal difficulties in patients with BPD by examining affective and neural responses to negative and positive SF which varied in applicability to the self-view. BPD patients reported more negative self-views compared with both the HC and LSE groups, confirming previous findings of a negative self-referential bias (Winter et al., Reference Winter, Herbert, Koplin, Schmahl, Bohus and Lis2015; Korn et al., Reference Korn, La Rosee, Heekeren and Roepke2016) and extending them by showing that distorted self-views in BPD cannot be attributed to LSE alone.

The consistency of self-views with SF was an important determinant of affective and neural responses. In general, more applicable feedback is related to a better mood especially during negative and intermediate feedback (van Schie et al., Reference van Schie, Chiu, Rombouts, Heiser and Elzinga2018). In BPD, however, the applicability of negative feedback did not reduce its negative impact resulting in a lower mood after negative feedback than HC. Thus, BPD responded similarly to all negative feedback regardless of consistency to the self. Responses in HC, on the other hand, vary with the consistency of the feedback with the self-view. Another study also found that BPD have trouble ignoring negative stimuli even when these are irrelevant (Domes et al., Reference Domes, Winter, Schnell, Vohs, Fast and Herpertz2006). BPD can perhaps not rely on their self-knowledge to judge the accuracy of the feedback and respond to negative feedback in a more rigid way (Fonagy et al., Reference Fonagy, Luyten and Allison2015).

Analyses of the neural responses seemed to corroborate these affective findings. In general, applicability of feedback relates to increased precuneus activation, especially during negative feedback (van Schie et al., Reference van Schie, Chiu, Rombouts, Heiser and Elzinga2018). The precuneus is implicated in putting self-relevant stimuli in autobiographical context (Northoff et al., Reference Northoff, Heinzel, de Greck, Bermpohl, Dobrowolny and Panksepp2006). Relating feedback to existing self-knowledge could be a way of critically evaluating negative feedback, reducing its impact on state affect when a specific trait is already incorporated in the self-view (vanDellen et al., Reference vanDellen, Campbell, Hoyle and Bradfield2011). Interestingly, BPD differed from HC in precuneus activation during negative v. positive feedback: BPD showed low recruitment and lack of differentiation in precuneus activity. HC in contrast, showed greater precuneus recruitment in response to negative compared with positive feedback. Where healthy individuals may benefit from using their (positive) self-views to evaluate the relevance of the negative feedback, BPD patients may have more difficulties in relying on their self-knowledge to critically evaluate the negative feedback (Fonagy and Allison, Reference Fonagy and Allison2014).

Individuals with low levels of self-esteem were more adversely affected by negative feedback than BPD. However, their self-views were less negative and more positive than BPD and only differed from HC in having less positive self-views (not more negative). It is therefore surprising that when negative feedback is equally applicable to HC and LSE, LSE responded with worse mood. Other research has suggested that when faced with threats to the self, e.g. through negative feedback, individuals with LSE have difficulties in recovering their state affect and give in to the threat, resulting in lower mood (vanDellen et al., Reference vanDellen, Campbell, Hoyle and Bradfield2011). When looking at the precuneus activation for LSE in this study, it seemed heightened for both negative and positive feedback, though this was not significantly different from BPD or HC. One possible explanation could be that LSE considered the relevance of negative (and positive) feedback to the self, given the precuneus activity, but were less able to disregard the negative feedback, resulting in even worse mood (vanDellen et al., Reference vanDellen, Campbell, Hoyle and Bradfield2011). This intriguing hypothesis should be addressed in future studies.

Against our expectation, positive feedback related to similar positive moods in BPD and HC. Therefore despite less positive self-views, BPD patients were able to benefit in mood from positive feedback. One other study found that BPD patients can positively update their self-view after positive feedback (Korn et al., Reference Korn, La Rosee, Heekeren and Roepke2016).

However, it is interesting that in terms of neural responses, BPD showed more right TPJ activation during negative compared with positive feedback, whereas the reverse pattern was observed in HC. The TPJ has been implicated in processes of reorienting attention as well as social cognition and self-other processing (Donaldson et al., Reference Donaldson, Rinehart and Enticott2015; Schurz et al., Reference Schurz, Tholen, Perner, Mars and Sallet2017). A stronger TPJ activation after negative compared with positive feedback could indicate that BPD have a stronger reorientation of attention toward negative compared with positive feedback than HC (Schurz et al., Reference Schurz, Tholen, Perner, Mars and Sallet2017). This would be in line with a more negative interpersonal evaluation bias previously found in BPD (Barnow et al., Reference Barnow, Stopsack, Grabe, Meinke, Spitzer, Kronmuller and Sieswerda2009). In addition, two interesting integrative perspectives on TPJ functionality relevant to this study are that the TPJ subserves the creation of a social context for understanding another person and that the TPJ plays a role in the differentiation between or blending of self- and other-representation. A possible explanation of the current TPJ finding could be that HC use the positive feedback to create a shared representation of self and other knowledge. Given the heightened TPJ activation in response to negative feedback patients with BPD may create this self-other representation after negative rather than positive feedback. This is in line with previous research indicating that BPD showed less emotional contagion for positive social signals (Matzke et al., Reference Matzke, Herpertz, Berger, Fleischer and Domes2014).

Though clinically self-other differentiation is very relevant to BPD pathology, future research is needed to clarify whether and how exactly TPJ activation relates to social interactions and maladaptive self-other differentiation in BPD. Of interest here, is that the characteristic interpersonal difficulties observed in BPD may be rooted in maladaptive self-other knowledge (Fertuck et al., Reference Fertuck, Grinband and Stanley2013; Miano et al., Reference Miano, Fertuck, Arntz and Stanley2013; Nicol et al., Reference Nicol, Pope, Sprengelmeyer, Young and Hall2013; Fonagy and Allison, Reference Fonagy and Allison2014; Deckers et al., Reference Deckers, Lobbestael, van Wingen, Kessels, Arntz and Egger2015; Bo et al., Reference Bo, Sharp, Fonagy and Kongerslev2017). Speculatively, SF processing observed in BPD not only supports negative interpersonal bias but also interferes with learning (positive aspects) about the self, maintaining disturbed self-views.

We like to mention a few strengths and limitations of the study that should be taken into account. In terms of strengths, first, the inclusion of the LSE group allowed us to decouple BPD pathology from LSE. Second, the SF paradigm designed for this study has high ecological validity, as it is credible and personally relevant. Third, using multilevel and parametric modulation analyses enabled us to model the idiosyncratic applicability ratings and investigate the effect of each feedback word on an individual. Herewith, we could take into account specific (disturbed) self-knowledge when receiving SF. Finally, by using both self-report ratings and fMRI activity, we integrated affective and neural responses (Eisenberger, Reference Eisenberger2015).

With respect to limitations, like many studies in this field, we only investigated female BPD patients and results may therefore not be generalizable to men. Second, the applicability ratings of the feedback words were measured after the SF task. The applicability ratings therefore may be influenced by having been evaluated by these words, though participants were instructed explicitly to rate the applicability regardless of the feedback of the confederate. Arousal of the feedback words was not directly assessed. Though responses seem specific to valence, as groups differed in their responses to positive and negative feedback, we cannot rule out the possibility that e.g. the BPD group perceived positive/negative feedback as more arousing than HC. Third, by comparing the BPD to a LSE control group we have taken an important confound into consideration. However, these groups may differ in other respects. Finally, it must be noted that the precuneus finding is affected by current depression and handedness and hence this finding should be interpreted with caution. Comorbidities are common in BPD, especially depression (Lieb et al., Reference Lieb, Zanarini, Schmahl, Linehan and Bohus2004) which in this sample was also quite prevalent (26%). Depression has been associated with a negative interpersonal style related to seeking verification of self-views which are often negative and at the same time relying on others for reassurance (Hames et al., Reference Hames, Hagan and Joiner2013). Noteworthily, the LSE group showed a similar rate of depression comorbidity (23%). Nevertheless, the LSE group showed different responses to the SF than BPD. The pattern of results that we found in BPD is therefore unlikely explained by MDD only. Still, a replication of these results with a depression group and larger sample size is recommended.

In conclusion, consistency of SF with self-views is a crucial determinant of the emotional and neural reactions to social interactions. Where healthy individuals may benefit from using their self-views to critically examine negative feedback, BPD patients seem to be adversely impacted by negative feedback regardless of the consistency with the self-view. Moreover, the increased TPJ activation after negative but not positive feedback may point to altered self-other differentiating processes in BPD, potentially obstructing learning positive information from others about themselves. Importantly, disturbed self-views in BPD patients go beyond low self-evaluation. The current findings suggest that clinically, one should pay special attention to how positive feedback (e.g. compliments) is received and how information based on interactions with others is processed. A lack of other representation during positive interactions may obstruct learning about the self from others, maintaining disturbed self-views and relations.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719000448.

Author ORCIDs

Charlotte C. van Schie, 0000-0003-0631-3289

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge the mental health institution Rivierduinen for their cooperation and their contribution in collecting data, in particular Bert van Klaveren and Doeska Birkhoff. Moreover, we thank all participants, especially the patients with BPD, for their effort and interest in participating in this study.

Financial support

This work was supported by the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO) with a VICI Grant (S.R., no. 016.130.677), (B.E., no. 453.15.006), and VIDI Grant (B.E., no. 016.085.353), by the Research Grant Council (C.C., no. 24618115), and the Research Committee Funding from The Chinese University of Hong Kong (C.C., no. 4052174).

Conflict of interest

None.

Footnotes

The notes appear after the main text.

1 Complete overview of clusters and peak voxels of all contrasts can be found in online Supplementary Table S7.

References

Auerbach, RP, Tarlow, N, Bondy, E, Stewart, JG, Aguirre, B, Kaplan, C, Yang, W and Pizzagalli, DA (2016) Electrocortical reactivity during self-referential processing in female youth with borderline personality disorder. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging 1, 335344.Google ScholarPubMed
Barnow, S, Stopsack, M, Grabe, HJ, Meinke, C, Spitzer, C, Kronmuller, K and Sieswerda, S (2009) Interpersonal evaluation bias in borderline personality disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy 47, 359365.10.1016/j.brat.2009.02.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D, Maechler, M, Bolker, BM and Walker, SC (2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67, 148.10.18637/jss.v067.i01CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bender, DS and Skodol, AE (2007) Borderline personality as a self-other representational disturbance. Journal of Personality Disorders 21, 500517.10.1521/pedi.2007.21.5.500CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berenson, KR, Downey, G, Rafaeli, E, Coifman, KG and Paquin, NL (2011) The rejection-rage contingency in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 120, 681690.10.1037/a0023335CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birn, RM, Diamond, JB, Smith, MA and Bandettini, PA (2006) Separating respiratory-variation-related fluctuations from neuronal-activity-related fluctuations in fMRI. Neuroimage 31, 15361548.10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.048CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bo, S, Sharp, C, Fonagy, P and Kongerslev, M (2017) Hypermentalizing, attachment, and epistemic trust in adolescent BPD: clinical illustrations. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment 8, 172182.10.1037/per0000161CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, JD (2010) High self-esteem buffers negative feedback: once more with feeling. Cognition & Emotion 24, 13891404.10.1080/02699930903504405CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, JD, Assanand, S and Di Paula, A (2003) The structure of self-concept and its relation to psychological adjustment. Journal of Personality 71, 115140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, S, English, T and Peng, K (2006) Self-verification and contextualized self-views. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 32, 930942.10.1177/0146167206287539CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheng, H and Puce, A (2014) Reducing respiratory effect in motion correction for EPI images with sequential slice acquisition order. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 227, 8389.10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.02.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiu, C-D, Chang, J-H and Hui, CM (2017) Self-concept integration and differentiation in subclinical individuals with dissociation proneness. Self and Identity 16, 664683.10.1080/15298868.2017.1296491CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daros, AR, Zakzanis, KK and Ruocco, AC (2013) Facial emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder. Psychological Medicine 43, 19531963.10.1017/S0033291712002607CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deckers, JW, Lobbestael, J, van Wingen, GA, Kessels, RP, Arntz, A and Egger, JI (2015) The influence of stress on social cognition in patients with borderline personality disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology 52, 119129.10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.11.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Desikan, RS, Segonne, F, Fischl, B, Quinn, BT, Dickerson, BC, Blacker, D, Buckner, RL, Dale, AM, Maguire, RP, Hyman, BT, Albert, MS and Killiany, RJ (2006) An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 31, 968980.10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diedrichsen, J, Balsters, JH, Flavell, J, Cussans, E and Ramnani, N (2009) A probabilistic MR atlas of the human cerebellum. Neuroimage 46, 3946.10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.045CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Domes, G, Winter, B, Schnell, K, Vohs, K, Fast, K and Herpertz, SC (2006) The influence of emotions on inhibitory functioning in borderline personality disorder. Psychological Medicine 36, 11631172.10.1017/S0033291706007756CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donaldson, PH, Rinehart, NJ and Enticott, PG (2015) Noninvasive stimulation of the temporoparietal junction: a systematic review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 55, 547572.10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.05.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duijsens, IJ, Eurelings-Bontekoe, EHM and Diekstra, RFW (1996) The VKP, a self-report instrument for DSM-III-R and ICD-10 personality disorders: construction and psychometric properties. Personality and Individual Differences 20, 171182.10.1016/0191-8869(95)00161-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberger, NI (2015) Meta-analytic evidence for the role of the anterior cingulate cortex in social pain. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 10, 12.10.1093/scan/nsu120CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eklund, A, Nichols, TE and Knutsson, H (2016) Cluster failure: why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 113, 79007905.10.1073/pnas.1602413113CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fertuck, EA, Grinband, J and Stanley, B (2013) Facial trust appraisal negatively biased in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research 207, 195202.10.1016/j.psychres.2013.01.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fertuck, EA, Jekal, A, Song, I, Wyman, B, Morris, MC, Wilson, ST, Brodsky, BS and Stanley, B (2009) Enhanced ‘reading the mind in the eyes’ in borderline personality disorder compared to healthy controls. Psychological Medicine 39, 19791988.10.1017/S003329170900600XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
First, MB, Spitzer, RL, Gibbon, M and Williams, JBW (1997) Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders (SCID-I/P). New York: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute.Google Scholar
First, MB, Gibbon, M, Spitzer, RL, Williams, JBM and Benjamin, LS (2000) Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders (SCID-II). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, Inc.Google Scholar
Fonagy, P and Allison, E (2014) The role of mentalizing and epistemic trust in the therapeutic relationship. Psychotherapy 51, 372380.10.1037/a0036505CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonagy, P, Luyten, P and Allison, E (2015) Epistemic petrification and the restoration of epistemic trust: a new conceptualization of borderline personality disorder and its psychosocial treatment. Journal of Personality Disorders 29, 575609.10.1521/pedi.2015.29.5.575CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, E, De Raedt, R, Barbez, C and Rosseel, Y (2008) Psychometric properties of the Dutch Rosenberg self-esteem scale. Psychologica Belgica 48, 2535.10.5334/pb-48-1-25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frazier, JA, Chiu, S, Breeze, JL, Makris, N, Lange, N, Kennedy, DN, Herbert, MR, Bent, EK, Koneru, VK, Dieterich, ME, Hodge, SM, Rauch, SL, Grant, PE, Cohen, BM, Seidman, LJ, Caviness, VS and Biederman, J (2005) Structural brain magnetic resonance imaging of limbic and thalamic volumes in pediatric bipolar disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 162, 12561265.10.1176/appi.ajp.162.7.1256CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Germans, S, van Heck, GL, Moran, P and Hodiamont, PPG (2008) The self-report standardized assessment of personality-abbreviated scale: preliminary results of a brief screening test for personality disorders. Personality and Mental Health 2, 7076.10.1002/pmh.34CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giesen-Bloo, JH, Wachters, LM, Schouten, E and Arntz, A (2010) The borderline personality disorder severity index-IV: psychometric evaluation and dimensional structure. Personality and Individual Differences 49, 136141.10.1016/j.paid.2010.03.023CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldstein, JM, Seidman, LJ, Makris, N, Ahern, T, O'Brien, LM, Caviness, VS Jr, Kennedy, DN, Faraone, SV and Tsuang, MT (2007) Hypothalamic abnormalities in schizophrenia: sex effects and genetic vulnerability. Biological Psychiatry 61, 935945.10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.06.027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunderson, JG (2007) Disturbed relationships as a phenotype for borderline personality disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry 164, 16371640.10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07071125CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ha, C, Sharp, C, Ensink, K, Fonagy, P and Cirino, P (2013) The measurement of reflective function in adolescents with and without borderline traits. Journal of Adolescence 36, 12151223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hames, JL, Hagan, CR and Joiner, TE (2013) Interpersonal processes in depression. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 9, 355377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, ET (1987) Self-Discrepancy: a theory relating self and affect. Psychological Review 94, 319340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorgensen, CR (2010) Invited essay: identity and borderline personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders 24, 344364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kerr, IB, Finlayson-Short, L, McCutcheon, LK, Beard, H and Chanen, AM (2015) The ‘Self’ and borderline personality disorder: conceptual and clinical considerations. Psychopathology 48, 339348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King-Casas, B, Sharp, C, Lomax-Bream, L, Lohrenz, T, Fonagy, P and Montague, PR (2008) The rupture and repair of cooperation in borderline personality disorder. Science 321, 806810.10.1126/science.1156902CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Korn, CW, La Rosee, L, Heekeren, HR and Roepke, S (2016) Social feedback processing in borderline personality disorder. Psychological Medicine 46, 575587.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Korrelboom, K (2011) COMET Voor Negatief Zelfbeeld. Houten: Bohn Stafleu van Loghum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lancaster, JL, Woldorff, MG, Parsons, LM, Liotti, M, Freitas, CS, Rainey, L, Kochunov, PV, Nickerson, D, Mikiten, SA and Fox, PT (2000) Automated Talairach atlas labels for functional brain mapping. Human Brain Mapping 10, 120131.3.0.CO;2-8>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lazarus, SA, Cheavens, JS, Festa, F and Zachary Rosenthal, M (2014) Interpersonal functioning in borderline personality disorder: a systematic review of behavioral and laboratory-based assessments. Clinical Psychology Review 34, 193205.10.1016/j.cpr.2014.01.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieb, K, Zanarini, MC, Schmahl, C, Linehan, MM and Bohus, M (2004) Borderline personality disorder. The Lancet 364, 453461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lis, S and Bohus, M (2013) Social interaction in borderline personality disorder. Current Psychiatric Reports 15, 338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loranger, AW (1999) IPDE: International Personality Disorder Examination: DSM-IV and ICD-10 Interviews. Odessa, FL: PARS Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Makris, N, Goldstein, JM, Kennedy, D, Hodge, SM, Caviness, VS, Faraone, SV, Tsuang, MT and Seidman, LJ (2006) Decreased volume of left and total anterior insular lobule in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 83, 155171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markus, H and Wurf, E (1987) The dynamic self-concept: a social psychological perspective. Annual Review of Psychology 38, 299337.10.1146/annurev.ps.38.020187.001503CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mars, RB, Jbabdi, S, Sallet, J, O'Reilly, JX, Croxson, PL, Olivier, E, Noonan, MP, Bergmann, C, Mitchell, AS, Baxter, MG, Behrens, TE, Johansen-Berg, H, Tomassini, V, Miller, KL and Rushworth, MF (2011) Diffusion-weighted imaging tractography-based parcellation of the human parietal cortex and comparison with human and macaque resting-state functional connectivity. The Journal of Neuroscience 31, 40874100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mars, RB, Sallet, J, Schuffelgen, U, Jbabdi, S, Toni, I and Rushworth, MF (2012) Connectivity-based subdivisions of the human right ‘temporoparietal junction area’: evidence for different areas participating in different cortical networks. Cerebral Cortex 22, 18941903.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matzke, B, Herpertz, SC, Berger, C, Fleischer, M and Domes, G (2014) Facial reactions during emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder: a facial electromyography study. Psychopathology 47, 101110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miano, A, Fertuck, EA, Arntz, A and Stanley, B (2013) Rejection sensitivity is a mediator between borderline personality disorder features and facial trust appraisal. Journal of Personality Disorders 27, 442456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mier, D, Lis, S, Esslinger, C, Sauer, C, Hagenhoff, M, Ulferts, J, Gallhofer, B and Kirsch, P (2013) Neuronal correlates of social cognition in borderline personality disorder. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 8, 531537.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicol, K, Pope, M, Sprengelmeyer, R, Young, AW and Hall, J (2013) Social judgement in borderline personality disorder. PLoS One 8, e73440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Northoff, G, Heinzel, A, de Greck, M, Bermpohl, F, Dobrowolny, H and Panksepp, J (2006) Self-referential processing in our brain – a meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. Neuroimage 31, 440457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Over, H (2016) The origins of belonging: social motivation in infants and young children. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 371, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team (2013) R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Renneberg, B, Herm, K, Hahn, A, Staebler, K, Lammers, CH and Roepke, S (2012) Perception of social participation in borderline personality disorder. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy 19, 473480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenberg, M (1965) Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.10.1515/9781400876136CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruocco, AC, Amirthavasagam, S, Choi-Kain, LW and McMain, SF (2013) Neural correlates of negative emotionality in borderline personality disorder: an activation-likelihood-estimation meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry 73, 153160.10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.07.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saunders, KE, Goodwin, GM and Rogers, RD (2015) Borderline personality disorder, but not euthymic bipolar disorder, is associated with a failure to sustain reciprocal cooperative behaviour: implications for spectrum models of mood disorders. Psychological Medicine 45, 15911600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmitt, DP and Allik, J (2005) Simultaneous administration of the Rosenberg self-esteem scale in 53 nations: exploring the universal and culture-specific features of global self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 89, 623642.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulze, L, Schmahl, C and Niedtfeld, I (2016) Neural correlates of disturbed emotion processing in borderline personality disorder: a multimodal meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry 79, 97106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schurz, M, Tholen, MG, Perner, J, Mars, RB and Sallet, J (2017) Specifying the brain anatomy underlying temporo-parietal junction activations for theory of mind: a review using probabilistic atlases from different imaging modalities. Human Brain Mapping 38, 47884805.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharp, C, Ha, C, Carbone, C, Kim, S, Perry, K, Williams, L and Fonagy, P (2013) Hypermentalizing in adolescent inpatients: treatment effects and association with borderline traits. Journal of Personality Disorders 27, 318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Staebler, K, Renneberg, B, Stopsack, M, Fiedler, P, Weiler, M and Roepke, S (2011) Facial emotional expression in reaction to social exclusion in borderline personality disorder. Psychological Medicine 41, 19291938.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stinson, DA, Logel, C, Holmes, JG, Wood, JV, Forest, AL, Gaucher, D, Fitzsimons, GM and Kath, J (2010) The regulatory function of self-esteem: testing the epistemic and acceptance signaling systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 99, 9931013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swann, WB Jr and Brooks, M (2012) Why threats trigger compensatory reactions: The need for coherence and quest for self-verification. Social Cognition 30, 758777.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Schie, CC, Chiu, CD, Rombouts, SARB, Heiser, WJ and Elzinga, BM (2018) When compliments don't hit but critiques do: an fMRI study into self-esteem and self-knowledge in processing social feedback. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 13, 404417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Strien, JW (1992) Classificatie van links- en rechtshandige proefpersonen. Nederlands Tijdschrift voor de Psychologie en Haar Grensgebieden 47, 8892.Google Scholar
vanDellen, MR, Campbell, WK, Hoyle, RH and Bradfield, EK (2011) Compensating, resisting, and breaking: a meta-analytic examination of reactions to self-esteem threat. Personality and Social Psychology Review 15, 5174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wickham, H (2009) ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. New York: Springer-Verlag.10.1007/978-0-387-98141-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilkinson-Ryan, T and Westen, D (2000). Disturbance in borderline personality disorder: an empirical investigation. American Journal of Psychiatry 157, 528541.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winter, D, Herbert, C, Koplin, K, Schmahl, C, Bohus, M and Lis, S (2015) Negative evaluation bias for positive self-referential information in borderline personality disorder. PLoS One 10, e0117083.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeigler-Hill, V (2006) Borderline personality features: instability of self-esteem and affect. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 26, 668687.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic data (N = 80)

Figure 1

Fig. 1. (a) Mean applicability ratings by group after negative, intermediate and positive feedback (error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals). (b) Illustration of mood ratings by group after negative, intermediate and positive feedback at the mean level of applicability of feedback. (c) Illustration of mean mood ratings by group after negative, intermediate and positive feedback for not to very applicable feedback. Applicability has a greater impact on mood during negative and intermediate feedback than positive feedback. Applicability has a greater impact on the mood of HC compared with BPD. Mood rating is rescaled to scores 1–4 for display purposes.

Figure 2

Table 2. Effect parameters of model predicting mood ratings by valence category (intermediate = reference), group (BPD = reference), and applicability of feedback and two-way interactions

Figure 3

Fig. 2. Left: Clusters of neural activation indicating HC > BPD (blue) and BPD > LSE (orange). Right: Mean contrast values for the HC > BPD clusters (blue clusters) by group and contrast.

Figure 4

Table 3. Selected neural correlates for group comparisons on contrasts of valence and applicability of feedbacka, cluster corrected Z = 2.3, cluster p < 0.05

Supplementary material: PDF

van Schie et al. supplementary material

van Schie et al. supplementary material 1

Download van Schie et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 261.2 KB