Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T14:46:23.446Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Theory of mind deficits for processing counterfactual information in persons with chronic schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 August 2008

R. S. Kern*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, Los Angeles, CA, USA Department of Veterans Affairs VISN 22 Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center, Los Angeles, CA, USA
M. F. Green
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, Los Angeles, CA, USA Department of Veterans Affairs VISN 22 Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center, Los Angeles, CA, USA
A. P. Fiske
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology; Center for Culture, Brain, and Development; and Center for Behavior, Evolution, and Culture, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
K. S. Kee
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, Los Angeles, CA, USA Department of Veterans Affairs VISN 22 Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center, Los Angeles, CA, USA California State University, Channel Islands, CA, USA
J. Lee
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, Los Angeles, CA, USA Department of Veterans Affairs VISN 22 Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center, Los Angeles, CA, USA
M. J. Sergi
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, Los Angeles, CA, USA Department of Veterans Affairs VISN 22 Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center, Los Angeles, CA, USA California State University, Northridge, CA, USA
W. P. Horan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, Los Angeles, CA, USA Department of Veterans Affairs VISN 22 Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center, Los Angeles, CA, USA
K. L. Subotnik
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, Los Angeles, CA, USA
C. A. Sugar
Affiliation:
Department of Biostatistics, UCLA School of Public Health, Los Angeles, CA, USA
K. H. Nuechterlein
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, Los Angeles, CA, USA Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
*
*Address for correspondence: R. S. Kern, Ph.D., VA Greater Los Angeles Healthcare System (MIRECC 210 A), Building 210, Room 116, 11301 Wilshire Blvd, Los Angeles, CA 90073, USA. (Email: rkern@ucla.edu)

Abstract

Background

Interpersonal communication problems are common among persons with schizophrenia and may be linked, in part, to deficits in theory of mind (ToM), the ability to accurately perceive the attitudes, beliefs and intentions of others. Particular difficulties might be expected in the processing of counterfactual information such as sarcasm or lies.

Method

The present study included 50 schizophrenia or schizo-affective out-patients and 44 demographically comparable healthy adults who were administered Part III of The Awareness of Social Inference Test (TASIT; a measure assessing comprehension of sarcasm versus lies) as well as measures of positive and negative symptoms and community functioning.

Results

TASIT data were analyzed using a 2 (group: patients versus healthy adults)×2 (condition: sarcasm versus lie) repeated-measures ANOVA. The results show significant effects for group, condition, and the group×condition interaction. Compared to controls, patients performed significantly worse on sarcasm but not lie scenes. Within-group contrasts showed that patients performed significantly worse on sarcasm versus lie scenes; controls performed comparably on both. In patients, performance on TASIT showed a significant correlation with positive, but not negative, symptoms. The group and interaction effects remained significant when rerun with a subset of patients with low-level positive symptoms. The findings for a relationship between TASIT performance and community functioning were essentially negative.

Conclusions

The findings replicate a prior demonstration of difficulty in the comprehension of sarcasm using a different test, but are not consistent with previous studies showing global ToM deficits in schizophrenia.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © 2008 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Andreasen, NC (1984 a). Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS). University of Iowa: Iowa City, IA.Google Scholar
Andreasen, NC (1984 b). Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS). University of Iowa: Iowa City, IA.Google Scholar
Auther, AM, Smith, CW, Cornblatt, BA (2006). Global Functioning Scale: Social (GFS: Social). Zucker Hillside Hospital: Glen Oaks, NY.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S (1995). Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind. Bradford/MIT Press: Cambridge, MA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S, Leslie, AM, Frith, U (1985). Does the autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’? Cognition 21, 3746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bibby, H, McDonald, S (2004). Theory of mind after traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychologia 43, 99104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blanchard, JJ, Cohen, AS (2006). The structure of negative symptoms within schizophrenia: implications for assessment. Schizophrenia Bulletin 32, 238245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brune, M (2003). Theory of mind and the role of IQ in chronic disorganized schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 60, 5764.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brune, M (2005). ‘Theory of mind’ in schizophrenia: a review of the literature. Schizophrenia Bulletin 31, 2142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, LJ, Chapman, JP (1978). The measurement of differential deficit. Journal of Psychiatric Research 14, 303311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corcoran, R (2001). Theory of mind and schizophrenia. In Social Cognition and Schizophrenia (ed. Corrigan, P. W. and Penn, D. L.), pp. 149174. American Psychological Association: Washington, DC.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corcoran, R, Cahill, C, Frith, CD (1997). The appreciation of visual jokes in people with schizophrenia: a study of ‘mentalizing’ ability. Schizophrenia Research 24, 319327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corcoran, R, Mercer, G, Frith, CD (1995). Schizophrenia, symptomatology and social inference: investigating ‘theory of mind’ in people with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 17, 513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornblatt, BA, Auther, AM, Niendam, T, Smith, CW, Zingerg, J, Bearden, CE, Cannon, TD (2007). Preliminary findings for two new measures of social and role functioning in the prodromal phase of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 33, 688702.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Couture, SM, Roberts, DL, Penn, DL (2006). The functional significance of social cognition in schizophrenia: a review. Schizophrenia Bulletin 32 (Suppl. 1), S44S63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, M, Purvis, K, Barnes, MA, Wilkinson, M, Winner, E (2001). Understanding of literal truth, ironic criticism, and deceptive praise following childhood head injury. Brain Language 78, 116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donahoe, CP, Carter, MJ, Bloem, WD, Hirsch, GL, Laasi, N, Wallace, CJ (1990). Assessment of interpersonal problem-solving skills. Psychiatry 53, 329339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doody, GA, Gotz, M, Johnstone, EC, Frith, CD, Owens, DG (1998). Theory of mind and psychoses. Psychological Medicine 28, 397405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P, Friesen, WV (1976). Pictures of Facial Affect. Consulting Psychological Press: Palo Alto, CA.Google Scholar
First, M, Spitzer, R, Gibbon, M, Williams, J (1996). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Patient Edition (SCID-I/P, Version 2.0). American Psychiatric Press Inc.: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Frith, CD (1992). The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia. Psychology Press: Hove, UK.Google Scholar
Frith, CD, Corcoran, R (1996). Exploring ‘theory of mind’ in people with schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine 26, 521530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Happe, FGE (1993). Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: a test of relevance theory. Cognition 48, 101119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrington, L, Siegert, RJ, McClure, J (2005). Theory of mind in schizophrenia: a critical review. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 10, 249286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herold, R, Tenyi, T, Lenard, K, Trixler, M (2002). Theory of mind deficit in people with schizophrenia during remission. Psychological Medicine 32, 11251129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inoue, Y, Yamada, K, Hirano, M, Shinohara, M, Tamaoki, T, Iguchi, H, Tonooka, Y, Kanba, S (2006). Impairment of theory of mind in patients in remission following first episode of schizophrenia. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 256, 326328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jahshan, CS, Sergi, MJ (2007). Theory of mind, neurocognition, and functional status in schizotypy. Schizophrenia Research 89, 278286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janssen, I, Krabbendam, L, Jolles, J, van Os, J (2003). Alterations in theory of mind in patients with schizophrenia and non-psychotic relatives. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 108, 110117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langdon, R, Coltheart, M, Ward, PB, Catts, SV (2002). Disturbed communication in schizophrenia: the role of poor pragmatics and poor mind-reading. Psychological Medicine 32, 12731284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leitman, D, Ziwich, R, Pasternak, R, Javitt, DC (2006). Theory of Mind (ToM) and counterfactuality deficits in schizophrenia: misperception or misinterpretation? Psychological Medicine 36, 10751083.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leslie, A (1987). Pretence and representation: the origins of ‘theory of mind’. Psychological Review 94, 412426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leslie, AM, Friedman, O, German, TP (2004). Core mechanisms in ‘theory of mind’. Trends in Cognitive Science 8, 528533.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marjoram, D, Gardner, C, Burns, J, Miller, P, Lawrie, SM, Johnstone, EC (2005 a). Symptomatology and social inference: a theory of mind study of schizophrenia and psychotic affective disorder. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 10, 347359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marjoram, D, Tansley, H, Miller, P, MacIntyre, D, Cunningham, Owens DG, Johnstone, EC, Lawrie, S (2005 b). A theory of mind investigation into the appreciation of visual jokes in schizophrenia. BMC Psychiatry 5, 12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mazza, M, De Risio, A, Surian, L, Roncone, R, Casacchia, M (2001). Selective impairments of theory of mind in people with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 47, 299308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCabe, R, Leudar, I, Antaki, C (2004). Do people with schizophrenia display theory of mind deficits in clinical interactions? Psychological Medicine 34, 401412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDonald, S (2000). Neuropsychological studies of sarcasm. Metaphor and Symbol 15, 8598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, S, Bornhofen, C, Shum, D, Long, E, Saunders, C, Neulinger, K (2006). Reliability and validity of The Awareness of Social Inference Test (TASIT): a clinical test of social perception. Disability and Rehabilitation 28, 15291542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDonald, S, Flanagan, S (2004). Social perception deficits after traumatic brain injury: the interaction between emotion recognition, mentalising ability and social communication. Neuropsychology 18, 572579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, S, Flanagan, S, Martin, I, Saunders, C (2004). The ecological validity of TASIT: a test of social perception. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation 14, 285302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, S, Flanagan, S, Rollins, J, Kinch, J (2003). TASIT: a new clinical tool for assessing social perception after traumatic brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma and Rehabilitation 18, 219238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McPheeters, HL (1984). Statewide mental health outcome evaluation: a perspective of two southern states. Community Mental Health Journal 20, 4455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchley, NJ, Barber, J, Gray, JM, Brooks, DN, Livingston, MG (1998). Comprehension of irony in schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 3, 127138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mo, S, Su, Y, Chan, RCK, Liu, J (2008). Comprehension of metaphor and irony in schizophrenia during remission: the role of theory of mind and IQ. Psychiatry Research 157, 2129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Niendam, TA, Bearden, CE, Johnson, JK, Cannon, TD (2006). Global Functioning Scale: Role (GFS: Role). University of California, Los Angeles: Los Angeles, CA.Google Scholar
Orbelo, DM, Grim, MA, Talbott, RE, Ross, ED (2005). Impaired comprehension of affective prosody in elderly subjects is not predicted by age-related hearing loss or age-related cognitive decline. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology 18, 2532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pilowsky, T, Yirmiya, N, Arbelle, S, Mozes, T (2000). Theory of mind abilities of children with schizophrenia, children with autism, and normally developing children. Schizophrenia Research 42, 145155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Premack, D, Woodruff, G (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a ‘theory of mind’? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4, 515526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Randall, F, Corcoran, R, Day, JC, Bentall, RP (2003). Attention, theory of mind, and causal attributions in people with persecutory delusions: a preliminary investigation. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 8, 287294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reichenberg, A, Weiser, M, Rabinowitz, J, Caspi, A, Schmeidler, J, Mark, M, Kaplan, Z, Davidson, M (2002). A population-based cohort study of premorbid intellectual, language, and behavioral functioning in patients with schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, and nonpsychotic bipolar disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 159, 20272035.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roncone, R, Falloon, IR, Mazza, M, De Risio, A, Pollice, R, Necozione, S, Morosini, P, Casacchia, M (2002). Is theory of mind in schizophrenia more strongly associated with clinical and social functioning than with neurocognitive deficits? Psychopathology 35, 280288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sperber, D, Wilson, D (2002). Pragmatics, modularity and mind-reading. Mind and Language 17, 323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stein, LI, Test, MA (1980). Alternatives to mental hospital treatment: I. Conceptual model treatment program and clinical evaluation. Archives of General Psychiatry 37, 392397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sprong, M, Schothorst, P, Vos, E, Hox, J, van Engeland, H (2007). Theory of mind in schizophrenia: meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry 191, 513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D (2001). Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR). The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX.Google Scholar
Weiser, M, Reichenberg, A, Rabinowitz, J, Nahon, D, Kravitz, E, Lubin, G, Knobler, HY, Davidson, M, Noy, S (2007). Impaired reading and mathematical abilities in male adolescents with average or above general intellectual abilities are associated with comorbid and future psychopathology. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 195, 883890.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed