Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T12:04:25.315Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A trans-diagnostic perspective on obsessive-compulsive disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 March 2017

C. M. Gillan*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, USA Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
N. A. Fineberg
Affiliation:
National Obsessive Compulsive Disorders Specialist Service, Hertfordshire Partnership NHS University Foundation Trust, UK Department of Postgraduate Medicine, University of Hertfordshire, Hatfield, UK
T. W. Robbins
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
*
*Address for correspondence: Dr C. M. Gillan, Department of Psychology, New York University, 6 Washington Place, New York, NY 10003, USA. (Email: Claire.gillan@gmail.com)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Progress in understanding the underlying neurobiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) has stalled in part because of the considerable problem of heterogeneity within this diagnostic category, and homogeneity across other putatively discrete, diagnostic categories. As psychiatry begins to recognize the shortcomings of a purely symptom-based psychiatric nosology, new data-driven approaches have begun to be utilized with the goal of solving these problems: specifically, identifying trans-diagnostic aspects of clinical phenomenology based on their association with neurobiological processes. In this review, we describe key methodological approaches to understanding OCD from this perspective and highlight the candidate traits that have already been identified as a result of these early endeavours. We discuss how important inferences can be made from pre-existing case-control studies as well as showcasing newer methods that rely on large general population datasets to refine and validate psychiatric phenotypes. As exemplars, we take ‘compulsivity’ and ‘anxiety’, putatively trans-diagnostic symptom dimensions that are linked to well-defined neurobiological mechanisms, goal-directed learning and error-related negativity, respectively. We argue that the identification of biologically valid, more homogeneous, dimensions such as these provides renewed optimism for identifying reliable genetic contributions to OCD and other disorders, improving animal models and critically, provides a path towards a future of more targeted psychiatric treatments.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017

Introduction

The mainstay of psychiatric research is the case-control methodology whereby a group of individuals meeting the criteria for a disorder defined by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edn (DSM-5; APA, 2013) is compared to a group of healthy controls. Although the DSM has proven extremely useful in establishing a reliable psychiatric nosology that can be applied in a systematic way across centres (Regier et al. Reference Regier, Narrow, Clarke, Kraemer, Kuramoto, Kuhl and Kupfer2013), and that has led to the widespread development of evidence-based treatments, it has been suggested that shortcomings in the use of these categories for basic research are at least partly responsible for the lack of specific and robust associations between psychopathology and underlying neurobiological processes (Hyman, Reference Hyman2007; Sanislow et al. Reference Sanislow, Pine, Quinn, Kozak, Garvey, Heinssen, Wang and Cuthbert2010).

Obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), is a chronic, costly and disabling brain disorder for which existing treatments usually produce disappointing outcomes (Fineberg et al. Reference Fineberg, Chamberlain, Goudriaan, Stein, Vanderschuren, Gillan, Shekar, Gorwood, Voon, Morein-Zamir, Denys, Sahakian, Moeller, Robbins and Potenza2014). It is defined in DSM-5 (APA, 2013) by the presence of obsessions and/or compulsions that are time consuming, distressing or disabling. Obsessions are repetitive thoughts, urges or images that are intrusive and unwanted, and that in most individuals cause anxiety or distress, for example recurrent thoughts about accidental death. They are associated with attempts by the individual to suppress or neutralize them with a compulsion. Compulsions are repetitive mental or overt acts that are experienced as being urge-driven either in response to an obsession or according to a rule that must be rigidly applied, and that are aimed at preventing or reducing anxiety or distress or preventing a feared event from happening. However, the compulsion either is not connected in a realistic way with the outcome it is designed to prevent, or is clearly excessive, such as a driver retracing their route to check for signs of an accident after experiencing a minor bump in the road.

DSM-5 acknowledges that these symptoms are not unique to OCD; outlining 14 other disorders (or disorder classes) whose symptoms also broadly fit these criteria, which the clinician must consider in the context of differential diagnosis. These disorders include other members of the obsessive-compulsive and related disorders family, in which obsessions and compulsions are focused either on bodily appearance (body dysmorphic disorder), grooming (trichotillomania and skin picking disorder) or the acquisition of and/or inability to discard personal items (hoarding disorder). Eating disorders are also characterized by behaviours that resemble obsessions and compulsions focused around body size and weight, whereas patients with generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) and depressive disorders experience intrusive, distressing ruminations that are similar to obsessions but are respectively focused on future or past mishaps. In the case of addictive disorders, the urge-driven addictive behaviours become increasingly rigid, stereotyped and compulsive over time, as their function changes from reward-seeking to preventing or neutralizing the distress associated with craving and withdrawal (Everitt et al. Reference Everitt, Dickinson and Robbins2001).

In OCD, the level of insight varies considerably from good to absent both between patients and within the same patient over time. Poor insight has been associated with treatment resistance (Jacob et al. Reference Jacob, Larson and Storch2014). Intrusive, irrational thoughts frequently accompany schizophrenia spectrum and other psychotic disorders, and it can be difficult to distinguish obsessions from delusions, based on phenomenology alone. Moreover, approximately 30% of schizophrenia cases report obsessive-compulsive symptomatology and 14% have been found to have OCD (Swets et al. Reference Swets, Dekker, van Emmerik-van Oortmerssen, Smid, Smit, de Haan and Schoevers2014). Some other individuals develop OCD symptoms as a result of environmental factors such as Paediatric Autoimmune Neuropsychiatric Disorder Associated with Streptococcus (PANDAS; Snider & Swedo, Reference Snider and Swedo2003).

Even after careful attempts at differential diagnosis of OCD, co-morbidity is present in the majority of cases (Ruscio et al. Reference Ruscio, Stein, Chiu and Kessler2010). Other psychiatric disorders, including anxiety, somatoform, impulse control, trauma-related, substance use, and personality disorders, are very common among patients with OCD and can affect adherence and response to treatment (Pallanti et al. Reference Pallanti, Grassi, Sarrecchia, Cantisani and Pellegrini2011; Torres et al. Reference Torres, Fontenelle, Shavitt, Ferrão, do Rosário, Storch and Miguel2016). Approximately three quarters of individuals with OCD in an epidemiological sample experienced either an anxiety disorder or an affective disorder (Fineberg et al. Reference Fineberg, Hengartner, Bergbaum, Gale, Rössler and Angst2013a ). Most forms of co-morbidity increase distress and impact negatively on family and work relationships, however, disorder-specific effects have also been observed. For example, agoraphobia and GAD were associated with increased OCD severity; bipolar disorder was associated with suicidal acts and panic disorder increased treatment-seeking behaviour (Fineberg et al. Reference Fineberg, Hengartner, Bergbaum, Gale, Rössler and Angst2013a ). In the case of selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitor (SSRI)-resistant OCD, a preferential effect for treatment with dopamine antagonists was seen for those with co-morbid tics (Bloch et al. Reference Bloch, Landeros-Weisenberger, Kelmendi, Coric, Bracken and Leckman2006). These findings indicate quite clearly that these and other disorders are not discrete entities, and that the lines between different DSM diagnoses are often blurred (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1. Data from 706 obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) patients re-plotted with permission from Nestadt and colleagues (2009). Disorders assessed were obsessive-compulsive personality disorder (OCPD), generalized anxiety disorder (GAD), major depression (recurrent), tics, pathological skin picking, separation anxiety disorder, pathological nail biting, panic disorder, alcohol dependence, body dysmorphic disorder, substance dependence, trichotillomania, hypochondriasis, bipolar disorder. (a) Co-morbidity rates in a sample of OCD cases – 87.3% of patients met the criteria for another DSM diagnosis (note: this figure is probably higher as only select diagnoses were assessed). (b) The prevalence of specific co-morbid disorders within the OCD cohort.

The issue of homogeneity across diagnostic categories is coupled with a related problem, heterogeneity within diagnostic categories. In the case of OCD, there is a mosaic of diverse phenomenological manifestations (Torres, Reference Torres2016). A number of authors have attempted to group these phenomenological variables into a smaller number of relatively homogeneous, temporally stable symptom dimensions, using factor analysis of large symptom datasets. This has produced a multidimensional model of OCD that includes symmetry/ordering, hoarding, contamination/cleaning, and obsessions/checking) (Mataix-Cols et al. Reference Mataix-Cols, Rosario-Campos and Leckman2005; van den Heuvel et al. Reference van den Heuvel, Remijnse, Mataix-Cols, Vrenken, Groenewegen, Uylings, van Balkom and Veltman2009). The authors propose these symptom dimensions should be understood as a spectrum of potentially overlapping syndromes that may (1) co-exist in any patient, (2) be continuous with normal obsessive-compulsive phenomena, and (3) extend beyond OCD. Although twin studies suggest there might be a genetic influence on dimension-specific risk (van Grootheest et al. Reference van Grootheest, Boomsma, Hettema and Kendler2008; Iervolino et al. Reference Iervolino, Rijsdijk, Cherkas, Fullana and Mataix-Cols2011), the neurobiological validity of most of these factors has been difficult to demonstrate, owing to, for example, the lack of cross-study consistency of brain structural abnormalities associated with the various subtypes (Pujol et al. Reference Pujol, Soriano-Mas, Alonso, Cardoner, Menchon, Deus and Vallejo2004; van den Heuvel et al. Reference van den Heuvel, Remijnse, Mataix-Cols, Vrenken, Groenewegen, Uylings, van Balkom and Veltman2009; Alvarenga et al. Reference Alvarenga, do Rosário, Batistuzzo, Diniz, Shavitt, Duran, Dougherty, Bressan, Miguel and Hoexter2012; de Wit et al. Reference de Wit, Alonso, Schweren, Mataix-Cols, Lochner, Menchón, Stein, Fouche, Soriano-Mas, Sato, Hoexter, Denys, Nakamae, Nishida, Kwon, Jang, Busatto, Cardoner, Cath, Fukui, Jung, Kim, Miguel, Narumoto, Phillips, Pujol, Remijnse, Sakai, Shin, Yamada, Veltman and van den Heuvel2014). The hoarding dimension has proven very useful however, showing a consistently worse response to treatment than other forms of OCD (Mataix-Cols et al. Reference Mataix-Cols, Rauch, Manzo, Jenike and Baer1999; Cullen et al. Reference Cullen, Brown, Riddle, Grados, Bienvenu, Hoehn-Saric, Shugart, Liang, Samuels and Nestadt2007). This particularly impactful finding has provoked a change in DSM-5, such that hoarding symptoms no longer exclusively contribute to a diagnosis of OCD, and are additionally captured by a separate category, ‘hoarding disorder’. Unfortunately, hoarding symptoms do not account for all of the heterogeneity of treatment response in OCD, where for example one meta-analysis determined that the number needed to treat varied from 6–12 for SSRIs compared to placebo (Soomro et al. Reference Soomro, Altman, Rajagopal and Oakley-Browne2008).

The noisy response to treatment that remains to be bridged is likely to be a result of mechanistic heterogeneity within the OCD population that we cannot easily identify by analysing symptoms alone. In response, some researchers have called for a shift away from the use of DSM categories, in favour of establishing new and biologically relevant trans-diagnostic traits that may play an important role in multiple disorders, as we currently define them (Robbins et al. Reference Robbins, Gillan, Smith, de Wit and Ersche2012; Cuthbert & Kozak, Reference Cuthbert and Kozak2013). The goal of such an enterprise is to permit evidence-based drug discovery, the development of robust animal models, and eventually realize a future of precision medicine in psychiatry. In a wider context, improved nosology based on more objective tests and criteria would lead to more homogeneous populations of patients for clinical trials, more accurate phenotypes for psychiatric genetics, and the potential for detection of early vulnerability, allowing for early therapeutic interventions that prevent progression to chronic illness.

The National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) has been an important leader in this regard, launching its Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) initiative (Insel et al. Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn, Sanislow and Wang2010), a framework based on the premise that clinical constructs should be defined (and therefore investigated) on the basis of their neurobiological validity. This review will outline such a trans-diagnostic framework for understanding the DSM-5 diagnosis of OCD, outlining putative traits that link genes, molecules, cells, circuits, physiology, behaviour, self-reports and paradigms to clinical phenomenology. The goal is to highlight how a trans-diagnostic approach can shed light on some key puzzles in OCD research, and in so doing showcase the value that can be derived from existing case-control investigations and how this can inform newer attempts at trans-diagnostic psychiatry. Structurally, we will draw heavily on the NIMH's RDoC concepts and criteria, but acknowledge that other new initiatives such as the Roadmap for Mental Health Research (ROAMER) is promoting a similar trans-diagnostic approach (Goschke, Reference Goschke2014). More broadly, we hope to convey the importance of bridging the old with the new. RDoC should be applauded for its focus on biological reality, but perhaps goes too far by ignoring symptom-based approaches entirely. In psychiatry, the symptoms cause the suffering and our focus should remain on understanding and reducing these symptoms, as the patient experiences them.

Case-control studies point to trans-diagnostic mechanisms in OCD: error-related negativity (ERN) and anxiety

Can OCD be better understood and treated from a dimensional perspective? Evidence for this comes from many sources (Hyman, Reference Hyman2010); of these, perhaps the most compelling (and frustrating) is the lack of specificity of various neurocognitive deficits observed in OCD (Endrass & Ullsperger, Reference Endrass and Ullsperger2014; Fineberg et al. Reference Fineberg, Chamberlain, Goudriaan, Stein, Vanderschuren, Gillan, Shekar, Gorwood, Voon, Morein-Zamir, Denys, Sahakian, Moeller, Robbins and Potenza2014). For example, ‘response inhibition’, the ability to cancel a prepotent motor response assayed using the stop-signal reaction-time task (Logan, Reference Logan, Dagenbach and Carr1994), is reliably impaired in not just OCD, but also attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and schizophrenia (Lipszyc & Schachar, Reference Lipszyc and Schachar2010). Pessimistically, one might conclude from this lack of specificity that our current neurocognitive markers are not useful for understanding the aetiology of OCD. Perhaps these markers reflect generalized executive impairment, a consequence of having any psychiatric disorder and the burdens thereof. Perhaps these markers are not diagnostic of any specific mental health issue, but simply place individuals into an at-risk state for many. What we will consider in the following sections is an admittedly well-trodden alternative: that it is our psychiatric taxonomy, not our neurocognitive models, that presents a problem for research and that the specificity of any neurocognitive model of a psychiatric diagnosis is fundamentally limited by the extent to which that diagnosis is biologically valid.

How can we determine if a neurocognitive model is specific to a trans-diagnostic phenotype, if not one of the existing DSM disorder categories? What does this mean for interpreting the results of prior work in these DSM diagnosed patient populations? A reasonable starting point is to first identify a model of biological relevance (e.g. neurocognitive or physiological marker) that exhibits partial specificity for psychiatric disorders that is perhaps not specific to one disorder, but certainly not common to all. One can then qualitatively identify phenotypic commonalities across the disorders that exhibit mechanistic commonalities and thus formulate hypotheses about trans-diagnostic phenotypes (Robbins et al. Reference Robbins, Gillan, Smith, de Wit and Ersche2012; Fineberg et al. Reference Fineberg, Chamberlain, Goudriaan, Stein, Vanderschuren, Gillan, Shekar, Gorwood, Voon, Morein-Zamir, Denys, Sahakian, Moeller, Robbins and Potenza2014). Publication bias is an obstacle here as studies showing no difference between patient cohorts and controls, which are needed to make a case for specificity, are less likely to be published or even submitted for publication (Rosenthal, Reference Rosenthal1979). One solution (albeit imperfect) to this problem is studying multiple disorders at once, thereby allowing for direct statistical comparison of disorders that have established deficits in a given domain and disorders that are hypothesized to be no different from controls. Failing this, one can gain insights by leveraging cases where a given neurocognitive mechanism is significantly enhanced in one cluster of disorders, but deficient in another (thereby avoiding publication bias), as is the case for one very promising neurocognitive deficit in OCD that we will discuss in some detail, ERN (Endrass & Ullsperger, Reference Endrass and Ullsperger2014).

The ERN is a negative deflection of the event-related potential (ERP) when subjects make an error (Falkenstein et al. Reference Falkenstein, Hohnsbein, Hoormann and Blanke1991), listed as the RDoC sub-construct ‘Performance Monitoring’. It is typically measured using electroencephalogram, but functional imaging has been coupled with this technique to confirm that the ERN has its source in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (Debener et al. Reference Debener, Ullsperger, Siegel, Fiehler, von Cramon and Engel2005). A host of studies have found enhanced ERN in OCD (Gehring et al. Reference Gehring, Himle and Nisenson2000; Johannes et al. Reference Johannes, Wieringa, Nager, Rada, Dengler, Emrich, Münte and Dietrich2001b ; Ruchsow et al. Reference Ruchsow, Gron, Reuter, Spitzer, Hermle and Kiefer2005, Reference Ruchsow, Reuter, Hermle, Ebert, Kiefer and Falkenstein2007; Endrass et al. Reference Endrass, Klawohn, Schuster and Kathmann2008, Reference Endrass, Schuermann, Kaufmann, Spielberg, Kniesche and Kathmann2010, Reference Endrass, Riesel, Kathmann and Buhlmann2014; Hajcak et al. Reference Hajcak, Franklin, Foa and Simons2008; Stern et al. Reference Stern, Liu, Gehring, Lister, Yin, Zhang, Fitzgerald, Himle, Abelson and Taylor2010; Riesel et al. Reference Riesel, Endrass, Kaufmann and Kathmann2011, Reference Riesel, Endrass, Auerbach and Kathmann2015; Xiao et al. Reference Xiao, Wang, Zhang, Li, Tang, Wang, Fan and Fromson2011; Hanna et al. Reference Hanna, Carrasco, Harbin, Nienhuis, LaRosa, Chen, Fitzgerald and Gehring2012; Carrasco et al. Reference Carrasco, Harbin, Nienhuis, Fitzgerald, Gehring and Hanna2013a , Reference Carrasco, Hong, Nienhuis, Harbin, Fitzgerald, Gehring and Hanna b ; Grützmann et al. Reference Grützmann, Endrass, Kaufmann, Allen, Eichele and Kathmann2014; Klawohn et al. Reference Klawohn, Riesel, Grützmann, Kathmann and Endrass2014; Liu et al. Reference Liu, Hanna, Carrasco, Gehring and Fitzgerald2014) (but see Nieuwenhuis et al. Reference Nieuwenhuis, Nielen, Mol, Hajcak and Veltman2005; Agam et al. Reference Agam, Greenberg, Isom, Falkenstein, Jenike, Wilhelm and Manoach2014; Mathews et al. Reference Mathews, Perez, Roach, Fekri, Vigil, Kupferman and Mathalon2015; Weinberg et al. Reference Weinberg, Kotov and Proudfit2015), GAD (Ladouceur et al. Reference Ladouceur, Dahl, Birmaher, Axelson and Ryan2006; Weinberg et al. Reference Weinberg, Klein and Hajcak2012, Reference Weinberg, Kotov and Proudfit2015, Reference Weinberg, Olvet and Hajcak2010) (but see Xiao et al. Reference Xiao, Wang, Zhang, Li, Tang, Wang, Fan and Fromson2011), social anxiety disorder (Endrass et al. Reference Endrass, Riesel, Kathmann and Buhlmann2014) and depression (Chiu & Deldin, Reference Chiu and Deldin2007; Holmes & Pizzagalli, Reference Holmes and Pizzagalli2008, Reference Holmes and Pizzagalli2010; Georgiadi et al. Reference Georgiadi, Liotti, Nixon and Liddle2011; Aarts et al. Reference Aarts, Vanderhasselt, Otte, Baeken and Pourtois2013; Tang et al. Reference Tang, Zhang, Simmonite, Li, Zhang, Guo, Li, Fang, Xu and Wang2013; Mueller et al. Reference Mueller, Pechtel, Cohen, Douglas and Pizzagalli2015) (although less consistently than in generalized anxiety or OCD, see Ruchsow et al. Reference Ruchsow, Herrnberger, Wiesend, Grön, Spitzer and Kiefer2004, Reference Ruchsow, Herrnberger, Beschoner, Grön, Spitzer and Kiefer2006; Olvet et al. Reference Olvet, Klein and Hajcak2010; Georgiadi et al. Reference Georgiadi, Liotti, Nixon and Liddle2011; Weinberg et al. Reference Weinberg, Klein and Hajcak2012) (Table 1). Accounts regarding the precise function of ERN are varied (Holroyd & Coles, Reference Holroyd and Coles2002; Yeung et al. Reference Yeung, Botvinick and Cohen2004), but studies have shown that the ERN primes defensive responses, for example by potentiating startle responses (Hajcak & Foti, Reference Hajcak and Foti2008) and encouraging avoidance behaviour (Frank et al. Reference Frank, Woroch and Curran2005). Together with a qualitative examination of the symptom commonalities across these disorders, this suggests that anxiety might be a reasonable candidate for consideration as a dimensional phenotype related to these deficits. This thesis is supported by work in non-clinical samples, wherein the magnitude of the ERN is associated with self-reported worry (Hajcak et al. Reference Hajcak, McDonald and Simons2003; Zambrano-Vazquez & Allen, Reference Zambrano-Vazquez and Allen2014), high levels of trait anxiety (Paulus et al. Reference Paulus, Feinstein, Simmons and Stein2004) and OC-symptoms (Hajcak & Simons, Reference Hajcak and Simons2002; Zambrano-Vazquez & Allen, Reference Zambrano-Vazquez and Allen2014). The effect appears to be trait (rather than state) dependent; the ERN remains enhanced following successful treatment in both adult and paediatric OCD (Hajcak et al. Reference Hajcak, Franklin, Foa and Simons2008; Riesel et al. Reference Riesel, Endrass, Auerbach and Kathmann2015), does not differ as a result of chronic medication (Stern et al. Reference Stern, Liu, Gehring, Lister, Yin, Zhang, Fitzgerald, Himle, Abelson and Taylor2010) and an enhanced ERN has been observed in unaffected first-degree relatives of both adult and paediatric OCD sufferers (Riesel et al. Reference Riesel, Endrass, Kaufmann and Kathmann2011; Carrasco et al. Reference Carrasco, Harbin, Nienhuis, Fitzgerald, Gehring and Hanna2013a ), suggesting that this may be an important endophenotype that predisposes individuals to a range of disorders that involve anxiety.

Table 1. Summary of results for studies examining error-related negativity (ERN) across psychiatric disorders

n.s., Non-significant; PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder; RT, reaction time; RL, reinforcement learning.

a Unmedicated were free of selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitors or antipsychotics for at least 2 weeks, and free of stimulants for 17 h. In the case of addiction, unmedicated applies when subjects meet the above criteria and are abstinent.

b Studies were included if they reported results of ERN analysis (either ERN or ERN corrected for correct-related negativity), included a case-control comparison, and reported generic ERN effects using a task that could be compared across studies.

c Paediatric/adolescent sample.

d Compared medicated and unmedicated groups, and no medication effect was observed.

e Compared to other diagnoses in trans-diagnostic design.

f Mixed anxiety (mostly generalized anxiety disorder).

g Remitted patients.

h Mixture adult and adolescent.

i Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder combined-type only.

Critically, the postulate that an enhanced ERN is a useful marker of an anxious phenotype rests on the specificity of this effect. We can begin to, albeit imperfectly, approach this issue using existing work in diagnosed patients – specifically if enhanced ERN is observed in disorders not in part characterized by pathological anxiety, then this suggests there is more evidence for the pessimistic view suggested earlier. Fortunately, evidence for such specificity is abundant for this particular neurocognitive marker; a large body of work has showed that a significantly decreased ERN magnitude is characteristic of a putatively distinct cluster (or clusters) of disorders, most consistently schizophrenia (Kopp & Rist, Reference Kopp and Rist1999; Alain et al. Reference Alain, McNeely, He, Christensen and West2002; Bates et al. Reference Bates, Kiehl, Laurens and Liddle2002, Reference Bates, Liddle, Kiehl and Ngan2004; Morris et al. Reference Morris, Yee and Nuechterlein2006, Reference Morris, Heerey, Gold and Holroyd2008, Reference Morris, Holroyd, Mann-Wrobel and Gold2011; Mathalon et al. Reference Mathalon, Jorgensen, Roach and Ford2009; Foti et al. Reference Foti, Kotov, Bromet and Hajcak2012; Horan et al. Reference Horan, Foti, Hajcak, Wynn and Green2012; Perez et al. Reference Perez, Ford, Roach, Woods, McGlashan, Srihari, Loewy, Vinogradov and Mathalon2012; Simmonite et al. Reference Simmonite, Bates, Groom, Jackson, Hollis and Liddle2012; Houthoofd et al. Reference Houthoofd, Morrens, Sabbe, Schrijvers, Vandendriessche, Hulstijn and de Bruijn2013; Kansal et al. Reference Kansal, Patriciu and Kiang2014; Minzenberg et al. Reference Minzenberg, Gomes, Yoon, Swaab and Carter2014; de la Asuncion et al. Reference de la Asuncion, Docx, Morrens, Sabbe and de Bruijn2015; Reinhart et al. Reference Reinhart, Zhu, Park and Woodman2015), but also bipolar disorder (Minzenberg et al. Reference Minzenberg, Gomes, Yoon, Swaab and Carter2014; Morsel et al. Reference Morsel, Morrens, Temmerman, Sabbe and de Bruijn2014) (but see Kopf et al. Reference Kopf, Volkert, Heidler, Dresler, Kittel-Schneider, Gessner, Herrmann, Ehlis and Reif2015), autism spectrum disorders (Vlamings et al. Reference Vlamings, Jonkman, Hoeksma, van Engeland and Kemner2008; Sokhadze et al. Reference Sokhadze, Baruth, El-Baz, Horrell, Sokhadze, Carroll, Tasman, Sears and Casanova2010, Reference Sokhadze, Baruth, Sears, Sokhadze, El-Baz, Williams, Klapheke and Casanova2012; South et al. Reference South, Larson, Krauskopf and Clawson2010) (but see Groen et al. Reference Groen, Wijers, Mulder, Waggeveld, Minderaa and Althaus2008; Henderson et al. Reference Henderson, Ono, McMahon, Schwartz, Usher and Mundy2015) and various forms of addiction (Forman et al. Reference Forman, Dougherty, Casey, Siegle, Braver, Barch, Stenger, Wick-Hull, Pisarov and Lorensen2004; Franken et al. Reference Franken, van Strien, Franzek and van de Wetering2007; Sokhadze et al. Reference Sokhadze, Stewart, Hollifield and Tasman2008; Luijten et al. Reference Luijten, van Meel and Franken2011; Zhou et al. Reference Zhou, Li and Zhu2013) (but see Schellekens et al. Reference Schellekens, de Bruijn, van Lankveld, Hulstijn, Buitelaar, de Jong and Verkes2010; Chen et al. Reference Chen, Jiang, Guo, Du, Wang and Zhao2013). Moreover, the amplitude of the ERN was found to predict treatment adherence for substance abuse (Steele et al. Reference Steele, Fink, Maurer, Arbabshirani, Wilber, Jaffe, Sidz, Pearlson, Calhoun, Clark and Kiehl2014) and decreases in the ERN are associated with self-reported impulsivity (Potts et al. Reference Potts, George, Martin and Barratt2006). Results in ADHD are mixed with some studies showing decreased ERN (Liotti et al. Reference Liotti, Pliszka, Perez, Kothmann and Woldorff2005; van Meel et al. Reference van Meel, Heslenfeld, Oosterlaan and Sergeant2007; Groen et al. Reference Groen, Wijers, Mulder, Waggeveld, Minderaa and Althaus2008; Herrmann et al. Reference Herrmann, Mader, Schreppel, Jacob, Heine, Boreatti-Hümmer, Ehlis, Scheuerpflug, Pauli and Fallgatter2010; Groom et al. Reference Groom, Liddle, Scerif, Liddle, Batty, Liotti and Hollis2013), but most reporting no differences (Wiersema et al. Reference Wiersema, van der Meere and Roeyers2005, Reference Wiersema, van der Meere and Roeyers2009; Groen et al. Reference Groen, Wijers, Mulder, Waggeveld, Minderaa and Althaus2008; Wild-Wall et al. Reference Wild-Wall, Oades, Schmidt-Wessels, Christiansen and Falkenstein2009; Zhang et al. Reference Zhang, Wang, Cai and Yan2009; Groom et al. Reference Groom, Cahill, Bates, Jackson, Calton, Liddle and Hollis2010; Sokhadze et al. Reference Sokhadze, Baruth, Sears, Sokhadze, El-Baz, Williams, Klapheke and Casanova2012) and only one study reporting an increase (Burgio-Murphy et al. Reference Burgio-Murphy, Klorman, Shaywitz, Fletcher, Marchione, Holahan, Stuebing, Thatcher and Shaywitz2007) (Table 1). Together, these data provide convergent evidence that an enhancement of the ERN is not ubiquitous in psychiatry, but rather appears to be directionally specific to disorders characterized predominantly by anxiety. This therefore may constitute a trans-diagnostic marker relevant for many disorders involving anxiety, including OCD.

Validating a trans-diagnostic mechanism: goal-directed learning and compulsivity

The suggestion that the consistent finding of enhanced ERN in OCD in fact reflects a trans-diagnostic phenotype representing anxiety is perhaps a compelling one given the pattern of results described above. However, without direct comparison across disorders and/or discrete symptom dimensions, conclusions regarding its phenomenological specificity cannot be drawn (indeed ‘anxiety’ is intended to serve as a placeholder, until such work is complete). We will now highlight an approach that can take this next step, drawing on data from an independent line of research in OCD. This body of research centres on the theory that an imbalance between ‘goal-directed control’ and ‘habit-learning’ drives symptoms in OCD (see Gillan & Robbins, Reference Gillan and Robbins2014 for detailed account) (see Supplementary material), such that compulsive behaviours in OCD are not goal-directed responses to anxiety/perceived threat, but are in fact stimulus-evoked, goal-insensitive habits (Dickinson, Reference Dickinson1985). This model suggests that compulsions are not a search for safety; they are a result of the need to realize a link that has developed between a stimulus or context and a given set of responses. In this view, the subjective experiences that accompany habits, such as ‘not just right experience’ (Coles et al. Reference Coles, Heimberg, Frost and Steketee2005; Ecker & Gönner, Reference Ecker and Gönner2008), or more complex obsessive thoughts are the result of the compulsive urge, not the cause. For example, there is preliminary evidence to suggest that obsessive thoughts can even arise as a result of compulsive habit formation in OCD, and not the other way around (Gillan et al. Reference Gillan, Morein-Zamir, Urcelay, Sule, Voon, Apergis-Schoute, Fineberg, Sahakian and Robbins2014b ; Gillan & Sahakian, Reference Gillan and Sahakian2015), constituting a sharp divergence from most cognitive models of OCD that focus on obsessive thoughts (Salkovskis, Reference Salkovskis1985). The original habit hypothesis of OCD was based on a convergence of neurobiological data illustrating that the pathophysiology of OCD overlaps extensively with that supporting the balance between goal-directed behaviour and habits (Graybiel & Rauch, Reference Graybiel and Rauch2000; Dolan & Dayan, Reference Dolan and Dayan2013). Empirical studies have found broad support for this idea; OCD patients show a reliable tendency to form habits excessively in both appetitive and aversive learning contexts (Gillan et al. Reference Gillan, Papmeyer, Morein-Zamir, Sahakian, Fineberg, Robbins and de Wit2011, Reference Gillan, Morein-Zamir, Urcelay, Sule, Voon, Apergis-Schoute, Fineberg, Sahakian and Robbins2014b ). Based on convergent evidence from follow-up investigations utilizing neuroimaging and computational modelling (Gillan et al. Reference Gillan, Morein-Zamir, Kaser, Fineberg, Sule, Sahakian, Cardinal and Robbins2014a , Reference Gillan, Apergis-Schoute, Morein-Zamir, Urcelay, Sule, Fineberg, Sahakian and Robbins2015a ; Voon et al. Reference Voon, Derbyshire, Rück, Irvine, Worbe, Enander, Schreiber, Gillan, Fineberg, Sahakian, Robbins, Harrison, Wood, Daw, Dayan, Grant and Bullmore2014), the working model theorizes that deficits in goal-directed control, mediated by the caudate and medial orbitofrontal cortex, are responsible for the excessive habits observed in OCD in the laboratory (Gillan & Robbins, Reference Gillan and Robbins2014). Clinically, these habits might manifest as compulsive behaviours and/or higher-order habitual needs or goals (Cushman & Morris, Reference Cushman and Morris2015).

Addressing the trans-diagnostic potential of this neurocognitive mechanism, goal-directed control has been suggested to play a role in a range of disorders that are clinically characterized by repetitive behaviours that persist despite negative events, or are experienced as ‘out of control’ (Gillan et al. Reference Gillan, Robbins, Sahakian, van den Heuvel and van Wingen2016a ). These disorders include addiction (Everitt & Robbins, Reference Everitt and Robbins2005), eating disorders (Godier & Park, Reference Godier and Park2014) and Tourette's syndrome (Groenewegen et al. Reference Groenewegen, van den Heuvel, Cath, Voorn and Veltman2003), and although this area of research is still in its infancy, the first empirical investigations have found evidence for goal-directed deficits in these disorders (Sjoerds et al. Reference Sjoerds, de Wit, van den Brink, Robbins, Beekman, Penninx and Veltman2013; Voon et al. Reference Voon, Derbyshire, Rück, Irvine, Worbe, Enander, Schreiber, Gillan, Fineberg, Sahakian, Robbins, Harrison, Wood, Daw, Dayan, Grant and Bullmore2014; Delorme et al. Reference Delorme, Salvador, Palminteri, de Wit, Roze, Hartmann and Worbe2015; Ersche et al. Reference Ersche, Gillan, Jones, Williams, Luitjen, de Wit, Ward, Sahakian, Bullmore and Robbins2016) (Table 2). As such, the candidacy of goal-directed deficits as a trans-diagnostic marker has been acknowledged in the RDoC matrix, which flags several relevant theoretical domains, i.e. ‘Habit’, ‘Goal Selection, Updating, Representation and Maintenance’ and ‘Response Selection, Inhibition or Suppression’. However, studies have recently been published showing similar deficits in goal-directed control in schizophrenia (Morris et al. Reference Morris, Quail, Griffiths, Green and Balleine2015), autism spectrum disorder (Alvares et al. Reference Alvares, Balleine, Whittle and Guastella2016), and most problematically, social anxiety disorder (Alvares et al. Reference Alvares, Balleine and Guastella2014, Reference Alvares, Balleine, Whittle and Guastella2016), which is not considered clinically to be a ‘compulsive’ disorder (Table 2).

Table 2. Summary of results for studies examining goal-directed learning

n.s., Non-significant.

Goal-directed learning was measured using devaluation, model-based (MB) learning or action-outcome (A-O) learning test. The latter two measures are proxies for devaluation sensitivity (Gillan et al. Reference Gillan, Papmeyer, Morein-Zamir, Sahakian, Fineberg, Robbins and de Wit2011, Reference Gillan, Otto, Phelps and Daw2015b ).

a Unmedicated were free of selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitors or antipsychotics for at least 2 weeks, and free of stimulants for 17 h. In the case of addiction, unmedicated applies when subjects meet the above criteria and are abstinent.

b Compared multiple diagnostic groups.

c Compared medicated and unmedicated groups, and no medication effect was observed.

d Paediatric sample.

Using a case-control design, it is difficult to disentangle a genuine lack of mechanistic specificity, as these data might indicate, from ‘DSM measurement error’, the confounding influence of co-morbid psychiatric disorders. For example, in the aforementioned studies in social anxiety disorder patients (Alvares et al. Reference Alvares, Balleine and Guastella2014, Reference Alvares, Balleine, Whittle and Guastella2016), because patients presented with multiple co-morbid disorders, one cannot know if the results are attributable to social anxiety symptoms or one or more of their co-morbid conditions. Indeed, like most disorders, OCD rates are higher in the social anxiety population (Grant et al. Reference Grant, Hasin, Blanco, Stinson, Chou, Goldstein, Dawson, Smith, Saha and Huang2005). To get around this issue, some researchers (ourselves included) have endeavoured to recruit only ‘pure cases’ of OCD, i.e. those that do not meet the criteria for any other psychiatric diagnoses (Table 2). Although somewhat effective, the problem with this approach is that it assumes that being one criterion short of diagnosis of depression, for example, is the same as having no depressive symptoms at all. In reality, OCD patients recruited in this way consistently have higher levels of sub-threshold symptoms of multiple other disorders, e.g. depression and anxiety (Gillan et al. Reference Gillan, Papmeyer, Morein-Zamir, Sahakian, Fineberg, Robbins and de Wit2011). While these confounding variables could be controlled for statistically, most patient studies simply do not have the necessary power to do so. Indeed, the difficulty in recruiting sufficient sample sizes for research studies with patients is compounded by the increasing number of (possibly confounding) disorders defined by the DSM in every new edition.

A novel solution to this problem was presented in a recent study that eschewed the traditional case-control design in favour of the greater sample sizes (and in turn statistical power) that can be achieved by leveraging normal variability in a large general population sample (Gillan et al. Reference Gillan, Kosinski, Whelan, Phelps and Daw2016b ). The study tested if the relationship between goal-directed control and psychopathology constitutes a trans-diagnostic trait by testing its generalizability and specificity. Just under 2000 subjects were recruited online and completed self-report questionnaires assessing multiple psychiatric symptoms and completed a web-based task that assessed goal-directed performance (i.e. ‘model-based learning’; Daw et al. Reference Daw, Gershman, Seymour, Dayan and Dolan2011). In line with the preceding case-control literature, normal variation in self-reported OCD symptomatology (Foa et al. Reference Foa, Huppert, Leiberg, Langner, Kichic, Hajcak and Salkovskis2002) was associated with decreases in goal-directed control in two separate samples collected in this study (also see a recent independent replication: Snorrason et al. Reference Snorrason, Lee, de Wit and Woods2016). Importantly, this effect was generalizable to symptoms of other aspects of compulsive psychopathology (as previously characterized in the literature: Everitt & Robbins, Reference Everitt and Robbins2005; Hogarth et al. Reference Hogarth, Chase and Baess2012; Godier & Park, Reference Godier and Park2014). Specifically, eating disorders (Garner et al. Reference Garner, Olmsted, Bohr and Garfinkel1982), impulsivity (Patton et al. Reference Patton, Stanford and Barratt1995) and alcohol addiction (Saunders et al. Reference Saunders, Aasland, Babor, de la Fuente and Grant1993) were also associated with deficits in goal-directed control, suggestive of a trans-diagnostic deficit linking these disorders. Crucially, these deficits were not associated with symptoms of trait anxiety (Spielberger et al. Reference Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg and Jacobs1983), depression (Zung, Reference Zung1965), social anxiety (Liebowitz, Reference Liebowitz1987) or apathy (Marin et al. Reference Marin, Biedrzycki and Firinciogullari1991). Schizotypy (Mason et al. Reference Mason, Linney and Claridge2005) sat somewhere in the middle, showing a marginal association with goal-directed deficits (Fig. 2 a).

Fig. 2. Validating a trans-diagnostic dimension. (a) The strength of the association between self-report symptoms of various DSM disorders and deficits in goal-directed control. The pattern is strikingly non-specific. (b) The association between goal-directed deficits and three ‘trans-diagnostic symptom dimensions identified in a data-driven factor analysis. Factor 1 corresponds to ‘Anxious Depression’, Factor 2 is ‘Compulsive Behavior and Intrusive Thought’, and Factor 3 is ‘Social Withdrawal’. The association between Factor 2 (‘Compulsive Behavior and Intrusive Thought’) and deficits in goal-directed learning is greater than that of any of the nine DSM-inspired questionnaires and crucially, the relationship exhibits excellent specificity with respect to ‘non-compulsive’ aspects of psychopathology, i.e. Factor 1 (‘Anxious Depression’) and Factor 3 (‘Social Withdrawal’). Data reproduced with permission from Gillan et al. (Reference Gillan, Kosinski, Whelan, Phelps and Daw2016b ). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Although these results are suggestive of generalizability and specificity, this kind of analysis does not take us much further than work in diagnosed patients, save to say that these effects appear to be continuous in the general population rather than being specific to diagnosed groups. A more important question is what the psychiatric phenotype that explains this pattern of results? Is there a trans-diagnostic symptom dimension shared by some disorders (e.g. OCD, addiction, eating disorders), but not others (e.g. social anxiety, depression)? To answer this question empirically, this study employed a two-stage validation methodology. First, a factor analysis was carried out on psychiatric self-report data to identify a smaller set of clinical phenotypes that more parsimoniously explain the self-report symptom data. This approach revealed that three factors could replace the nine original questionnaires and of particular interest was a factor linking disorders at the level of compulsive behaviour and thought, so-called ‘Compulsive Behaviour and Intrusive Thought’. This factor comprised select features of OCD, eating disorders, addiction and some aspects of impulsivity and schizotypy that pertain to a loss of control over repetitive behaviour and thought. Next, this phenotype was tested for biological validity by assessing if subjects’ scores on this factor predicted goal-directed performance in an independent analysis. ‘Compulsive Behaviour and Intrusive Thought’ was found to be more predictive of goal-directed deficits than any DSM-inspired questionnaire collected in this study (Fig. 2 b), illustrating the power of data-driven clinical phenotyping. For example, this trans-diagnostic symptom dimension predicted task performance better than total scores on questionnaires measuring severity of DSM disorders such as OCD, eating disorders and addiction. Crucially, this relationship was dissociable from other trans-diagnostic phenotypes identified in this study, ‘Anxious-Depression’ and ‘Social Withdrawal’ (which showed no relation to goal-directed deficits), which could be compared directly in this study, providing compelling evidence for the specificity of this deficit to ‘Compulsive Behaviour and Intrusive Thought’. This approach showcases how in an appropriately powered general population sample, a neurocognitive model can be tested for both generalizability and specificity, leading to a data-driven definition of a neurobiologically validated clinical phenotype.

Going deeper: ‘units of analysis’

In tandem with new approaches to empirically define psychiatric phenotypes, the task for basic research is to delineate the neurobiology that underpins them. RDoC proposes that this should be done, where appropriate, using units of analysis that link clinical phenomenology (e.g. self-reports) to behaviour/physiology (via paradigms) to the underlying circuit, cells, molecules and finally, genes. The aims of this approach are to facilitate evidence-based drug-development (e.g. based on pharmacological underpinnings), to guide new therapeutic approaches such as psychological therapies (e.g. based on neurocognitive changes) or brain stimulation (e.g. based on knowledge of brain circuits), and to identify and treat those at risk (e.g. using genetic tests) (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3. A trans-diagnostic approach to obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). The predominant symptoms of OCD may result from complex interaction between independent trans-diagnostic dimensions, of which anxiety and compulsivity are good (but not the only) candidates. In this simplified schematic, we show just two putative trans-diagnostic psychiatric dimensions, there are of course many others. The task for basic research, outlined in the Research Domain Criteria initiative is to understand these dimensions at units of analysis, linking self-report symptoms (such as anxiety and compulsivity) to paradigms, physiology, circuits, cells, molecules and genes. GAD, Generalized anxiety disorder.

Here, we will highlight just a couple of examples drawing on units of analysis that have the most direct implications for treatment. Might the ERN be useful for deciding on which treatment to prescribe to which individual? To begin to answer this, we must review what is known about the pharmacology of the ERN (i.e. as a molecular unit of analysis). Benzodiazepines, specifically alprazolam (Riba et al. Reference Riba, Rodríguez-Fornells, Münte and Barbanoj2005), lorazepam (de Bruijn et al. Reference de Bruijn, Hulstijn, Verkes, Ruigt and Sabbe2004) and oxazepam (Johannes et al. Reference Johannes, Wieringa, Nager, Dengler and Münte2001a ), reduce the amplitude of the ERN. This suggests a potential role for GABA (which is thought to directly inhibit ACC functioning) in the ERN and with that, a putative biological basis for the therapeutic effect of these drugs for GAD and therefore plausibly also for anxiety as a trans-diagnostic trait (Ravindran & Stein, Reference Ravindran and Stein2010). Unfortunately, this theory runs into issues when one considers that benzodiazepines are ineffective for treating OCD (e.g. clonazepam, Hollander et al. Reference Hollander, Kaplan and Stahl2003). Moreover, antidepressants, such as paroxetine and mirtazapine, which appear to have no effect on the ERN (de Bruijn et al. Reference de Bruijn, Hulstijn, Verkes, Ruigt and Sabbe2004, Reference de Bruijn, Sabbe, Hulstijn, Ruigt and Verkes2006), have good efficacy for treating both OCD and GAD. This apparent lack of pharmacological specificity is compounded by research on dopamine, which is probably been the most studied neurotransmitter with respect to the ERN. Studies in this area have shown that amphetamine administration increases the amplitude of the ERN (de Bruijn et al. Reference de Bruijn, Hulstijn, Verkes, Ruigt and Sabbe2004), while antipsychotics such as haloperidol and olanzapine reduce it (Zirnheld et al. Reference Zirnheld, Carroll, Kieffaber, O'Donnell, Shekhar and Hetrick2004; de Bruijn et al. Reference de Bruijn, Sabbe, Hulstijn, Ruigt and Verkes2006). Although antipsychotics can be useful for OCD in particularly treatment resistant cases, GAD does not respond to antipsychotics (Ravindran & Stein, Reference Ravindran and Stein2010).

Thus, the question of how an ERN measurement can help inform treatment response across disorders is far from clear. It may be the case that combined measures, such as ERN, structural scans and self-report, are needed to produce a useful therapeutic signal. For example, although the core symptoms of OCD do not typically reduce following benzodiazepines, this drug class might be useful for decreasing anxiety in certain OCD patients – and these particularly anxious patients might be driving the ERN findings from studies examining this patient population at the group level. Alternatively, the ERN might have no specific causal role for psychiatry whatsoever, it might arise from a range of discrete upsets to neural harmony – longitudinal designs are needed to answer these questions definitively.

Turning to circuits, cells and the link between self-report compulsivity and deficits in goal-directed control (Gillan et al. Reference Gillan, Kosinski, Whelan, Phelps and Daw2016b ), studies point to the involvement of the medial orbitofrontal cortex (mOFC) and caudate based on their involvement in goal-directed control across species (Dolan & Dayan, Reference Dolan and Dayan2013) and convergent data suggesting these are critically involved in OCD and other compulsive disorders. For example, hyperactivity in the caudate nucleus is linked to failures in goal-directed behaviour observed in OCD patients (Gillan et al. Reference Gillan, Apergis-Schoute, Morein-Zamir, Urcelay, Sule, Fineberg, Sahakian and Robbins2015a ). In terms of brain structure, deficits in goal-directed control are associated with reductions of grey-matter volume in the caudate and mOFC in healthy individuals (Voon et al. Reference Voon, Derbyshire, Rück, Irvine, Worbe, Enander, Schreiber, Gillan, Fineberg, Sahakian, Robbins, Harrison, Wood, Daw, Dayan, Grant and Bullmore2014). Moreover, in this same study, the observation that these regional volumes are reduced in binge-eating disorder patients was statistically explained by their deficits of goal-directed control relative to controls (Voon et al. Reference Voon, Derbyshire, Rück, Irvine, Worbe, Enander, Schreiber, Gillan, Fineberg, Sahakian, Robbins, Harrison, Wood, Daw, Dayan, Grant and Bullmore2014). Although imaging studies in humans are important for validating animal models of psychiatric populations, animal models are crucial as they can uniquely assess causality (Ahmari, Reference Ahmari2015). To this end, one exemplary study showed that chronic stimulation of mOFC neurons that project to the ventromedial striatum [i.e. mimicking the hyperactivity observed in human OCD patients (Gillan et al. Reference Gillan, Apergis-Schoute, Morein-Zamir, Urcelay, Sule, Fineberg, Sahakian and Robbins2015a )] induces compulsive grooming behaviour in mice (Ahmari et al. Reference Ahmari, Spellman, Douglass, Kheirbek, Simpson, Deisseroth, Gordon and Hen2013). Furthermore, the authors found that excessive grooming was ameliorated with chronic SSRI treatment, the first line pharmacotherapy for OCD (Fineberg et al. Reference Fineberg, Reghunandanan, Brown and Pampaloni2013b ), thereby showing convergence across multiple units of analysis, from behaviour to brain circuits to molecules. What remains to be seen is whether this kind of chronic stimulation also induces deficits in goal-directed control in tandem with compulsive grooming, and if deficits in goal-directed control are remediated by treatment with SSRIs. These are testable hypotheses that, if supported, could mean that computerized tasks that assess this neurocognitive mechanism might be used to inform treatment allocation in the future. In addition to guiding pharmacotherapy decisions, work at the circuit level can be used to select target sites for deep-brain stimulation, of which there are many candidates for OCD (Mallet et al. Reference Mallet, Polosan, Jaafari, Baup, Welter, Fontaine, du Montcel, Yelnik, Chéreau, Arbus, Raoul, Aouizerate, Damier, Chabardès, Czernecki, Ardouin, Krebs, Bardinet, Chaynes, Burbaud, Cornu, Derost, Bougerol, Bataille, Mattei, Dormont, Devaux, Vérin, Houeto, Pollak, Benabid, Agid, Krack, Millet, Pelissolo and Group2008; Nuttin et al. Reference Nuttin, Gabriëls, Cosyns, Meyerson, Andréewitch, Sunaert, Maes, Dupont, Gybels, Gielen and Demeulemeester2008; Denys et al. Reference Denys, Mantione, Figee, van den Munckhof, Koerselman, Westenberg, Bosch and Schuurman2010; Greenberg et al. Reference Greenberg, Gabriels, Malone, Rezai, Friehs, Okun, Shapira, Foote, Cosyns, Kubu, Malloy, Salloway, Giftakis, Rise, Machado, Baker, Stypulkowski, Goodman, Rasmussen and Nuttin2010) and indeed to guide the allocation of distinct forms of behavioural therapy (Saxena et al. Reference Saxena, Gorbis, O'Neill, Baker, Mandelkern, Maidment, Chang, Salamon, Brody, Schwartz and London2009). The question of whether deficits in goal-directed control diminish with successful treatment (and are akin to state phenomena) or remain elevated and stable (trait) awaits investigation. But even more interesting in our minds is the question of whether or not goal-directed performance at baseline predicts better response to one treatment over another. This may be how the success of the marker should be ultimately judged.

Precision therapeutics: insights and trans-diagnostic opportunities

Although defining biologically valid clinical phenotypes as accurately and discretely as possible is valuable, it may not be the most important thing that trans-diagnostic psychiatry research can contribute. Even if a phenotype such as ‘compulsivity’ possesses good neurobiological validity, specificity and homogeneity, this may not translate into homogeneity of treatment response. Rather, discrete neural pathophysiology may give rise to common network-level dysfunctions and behavioural manifestations, and these distal phenotypic markers might therefore offer minimal insight for clinicians in deciding on whether or not an SSRI will be effective in an individual. One way to address this is to work from the bottom-up – to take a step back from phenotypic classification and focus instead on establishing robust and direct links between neurocognitive markers and treatment response. This kind of work has already begun, typically within the confines of one diagnostic category or another and we will briefly mention some of the most promising data. We will take a particular focus on pharmacological interventions, but the general approach can similarly be applied to behavioural interventions. Then we will highlight how treatment prediction research involving sufferers of many DSM categories that are prescribed the same treatment is likely to be an important approach of the future.

Taking neural markers as an example, several studies have had early success at using functional imaging at the group level to predict treatment response within the OCD population (for review see Ball et al. Reference Ball, Stein and Paulus2014). One potential marker of SRI response in OCD is reduced baseline activity in the OFC and ACC (Swedo et al. Reference Swedo, Pietrini, Leonard, Schapiro, Rettew, Goldberger, Rapoport, Rapoport and Grady1992, Reference Swedo, Schapiro, Grady, Cheslow, Leonard, Kumar, Friedland, Rapoport and Rapoport1989; Brody et al. Reference Brody, Saxena, Schwartz, Stoessel, Maidment, Phelps and Baxter1998; Saxena et al. Reference Saxena, Brody, Maidment, Dunkin, Colgan, Alborzian, Phelps and Baxter1999). Unfortunately several studies that came later failed to replicate (Saxena et al. Reference Saxena, Brody, Ho, Zohrabi, Maidment and Baxter2003; Carey et al. Reference Carey, Warwick, Niehaus, van der Linden, van Heerden, Harvey, Seedat and Stein2004; Ho Pian et al. Reference Ho Pian, van Megen, Ramsey, Mandl, van Rijk, Wynne and Westenberg2005; van der Wee et al. Reference van der Wee, Ramsey, van Megen, Denys, Westenberg and Kahn2007), finding no significant effects or in some cases entirely different neural markers. A recent study tested whether a task-related ACC measure like ERN might fare better than these resting studies at predicting clinical response. Although the result should be taken cautiously as the sample size was particularly small (only 10 patients returned at post-treatment assessment), the authors were unable to find a relationship between baseline ERN and cognitive behaviour therapy response in a paediatric OCD sample (Hajcak et al. Reference Hajcak, Franklin, Foa and Simons2008).

One possibility is that the predictive value of ACC activity (if there is any) could be exclusive to a putative anxiety dimension and this might get washed out in studies relying on heterogeneous diagnostic categories and small samples. Recruiting large samples across diagnostic categories and recording changes in the symptoms of dissociable psychiatric dimensions like anxiety and compulsivity might help isolate reliable effects. Indeed, in the one study that adopted a trans-diagnostic approach, recruiting individuals with OCD, depression or both, the authors were able to identify dissociable neural markers of subsequent depressive v. obsessive-compulsive response to paroxetine (Saxena et al. Reference Saxena, Brody, Ho, Zohrabi, Maidment and Baxter2003). This means that not only is the same drug effective in treating distinct psychiatric problems, its efficacy at treating depression and OCD symptoms (sometimes in the same individual) is predicted by dissociable patterns of abnormal baseline neural activity. Given that individuals with a diagnosis of OCD are aetiologically and phenotypically heterogeneous, the task for future research is not to identify mean baseline activity patterns in responders v. non-responders on some singular measure of function, but to use large datasets to tease apart the mechanisms through which a given drug can reduce symptoms of largely independent psychiatric dimensions, and use these insights to identify the patients for whom a symptom reduction is likely to happen following treatment.

To conclude this section with some practical statements, morphometric predictors of SRI response in OCD already appear to be somewhat more robust than functional ones. The only two structural MRI studies to date have both shown that decreased grey matter in the lateral OFC is also linked to SRI response (Hoexter et al. Reference Hoexter, Dougherty, Shavitt, D'Alcante, Duran, Lopes, Diniz, Batistuzzo, Evans, Bressan, Busatto and Miguel2013, Reference Hoexter, Diniz, Lopes, Batistuzzo, Shavitt, Dougherty, Duran, Bressan, Busatto, Miguel and Sato2015). Hoexter and colleagues showed that OFC grey-matter volume had 77% sensitivity and 81% specificity at predicting SRI response in OCD. If these figures hold up to replication using the same analysis pipeline, the interpretation is that if this marker were used to determine whether or not an SRI is prescribed to a drug-naive individual, just two out of every 10 people assigned to SSRI would not respond, while another two of the 10 not assigned the treatment would have missed out on the benefits of the SSRI (Hoexter et al. Reference Hoexter, Diniz, Lopes, Batistuzzo, Shavitt, Dougherty, Duran, Bressan, Busatto, Miguel and Sato2015). This would make it a useful tool for clinicians, were MRI scans not expensive and impractical for most clinics to use diagnostically (at least for now). This is where we suggest cognitive neuroscience might be able to intervene by replacing brain scans with cognitive tasks that are good indicators of neurobiology, such those linked to normal and pathological variation in OFC grey-matter volume, of which goal-directed control is one notable example (Voon et al. Reference Voon, Derbyshire, Rück, Irvine, Worbe, Enander, Schreiber, Gillan, Fineberg, Sahakian, Robbins, Harrison, Wood, Daw, Dayan, Grant and Bullmore2014). Cognitive behavioural therapy for OCD similarly might benefit from the addition of components that focus more directly on habit formation and goal-directed control in those patients for whom it is more relevant. For example, one might focus psychoeducation: patients could be taught about identifying habit ‘trigger’ situations, or identifying the development of new habits that might later develop into more elaborate compulsions. Alternatively, habit-reversal training (Snorrason et al. Reference Snorrason, Berlin and Lee2015) may be used to complement standard exposure and response prevention techniques. These behavioural treatments might be further enhanced with targeted pharmacotherapy, for which basic data are accruing (Lovinger, Reference Lovinger2010).

Combining trans-diagnostic with existing approaches

We have argued strongly in favour of the trans-diagnostic model as a rational approach to advance basic science research in psychiatry, but must make clear that to date this approach has yet to facilitate gene discovery or the development of new, better treatments. It will take time for sufficient data to accrue and as such, dismissing symptom-based approaches entirely is premature. Existing clinical trial datasets, based on patients grouped according to conventional diagnostic criteria such as DSM, have demonstrated a clear specificity of effect for certain psychiatric treatments [e.g. serotonin v. noradrenaline reuptake inhibitors in OCD (Fineberg et al. Reference Fineberg, Chamberlain, Goudriaan, Stein, Vanderschuren, Gillan, Shekar, Gorwood, Voon, Morein-Zamir, Denys, Sahakian, Moeller, Robbins and Potenza2014)], and a magnitude of efficacy for some of our treatments commensurate with the effect sizes seen in other branches of medicine (Davis et al. Reference Davis, Giakas, Qu, Prasad and Leucht2011; Leucht et al. Reference Leucht, Helfer, Gartlehner and Davis2015). We have also seen that specific drug response can be linked to easily observable symptoms, such as the presence of tics (Bloch et al. Reference Bloch, Landeros-Weisenberger, Kelmendi, Coric, Bracken and Leckman2006). The hope is that by revisiting how we define disorder and/or dimensions using biological and cognitive data will further enhance the precision with which treatments can be delivered to individuals. In addition, new ways of assessing the outcomes of treatment trials, e.g. using statistical techniques such as group-based mixture models, that divide ‘diagnostic’ patient samples into clinically meaningful subgroups with greater predictive value, hold promise for the advancement of personalized psychiatric treatment (Thase et al. Reference Thase, Larsen and Kennedy2011). Thus, most progress is likely to be made with a balanced approach, taking into account and if possible combining the most valid and reliable diagnostic and trans-diagnostic methodologies.

Conclusion

This paper presented a trans-diagnostic perspective on OCD – a diagnostic category – that selectively focused on just two putative mechanisms relevant for our understanding of anxiety and compulsivity in OCD. These candidate mechanisms illustrate how two orthogonal trans-diagnostic mechanisms might converge to produce a phenotype that is currently defined as a unitary category. Specifically, avoidance compulsions, obsessions and anxiety are the defining clinical features of OCD according to DSM-5 (APA, 2013). As the symptom complex that this triad produces is unlikely to be underpinned by one specific cognitive/neural mechanism, breaking OCD down into orthogonal trans-diagnostic mechanisms may be of benefit for developing translational models – particularly for animal models that are crucial for causal mechanistic investigations. This research initiative has just begun for OCD, and many candidate dimensions await empirical validation or rejection. This can be achieved in many different ways, just a few of which have been outlined here (see other good examples: Fair et al. Reference Fair, Bathula, Nikolas and Nigg2012; Brodersen et al. Reference Brodersen, Deserno, Schlagenhauf, Lin, Penny, Buhmann and Stephan2014), and at many units of analysis. Most importantly, psychiatric diagnostic tools of the future must focus on maximizing predictive value, refining treatment allocation (both behavioural and pharmacological), identifying prospects and tools for early intervention, determining risk and clinical course. To do this, we need a new wave of focused longitudinal studies in psychiatry that can assess which cognitive tests or neural markers possess the most clinically useful properties. While studies of this kind have been historically difficult to conduct, costly and complicated, the field is already rising to the challenge by developing new methodologies (Brodersen et al. Reference Brodersen, Deserno, Schlagenhauf, Lin, Penny, Buhmann and Stephan2014; Gillan & Daw, Reference Gillan and Daw2016) that will help us to put biological psychiatry to the clinical test.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291716002786.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Michael Evans for his contribution to the preparation of Tables 1 and 2. We also thank Gerald Nestadt and Jack Samuels for providing the data for Fig. 1 from their original paper (Nestadt et al. Reference Nestadt, Di, Riddle, Grados, Greenberg, Fyer, McCracken, Rauch, Murphy, Rasmussen, Cullen, Pinto, Knowles, Piacentini, Pauls, Bienvenu, Wang, Liang, Samuels and Roche2009). C.M.G. is supported by a Sir Henry Wellcome Postdoctoral Fellowship from the Wellcome Trust (101521/Z/12/Z). T.W.R. is supported by a Senior Investigator Award from the Wellcome Trust (106431/Z/14/Z).

Declaration of Interest

C.M.G. has no disclosures. N.A.F has received payment for consultancy from Servier, Lundbeck, GSK, AstraZeneca and Bristol-Myers Squibb, for research activities from Servier, Lundbeck, ECNP, MRC, NIHR, Wellcome and for educational activities from Shire, Otsuka, Lundbeck, Janssen, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Wyeth, Servier, College of Mental Health Pharmacists, Royal College of Psychiatrists, Royal Australia and New Zealand College of Psychiatrists, BAP. T.W.R consults for Cambridge Cognition, E. Lilly , Lundbeck, Teva, Shire Pharmaceuticals, Otsuka. Research Grants; GSK, Lundbeck, Eli Lilly Inc. He receives royalties from Cambridge Cognition (CANTAB). He is an editor at Springer and Elsevier.

References

Aarts, K, Vanderhasselt, MA, Otte, G, Baeken, C, Pourtois, G (2013). Electrical brain imaging reveals the expression and timing of altered error monitoring functions in major depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 122, 939950.Google Scholar
Adams, CD (1982). Variations in the sensitivity of instrumental responding to reinforcer devaluation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, Section B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology 34, 7798.Google Scholar
Agam, Y, Greenberg, JL, Isom, M, Falkenstein, MJ, Jenike, E, Wilhelm, S, Manoach, DS (2014). Aberrant error processing in relation to symptom severity in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a multimodal neuroimaging study. Neuroimage: Clinical 5, 141151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ahmari, S, Spellman, T, Douglass, N, Kheirbek, M, Simpson, H, Deisseroth, K, Gordon, J, Hen, R (2013). Repeated cortico-striatal stimulation generates persistent OCD-like behavior. Science 340, 12341239.Google Scholar
Ahmari, SE (2015). Using mice to model obsessive compulsive disorder: from genes to circuits. Neuroscience.Google ScholarPubMed
Alain, C, McNeely, HE, He, Y, Christensen, BK, West, R (2002). Neurophysiological evidence of error-monitoring deficits in patients with schizophrenia. Cerebral Cortex 12, 840846.Google Scholar
Alvarenga, PG, do Rosário, MC, Batistuzzo, MC, Diniz, JB, Shavitt, RG, Duran, FL, Dougherty, DD, Bressan, RA, Miguel, EC, Hoexter, MQ (2012). Obsessive-compulsive symptom dimensions correlate to specific gray matter volumes in treatment-naïve patients. Journal of Psychiatry Research 46, 16351642.Google Scholar
Alvares, GA, Balleine, BW, Guastella, AJ (2014). Impairments in goal-directed actions predict treatment response to cognitive-behavioral therapy in social anxiety disorder. PLoS ONE 9, e94778.Google Scholar
Alvares, GA, Balleine, BW, Whittle, L, Guastella, AJ (2016). Reduced goal-directed action control in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research Published online: 21 March 2016. doi:10.1002/aur.1613.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
APA (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edn. American Psychiatric Publishing: Arlington, VA.Google Scholar
Ball, TM, Stein, MB, Paulus, MP (2014). Toward the application of functional neuroimaging to individualized treatment for anxiety and depression. Depression and Anxiety 31, 920933.Google Scholar
Bates, AT, Kiehl, KA, Laurens, KR, Liddle, PF (2002). Error-related negativity and correct response negativity in schizophrenia. Clinical Neurophysiology 113, 14541463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, AT, Liddle, PF, Kiehl, KA, Ngan, ET (2004). State dependent changes in error monitoring in schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatric Research 38, 347356.Google Scholar
Bloch, MH, Landeros-Weisenberger, A, Kelmendi, B, Coric, V, Bracken, MB, Leckman, JF (2006). A systematic review: antipsychotic augmentation with treatment refractory obsessive-compulsive disorder. Molecular Psychiatry 11, 622632.Google Scholar
Brodersen, KH, Deserno, L, Schlagenhauf, F, Lin, Z, Penny, WD, Buhmann, JM, Stephan, KE (2014). Dissecting psychiatric spectrum disorders by generative embedding. Neuroimage: Clinical 4, 98111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brody, AL, Saxena, S, Schwartz, JM, Stoessel, PW, Maidment, K, Phelps, ME, Baxter, LR (1998). FDG-PET predictors of response to behavioral therapy and pharmacotherapy in obsessive compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Research 84, 16.Google Scholar
Burgio-Murphy, A, Klorman, R, Shaywitz, SE, Fletcher, JM, Marchione, KE, Holahan, J, Stuebing, KK, Thatcher, JE, Shaywitz, BA (2007). Error-related event-related potentials in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, reading disorder, and math disorder. Biological Psychology 75, 7586.Google Scholar
Carey, PD, Warwick, J, Niehaus, DJ, van der Linden, G, van Heerden, BB, Harvey, BH, Seedat, S & Stein, DJ (2004). Single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) of anxiety disorders before and after treatment with citalopram. BMC Psychiatry 4, 30.Google Scholar
Carrasco, M, Harbin, SM, Nienhuis, JK, Fitzgerald, KD, Gehring, WJ, Hanna, GL (2013 a). Increased error-related brain activity in youth with obsessive-compulsive disorder and unaffected siblings. Depression and Anxiety 30, 3946.Google Scholar
Carrasco, M, Hong, C, Nienhuis, JK, Harbin, SM, Fitzgerald, KD, Gehring, WJ, Hanna, GL (2013 b). Increased error-related brain activity in youth with obsessive-compulsive disorder and other anxiety disorders. Neuroscience Letters 541, 214218.Google Scholar
Chen, H, Jiang, H, Guo, Q, Du, J, Wang, J, Zhao, M (2013). Case-control study of error-related negativity among males with heroin dependence undergoing rehabilitation. Shanghai Archives of Psychiatry 25, 141148.Google ScholarPubMed
Chiu, PH, Deldin, PJ (2007). Neural evidence for enhanced error detection in major depressive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 164, 608616.Google Scholar
Clemans, ZA, El-Baz, AS, Hollifield, M, Sokhadze, EM (2012). Single trial time-frequency domain analysis of error processing in post-traumatic stress disorder. Neuroscience Letters 525, 105110.Google Scholar
Coles, ME, Heimberg, RG, Frost, RO, Steketee, G (2005). Not just right experiences and obsessive-compulsive features: experimental and self-monitoring perspectives. Behaviour, Research and Therapy 43, 153167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cullen, B, Brown, CH, Riddle, MA, Grados, M, Bienvenu, OJ, Hoehn-Saric, R, Shugart, YY, Liang, KY, Samuels, J, Nestadt, G (2007). Factor analysis of the yale-brown obsessive compulsive scale in a family study of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Depression and Anxiety 24, 130138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cushman, F, Morris, A (2015). Habitual control of goal selection in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 112, 1381713822.Google Scholar
Cuthbert, BN, Kozak, MJ (2013). Constructing constructs for psychopathology: the NIMH research domain criteria. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 122, 928937.Google Scholar
Davis, JM, Giakas, WJ, Qu, J, Prasad, P, Leucht, S (2011). Should we treat depression with drugs or psychological interventions? A reply to Ioannidis. Philosophy, Ethics, and Humanities in Medicine 6, 8.Google Scholar
Daw, ND, Gershman, SJ, Seymour, B, Dayan, P, Dolan, RJ (2011). Model-based influences on humans’ choices and striatal prediction errors. Neuron 69, 12041215.Google Scholar
Debener, S, Ullsperger, M, Siegel, M, Fiehler, K, von Cramon, DY, Engel, AK (2005). Trial-by-trial coupling of concurrent electroencephalogram and functional magnetic resonance imaging identifies the dynamics of performance monitoring. Journal of Neuroscience 25, 1173011737.Google Scholar
de Bruijn, ER, Hulstijn, W, Verkes, RJ, Ruigt, GS, Sabbe, BG (2004). Drug-induced stimulation and suppression of action monitoring in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 177, 151160.Google Scholar
de Bruijn, ER, Sabbe, BG, Hulstijn, W, Ruigt, GS, Verkes, RJ (2006). Effects of antipsychotic and antidepressant drugs on action monitoring in healthy volunteers. Brain Research 1105, 122129.Google Scholar
de la Asuncion, J, Docx, L, Morrens, M, Sabbe, B, de Bruijn, ER (2015). Neurophysiological evidence for diminished monitoring of own, but intact monitoring of other's errors in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research 230, 220226.Google Scholar
Delorme, C, Salvador, A, Palminteri, S, de Wit, S, Roze, E, Hartmann, A, Worbe, Y (2015). Goal-directed and habitual behaviour systems in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Movement Disorders 30, S490S490.Google Scholar
Delorme, C, Salvador, A, Valabrègue, R, Roze, E, Palminteri, S, Vidailhet, M, de Wit, S, Robbins, T, Hartmann, A, Worbe, Y (2016). Enhanced habit formation in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Brain 139, 605615.Google Scholar
Denys, D, Mantione, M, Figee, M, van den Munckhof, P, Koerselman, F, Westenberg, H, Bosch, A, Schuurman, R (2010). Deep brain stimulation of the nucleus accumbens for treatment-refractory obsessive-compulsive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry 67, 10611068.Google Scholar
de Wit, SJ, Alonso, P, Schweren, L, Mataix-Cols, D, Lochner, C, Menchón, JM, Stein, DJ, Fouche, JP, Soriano-Mas, C, Sato, JR, Hoexter, MQ, Denys, D, Nakamae, T, Nishida, S, Kwon, JS, Jang, JH, Busatto, GF, Cardoner, N, Cath, DC, Fukui, K, Jung, WH, Kim, SN, Miguel, EC, Narumoto, J, Phillips, ML, Pujol, J, Remijnse, PL, Sakai, Y, Shin, NY, Yamada, K, Veltman, DJ, van den Heuvel, OA (2014). Multicenter voxel-based morphometry mega-analysis of structural brain scans in obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 171, 340349.Google Scholar
Dickinson, A (1985). Actions and habits: the development of behavioural autonomy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 308, 6778.Google Scholar
Dolan, RJ, Dayan, P (2013). Goals and habits in the brain. Neuron 80, 312325.Google Scholar
Ecker, W, Gönner, S (2008). Incompleteness and harm avoidance in OCD symptom dimensions. Behaviour, Research and Therapy 46, 895904.Google Scholar
Endrass, T, Klawohn, J, Schuster, F, Kathmann, N (2008). Overactive performance monitoring in obsessive-compulsive disorder: ERP evidence from correct and erroneous reactions. Neuropsychologia 46, 18771887.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Endrass, T, Riesel, A, Kathmann, N, Buhlmann, U (2014). Performance monitoring in obsessive-compulsive disorder and social anxiety disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 123, 705714.Google Scholar
Endrass, T, Schuermann, B, Kaufmann, C, Spielberg, R, Kniesche, R, Kathmann, N (2010). Performance monitoring and error significance in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psychology 84, 257263.Google Scholar
Endrass, T, Ullsperger, M (2014). Specificity of performance monitoring changes in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 46, 124138.Google Scholar
Ersche, KD, Gillan, CM, Jones, PS, Williams, GB, Luitjen, M, de Wit, S, Ward, LHE, Sahakian, BJ, Bullmore, ET, Robbins, TW (2016). Carrots and sticks fail to change behavior in cocaine addiction. Science 352, 14681471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Everitt, BJ, Dickinson, A, Robbins, TW (2001). The neuropsychological basis of addictive behaviour. Brain Research Reviews 36, 129138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Everitt, BJ, Robbins, TW (2005). Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion. Nature Neuroscience 8, 14811489.Google Scholar
Fair, DA, Bathula, D, Nikolas, MA, Nigg, JT (2012). Distinct neuropsychological subgroups in typically developing youth inform heterogeneity in children with ADHD. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 109, 67696774.Google Scholar
Falkenstein, M, Hohnsbein, J, Hoormann, J, Blanke, L (1991). Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 78, 447455.Google Scholar
Fineberg, NA, Chamberlain, SR, Goudriaan, AE, Stein, DJ, Vanderschuren, LJ, Gillan, CM, Shekar, S, Gorwood, PA, Voon, V, Morein-Zamir, S, Denys, D, Sahakian, BJ, Moeller, FG, Robbins, TW, Potenza, MN (2014). New developments in human neurocognition: clinical, genetic, and brain imaging correlates of impulsivity and compulsivity. CNS Spectrums 19, 6989.Google Scholar
Fineberg, NA, Hengartner, MP, Bergbaum, C, Gale, T, Rössler, W, Angst, J (2013 a). Lifetime comorbidity of obsessive-compulsive disorder and sub-threshold obsessive-compulsive symptomatology in the community: impact, prevalence, socio-demographic and clinical characteristics. International Journal of Psychiatry in Clinical Practice 17, 188196.Google Scholar
Fineberg, NA, Reghunandanan, S, Brown, A, Pampaloni, I (2013 b). Pharmacotherapy of obsessive-compulsive disorder: evidence-based treatment and beyond. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 47, 121141.Google Scholar
Foa, EB, Huppert, JD, Leiberg, S, Langner, R, Kichic, R, Hajcak, G, Salkovskis, P (2002). The obsessive-compulsive inventory: development and validation of a short version. Psychological Assessment 14, 485496.Google Scholar
Forman, SD, Dougherty, GG, Casey, BJ, Siegle, GJ, Braver, TS, Barch, DM, Stenger, VA, Wick-Hull, C, Pisarov, LA, Lorensen, E (2004). Opiate addicts lack error-dependent activation of rostral anterior cingulate. Biological Psychiatry 55, 531537.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foti, D, Kotov, R, Bromet, E, Hajcak, G (2012). Beyond the broken error-related negativity: functional and diagnostic correlates of error processing in psychosis. Biological Psychiatry 71, 864872.Google Scholar
Frank, MJ, Woroch, BS, Curran, T (2005). Error-related negativity predicts reinforcement learning and conflict biases. Neuron 47, 495501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franken, IH, van Strien, JW, Franzek, EJ, van de Wetering, BJ (2007). Error-processing deficits in patients with cocaine dependence. Biological Psychology 75, 4551.Google Scholar
Garner, DM, Olmsted, MP, Bohr, Y, Garfinkel, PE (1982). The eating attitudes test: psychometric features and clinical correlates. Psychological Medicine 12, 871878.Google Scholar
Gehring, WJ, Himle, J, Nisenson, LG (2000). Action-monitoring dysfunction in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychological Science 11, 16.Google Scholar
Georgiadi, E, Liotti, M, Nixon, NL, Liddle, PF (2011). Electrophysiological evidence for abnormal error monitoring in recurrent major depressive disorder. Psychophysiology 48, 11921202.Google Scholar
Geurts, H, de Wit, S (2013). Goal-directed action control in children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism 18, 409418.Google Scholar
Gillan, C, Daw, N (2016). Taking psychiatry research online. Neuron 91, 1923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillan, C, Kosinski, M, Whelan, R, Phelps, E, Daw, N (2016 b). Characterizing a psychiatric symptom dimension related to deficits in goal-directed control. eLife 5, e11305.Google Scholar
Gillan, C, Robbins, T, Sahakian, B, van den Heuvel, O, van Wingen, G (2016 a). The role of habit in compulsivity. European Neuropsychopharmacology 5, 828840.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillan, CM, Apergis-Schoute, AM, Morein-Zamir, S, Urcelay, GP, Sule, A, Fineberg, NA, Sahakian, BJ, Robbins, TW (2015 a). Functional neuroimaging of avoidance habits in obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 172, 284293.Google Scholar
Gillan, CM, Morein-Zamir, S, Kaser, M, Fineberg, NA, Sule, A, Sahakian, BJ, Cardinal, RN, Robbins, TW (2014 a). Counterfactual processing of economic action-outcome alternatives in obsessive-compulsive disorder: further evidence of impaired goal-directed behavior. Biological Psychiatry 75, 639646.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillan, CM, Morein-Zamir, S, Urcelay, GP, Sule, A, Voon, V, Apergis-Schoute, AM, Fineberg, NA, Sahakian, BJ & Robbins, TW (2014 b). Enhanced avoidance habits in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry 75, 631638.Google Scholar
Gillan, CM, Otto, AR, Phelps, EA, Daw, ND (2015 b). Model-based learning protects against forming habits. Cognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 15, 523536.Google Scholar
Gillan, CM, Papmeyer, M, Morein-Zamir, S, Sahakian, BJ, Fineberg, NA, Robbins, TW, de Wit, S (2011). Disruption in the balance between goal-directed behavior and habit learning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 168, 718726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillan, CM, Robbins, TW (2014). Goal-directed learning and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London, Series B: Biological Sciences 369, 20130475.Google Scholar
Gillan, CM, Sahakian, BJ (2015). Which is the driver, the obsessions or the compulsions, in OCD? Neuropsychopharmacology 40, 247248.Google Scholar
Godier, LR, Park, RJ (2014). Compulsivity in anorexia nervosa: a transdiagnostic concept. Frontiers in Psychology 5, 778.Google Scholar
Goschke, T (2014). Dysfunctions of decision-making and cognitive control as transdiagnostic mechanisms of mental disorders: advances, gaps, and needs in current research. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 23, 4157.Google Scholar
Grant, BF, Hasin, DS, Blanco, C, Stinson, FS, Chou, SP, Goldstein, RB, Dawson, DA, Smith, S, Saha, TD, Huang, B (2005). The epidemiology of social anxiety disorder in the United States: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 66, 13511361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graybiel, AM (1998). The basal ganglia and chunking of action repertoires. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 70, 119136.Google Scholar
Graybiel, AM, Rauch, SL (2000). Toward a neurobiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuron 28, 343347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, BD, Gabriels, LA, Malone, DA, Rezai, AR, Friehs, GM, Okun, MS, Shapira, NA, Foote, KD, Cosyns, PR, Kubu, CS, Malloy, PF, Salloway, SP, Giftakis, JE, Rise, MT, Machado, AG, Baker, KB, Stypulkowski, PH, Goodman, WK, Rasmussen, SA, Nuttin, BJ (2010). Deep brain stimulation of the ventral internal capsule/ventral striatum for obsessive-compulsive disorder: worldwide experience. Molecular Psychiatry 15, 6479.Google Scholar
Groen, Y, Wijers, AA, Mulder, LJ, Waggeveld, B, Minderaa, RB, Althaus, M (2008). Error and feedback processing in children with ADHD and children with Autistic Spectrum Disorder: an EEG event-related potential study. Clinical Neurophysiology 119, 24762493.Google Scholar
Groenewegen, HJ, van den Heuvel, OA, Cath, DC, Voorn, P, Veltman, DJ (2003). Does an imbalance between the dorsal and ventral striatopallidal systems play a role in Tourette's syndrome? A neuronal circuit approach. Brain and Development 25 (Suppl. 1), S3S14.Google Scholar
Groom, MJ, Cahill, JD, Bates, AT, Jackson, GM, Calton, TG, Liddle, PF, Hollis, C (2010). Electrophysiological indices of abnormal error-processing in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 51, 6676.Google Scholar
Groom, MJ, Liddle, EB, Scerif, G, Liddle, PF, Batty, MJ, Liotti, M, Hollis, CP (2013). Motivational incentives and methylphenidate enhance electrophysiological correlates of error monitoring in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 54, 836845.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grützmann, R, Endrass, T, Kaufmann, C, Allen, E, Eichele, T, Kathmann, N (2014). Presupplementary motor area contributes to altered error monitoring in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry 80, 562571.Google Scholar
Hajcak, G, Foti, D (2008). Errors are aversive: defensive motivation and the error-related negativity. Psychological Science 19, 103108.Google Scholar
Hajcak, G, Franklin, ME, Foa, EB, Simons, RF (2008). Increased error-related brain activity in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder before and after treatment. American Journal of Psychiatry 165, 116123.Google Scholar
Hajcak, G, McDonald, N, Simons, RF (2003). Anxiety and error-related brain activity. Biological Psychology 64, 7790.Google Scholar
Hajcak, G, Simons, RF (2002). Error-related brain activity in obsessive-compulsive undergraduates. Psychiatry Research 110, 6372.Google Scholar
Hanna, GL, Carrasco, M, Harbin, SM, Nienhuis, JK, LaRosa, CE, Chen, P, Fitzgerald, KD, Gehring, WJ (2012). Error-related negativity and tic history in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 51, 902910.Google Scholar
Henderson, HA, Ono, KE, McMahon, CM, Schwartz, CB, Usher, LV, Mundy, PC (2015). The costs and benefits of self-monitoring for higher functioning children and adolescents with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 45, 548559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herrmann, MJ, Mader, K, Schreppel, T, Jacob, C, Heine, M, Boreatti-Hümmer, A, Ehlis, AC, Scheuerpflug, P, Pauli, P, Fallgatter, AJ (2010). Neural correlates of performance monitoring in adult patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). World Journal of Biological Psychiatry 11, 457464.Google Scholar
Hoexter, MQ, Diniz, JB, Lopes, AC, Batistuzzo, MC, Shavitt, RG, Dougherty, DD, Duran, FL, Bressan, RA, Busatto, GF, Miguel, EC, Sato, JR (2015). Orbitofrontal thickness as measure for treatment response prediction in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Depression and Anxiety 32, 900908.Google Scholar
Hoexter, MQ, Dougherty, DD, Shavitt, RG, D'Alcante, CC, Duran, FL, Lopes, AC, Diniz, JB, Batistuzzo, MC, Evans, KC, Bressan, RA, Busatto, GF, Miguel, EC (2013). Differential prefrontal gray matter correlates of treatment response to fluoxetine or cognitive-behavioral therapy in obsessive-compulsive disorder. European Neuropsychopharmacology 23, 569580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hogarth, L, Chase, H, Baess, K (2012). Impaired goal-directed behavioural control in human impulsivity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 65, 305316.Google Scholar
Hollander, E, Kaplan, A, Stahl, SM (2003). A double-blind, placebo-controlled trial of clonazepam in obsessive-compulsive disorder. World Journal of Biological Psychiatry 4, 3034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmes, AJ, Pizzagalli, DA (2008). Spatiotemporal dynamics of error processing dysfunctions in major depressive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry 65, 179188.Google Scholar
Holmes, AJ, Pizzagalli, DA (2010). Effects of task-relevant incentives on the electrophysiological correlates of error processing in major depressive disorder. Cognitive Affective Behavior and Neuroscience 10, 119128.Google Scholar
Holroyd, CB, Coles, MG (2002). The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychological Reviews 109, 679709.Google Scholar
Ho Pian, KL, van Megen, HJ, Ramsey, NF, Mandl, R, van Rijk, PP, Wynne, HJ, Westenberg, HG (2005). Decreased thalamic blood flow in obsessive-compulsive disorder patients responding to fluvoxamine. Psychiatry Research 138, 8997.Google Scholar
Horan, WP, Foti, D, Hajcak, G, Wynn, JK, Green, MF (2012). Impaired neural response to internal but not external feedback in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine 42, 16371647.Google Scholar
Houthoofd, S, Morrens, M, Sabbe, B, Schrijvers, D, Vandendriessche, F, Hulstijn, W, de Bruijn, ER (2013). Trait and state aspects of internal and external performance monitoring in schizophrenia. International Journal of Psychophysiology 87, 4251.Google Scholar
Hyman, SE (2007). Can neuroscience be integrated into the DSM-V? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8, 725732.Google Scholar
Hyman, SE (2010). The diagnosis of mental disorders: the problem of reification. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 6, 155179.Google Scholar
Iervolino, AC, Rijsdijk, FV, Cherkas, L, Fullana, MA, Mataix-Cols, D (2011). A multivariate twin study of obsessive-compulsive symptom dimensions. Archives of General Psychiatry 68, 637644.Google Scholar
Insel, T, Cuthbert, B, Garvey, M, Heinssen, R, Pine, DS, Quinn, K, Sanislow, C, Wang, P (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry 167, 748751.Google Scholar
Jacob, ML, Larson, MJ, Storch, EA (2014). Insight in adults with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry 55, 896903.Google Scholar
Johannes, S, Wieringa, BM, Nager, W, Dengler, R, Münte, TF (2001 a). Oxazepam alters action monitoring. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 155, 100106.Google Scholar
Johannes, S, Wieringa, BM, Nager, W, Rada, D, Dengler, R, Emrich, HM, Münte, TF, Dietrich, DE (2001 b). Discrepant target detection and action monitoring in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Research 108, 101110.Google Scholar
Jonkman, LM, van Melis, JJ, Kemner, C, Markus, CR (2007). Methylphenidate improves deficient error evaluation in children with ADHD: an event-related brain potential study. Biological Psychology 76, 217229.Google Scholar
Kansal, V, Patriciu, I, Kiang, M (2014). Illness insight and neurophysiological error-processing deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 156, 122127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klawohn, J, Riesel, A, Grützmann, R, Kathmann, N, Endrass, T (2014). Performance monitoring in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a temporo-spatial principal component analysis. Cognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 14, 983995.Google Scholar
Kopf, J, Volkert, J, Heidler, S, Dresler, T, Kittel-Schneider, S, Gessner, A, Herrmann, MJ, Ehlis, AC, Reif, A (2015). Electrophysiological evidence of a typical cognitive distortion in bipolar disorder. Cortex 66, 103114.Google Scholar
Kopp, B, Rist, F (1999). An event-related brain potential substrate of disturbed response monitoring in paranoid schizophrenic patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 108, 337346.Google Scholar
Ladouceur, CD, Dahl, RE, Birmaher, B, Axelson, DA, Ryan, ND (2006). Increased error-related negativity (ERN) in childhood anxiety disorders: ERP and source localization. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 47, 10731082.Google Scholar
Ladouceur, CD, Slifka, JS, Dahl, RE, Birmaher, B, Axelson, DA, Ryan, ND (2012). Altered error-related brain activity in youth with major depression. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 2, 351362.Google Scholar
Leucht, S, Helfer, B, Gartlehner, G, Davis, JM (2015). How effective are common medications: a perspective based on meta-analyses of major drugs. BMC Medicine 13, 253.Google Scholar
Liebowitz, MR (1987). Social phobia. Modern Problems of Pharmacopsychiatry 22, 141173.Google Scholar
Liotti, M, Pliszka, SR, Perez, R, Kothmann, D, Woldorff, MG (2005). Abnormal brain activity related to performance monitoring and error detection in children with ADHD. Cortex 41, 377388.Google Scholar
Lipszyc, J, Schachar, R (2010). Inhibitory control and psychopathology: a meta-analysis of studies using the stop signal task. Journal of the International Neuropsycholical Society 16, 10641076.Google Scholar
Liu, Y, Hanna, GL, Carrasco, M, Gehring, WJ, Fitzgerald, KD (2014). Altered relationship between electrophysiological response to errors and gray matter volumes in an extended network for error-processing in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder. Human Brain Mapping 35, 11431153.Google Scholar
Logan, G (1994). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: a user's guide to the stop signal paradigm. In Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory and Language (ed. Dagenbach, D. and Carr, T. H.), pp. 189239. Academic: San Diego.Google Scholar
Lovinger, DM (2010). Neurotransmitter roles in synaptic modulation, plasticity and learning in the dorsal striatum. Neuropharmacology 58, 951961.Google Scholar
Luijten, M, van Meel, CS, Franken, IH (2011). Diminished error processing in smokers during smoking cue exposure. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 97, 514520.Google Scholar
Mallet, L, Polosan, M, Jaafari, N, Baup, N, Welter, ML, Fontaine, D, du Montcel, ST, Yelnik, J, Chéreau, I, Arbus, C, Raoul, S, Aouizerate, B, Damier, P, Chabardès, S, Czernecki, V, Ardouin, C, Krebs, MO, Bardinet, E, Chaynes, P, Burbaud, P, Cornu, P, Derost, P, Bougerol, T, Bataille, B, Mattei, V, Dormont, D, Devaux, B, Vérin, M, Houeto, JL, Pollak, P, Benabid, AL, Agid, Y, Krack, P, Millet, B, Pelissolo, A, Group, SS (2008). Subthalamic nucleus stimulation in severe obsessive-compulsive disorder. New England Journal of Medicine 359, 21212134.Google Scholar
Marin, RS, Biedrzycki, RC, Firinciogullari, S (1991). Reliability and validity of the apathy evaluation scale. Psychiatry Research 38, 143162.Google Scholar
Mason, O, Linney, Y, Claridge, G (2005). Short scales for measuring schizotypy. Schizophrenia Research 78, 293296.Google Scholar
Mataix-Cols, D, Rauch, SL, Manzo, PA, Jenike, MA, Baer, L (1999). Use of factor-analyzed symptom dimensions to predict outcome with serotonin reuptake inhibitors and placebo in the treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 156, 14091416.Google Scholar
Mataix-Cols, D, Rosario-Campos, MC, Leckman, JF (2005). A multidimensional model of obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 162, 228238.Google Scholar
Mathalon, DH, Jorgensen, KW, Roach, BJ, Ford, JM (2009). Error detection failures in schizophrenia: ERPs and FMRI. International Journal of Psychophysiology 73, 109117.Google Scholar
Mathews, CA, Perez, VB, Roach, BJ, Fekri, S, Vigil, O, Kupferman, E, Mathalon, DH (2015). Error-related brain activity dissociates hoarding disorder from obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychological Medicine 46, 367379.Google Scholar
Minzenberg, MJ, Gomes, GC, Yoon, JH, Swaab, TY, Carter, CS (2014). Disrupted action monitoring in recent-onset psychosis patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Research 221, 114121.Google Scholar
Morris, RW, Quail, S, Griffiths, KR, Green, MJ, Balleine, BW (2015). Corticostriatal control of goal-directed action is impaired in Schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 77, 187195.Google Scholar
Morris, SE, Heerey, EA, Gold, JM, Holroyd, CB (2008). Learning-related changes in brain activity following errors and performance feedback in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 99, 274285.Google Scholar
Morris, SE, Holroyd, CB, Mann-Wrobel, MC, Gold, JM (2011). Dissociation of response and feedback negativity in schizophrenia: electrophysiological and computational evidence for a deficit in the representation of value. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 5, 123.Google Scholar
Morris, SE, Yee, CM & Nuechterlein, KH (2006). Electrophysiological analysis of error monitoring in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 115, 239250.Google Scholar
Morsel, AM, Morrens, M, Temmerman, A, Sabbe, B, de Bruijn, ER (2014). Electrophysiological (EEG) evidence for reduced performance monitoring in euthymic bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorder 16, 820829.Google Scholar
Mueller, EM, Pechtel, P, Cohen, AL, Douglas, SR, Pizzagalli, DA (2015). Potentiated processing of negative feedback in depression is attenuated by anhedonia. Depression and Anxiety 32, 296305.Google Scholar
Nestadt, G, Di, CZ, Riddle, MA, Grados, MA, Greenberg, BD, Fyer, AJ, McCracken, JT, Rauch, SL, Murphy, DL, Rasmussen, SA, Cullen, B, Pinto, A, Knowles, JA, Piacentini, J, Pauls, DL, Bienvenu, OJ, Wang, Y, Liang, KY, Samuels, JF, Roche, KB (2009). Obsessive-compulsive disorder: subclassification based on co-morbidity. Psychological Medicine 39, 14911501.Google Scholar
Nieuwenhuis, S, Nielen, MM, Mol, N, Hajcak, G, Veltman, DJ (2005). Performance monitoring in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Research 134, 111122.Google Scholar
Nuttin, BJ, Gabriëls, LA, Cosyns, PR, Meyerson, BA, Andréewitch, S, Sunaert, SG, Maes, AF, Dupont, PJ, Gybels, JM, Gielen, F, Demeulemeester, HG (2008). Long-term electrical capsular stimulation in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neurosurgery 62, 966977.Google Scholar
Olvet, DM, Klein, DN, Hajcak, G (2010). Depression symptom severity and error-related brain activity. Psychiatry Research 179, 3037.Google Scholar
Otto, AR, Gershman, SJ, Markman, AB, Daw, ND (2013). the curse of planning: dissecting multiple reinforcement-learning systems by taxing the central executive. Psychological Science 24, 751761.Google Scholar
Pallanti, S, Grassi, G, Sarrecchia, ED, Cantisani, A, Pellegrini, M (2011). Obsessive-compulsive disorder comorbidity: clinical assessment and therapeutic implications. Frontiers in Psychiatry 2, 70.Google Scholar
Patton, JH, Stanford, MS, Barratt, ES (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology 51, 768774.Google Scholar
Paulus, MP, Feinstein, JS, Simmons, A, Stein, MB (2004). Anterior cingulate activation in high trait anxious subjects is related to altered error processing during decision making. Biological Psychiatry 55, 11791187.Google Scholar
Perez, VB, Ford, JM, Roach, BJ, Woods, SW, McGlashan, TH, Srihari, VH, Loewy, RL, Vinogradov, S, Mathalon, DH (2012). Error monitoring dysfunction across the illness course of schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 121, 372387.Google Scholar
Potts, GF, George, MR, Martin, LE, Barratt, ES (2006). Reduced punishment sensitivity in neural systems of behavior monitoring in impulsive individuals. Neuroscience Letters 397, 130134.Google Scholar
Pujol, J, Soriano-Mas, C, Alonso, P, Cardoner, N, Menchon, JM, Deus, J, Vallejo, J (2004). Mapping structural brain alterations in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry 61, 720730.Google Scholar
Rabinak, CA, Holman, A, Angstadt, M, Kennedy, AE, Hajcak, G, Phan, KL (2013). Neural response to errors in combat-exposed returning veterans with and without post-traumatic stress disorder: a preliminary event-related potential study. Psychiatry Research 213, 7178.Google Scholar
Rachman, S, de Silva, P (1978). Abnormal and normal obsessions. Behaviour, Research and Therapy 16, 233248.Google Scholar
Ravindran, LN, Stein, MB (2010). The pharmacologic treatment of anxiety disorders: a review of progress. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 71, 839854.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Regier, DA, Narrow, WE, Clarke, DE, Kraemer, HC, Kuramoto, SJ, Kuhl, EA, Kupfer, DJ (2013). DSM-5 field trials in the United States and Canada, Part II: test-retest reliability of selected categorical diagnoses. American Journal of Psychiatry 170, 5970.Google Scholar
Reinhart, RM, Zhu, J, Park, S, Woodman, GF (2015). Medial-frontal stimulation enhances learning in schizophrenia by restoring prediction error signaling. Journal of Neuroscience 35, 1223212240.Google Scholar
Riba, J, Rodríguez-Fornells, A, Münte, TF, Barbanoj, MJ (2005). A neurophysiological study of the detrimental effects of alprazolam on human action monitoring. Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research 25, 554565.Google Scholar
Riesel, A, Endrass, T, Auerbach, LA, Kathmann, N (2015). overactive performance monitoring as an endophenotype for obsessive-compulsive disorder: evidence from a treatment study. American Journal of Psychiatry 172, 665673.Google Scholar
Riesel, A, Endrass, T, Kaufmann, C, Kathmann, N (2011). Overactive error-related brain activity as a candidate endophenotype for obsessive-compulsive disorder: evidence from unaffected first-degree relatives. American Journal of Psychiatry 168, 317324.Google Scholar
Robbins, TW, Gillan, CM, Smith, DG, de Wit, S, Ersche, KD (2012). Neurocognitive endophenotypes of impulsivity and compulsivity: towards dimensional psychiatry. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16, 8191.Google Scholar
Rosenthal, R (1979). The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychological Bulletin 86, 638641.Google Scholar
Ruchsow, M, Gron, G, Reuter, K, Spitzer, M, Hermle, L, Kiefer, M (2005). Error-related brain activity in patients with obsessive compulsive disorder and in healthy controls. Journal of Psychophysiology 19, 298304.Google Scholar
Ruchsow, M, Herrnberger, B, Beschoner, P, Grön, G, Spitzer, M, Kiefer, M (2006). Error processing in major depressive disorder: evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Psychiatry Research 40, 3746.Google Scholar
Ruchsow, M, Herrnberger, B, Wiesend, C, Grön, G, Spitzer, M, Kiefer, M (2004). The effect of erroneous responses on response monitoring in patients with major depressive disorder: a study with event-related potentials. Psychophysiology 41, 833840.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruchsow, M, Reuter, K, Hermle, L, Ebert, D, Kiefer, M, Falkenstein, M (2007). Executive control in obsessive-compulsive disorder: event-related potentials in a Go/Nogo task. Journal of Neural Transmission 114, 15951601.Google Scholar
Ruscio, AM, Stein, DJ, Chiu, WT, Kessler, RC (2010). The epidemiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Molecular Psychiatry 15, 5363.Google Scholar
Salkovskis, PM (1985). Obsessional-compulsive problems: a cognitive-behavioural analysis. Behaviour, Research and Therapy 23, 571583.Google Scholar
Sanislow, CA, Pine, DS, Quinn, KJ, Kozak, MJ, Garvey, MA, Heinssen, RK, Wang, PS, Cuthbert, BN (2010). Developing constructs for psychopathology research: research domain criteria. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 119, 631639.Google Scholar
Saunders, JB, Aasland, OG, Babor, TF, de la Fuente, JR, Grant, M (1993). Development of the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption–II. Addiction 88, 791804.Google Scholar
Saxena, S, Brody, AL, Ho, ML, Zohrabi, N, Maidment, KM, Baxter, LR (2003). Differential brain metabolic predictors of response to paroxetine in obsessive-compulsive disorder versus major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry 160, 522532.Google Scholar
Saxena, S, Brody, AL, Maidment, KM, Dunkin, JJ, Colgan, M, Alborzian, S, Phelps, ME, Baxter, LR (1999). Localized orbitofrontal and subcortical metabolic changes and predictors of response to paroxetine treatment in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 21, 683693.Google Scholar
Saxena, S, Gorbis, E, O'Neill, J, Baker, SK, Mandelkern, MA, Maidment, KM, Chang, S, Salamon, N, Brody, AL, Schwartz, JM, London, ED (2009). Rapid effects of brief intensive cognitive-behavioral therapy on brain glucose metabolism in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Molecular Psychiatry 14, 197205.Google Scholar
Schellekens, AF, de Bruijn, ER, van Lankveld, CA, Hulstijn, W, Buitelaar, JK, de Jong, CA, Verkes, RJ (2010). Alcohol dependence and anxiety increase error-related brain activity. Addiction 105, 19281934.Google Scholar
Schwabe, L, Wolf, OT (2009). Stress prompts habit behavior in humans. Journal of Neuroscience 29, 71917198.Google Scholar
Simmonite, M, Bates, AT, Groom, MJ, Jackson, GM, Hollis, C, Liddle, PF (2012). Error processing-associated event-related potentials in schizophrenia and unaffected siblings. International Journal of Psychophysiology 84, 7479.Google Scholar
Sjoerds, Z, de Wit, S, van den Brink, W, Robbins, TW, Beekman, AT, Penninx, BW, Veltman, DJ (2013). Behavioral and neuroimaging evidence for overreliance on habit learning in alcohol-dependent patients. Translational Psychiatry 3, e337.Google Scholar
Snider, LA, Swedo, SE (2003). Childhood-onset obsessive-compulsive disorder and tic disorders: case report and literature review. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology 13 (Suppl. 1), S81S88.Google Scholar
Snorrason, I, Berlin, GS, Lee, HJ (2015). Optimizing psychological interventions for trichotillomania (hair-pulling disorder): an update on current empirical status. Psychology Research and Behavior Management 8, 105113.Google Scholar
Snorrason, I, Lee, HJ, de Wit, S, Woods, DW (2016). Are nonclinical obsessive-compulsive symptoms associated with bias toward habits? Psychiatry Research 241, 221223.Google Scholar
Sokhadze, E, Baruth, J, El-Baz, A, Horrell, T, Sokhadze, G, Carroll, T, Tasman, A, Sears, L, Casanova, MF (2010). Impaired error monitoring and correction function in autism. Journal of Neurotherapy 14, 7995.Google Scholar
Sokhadze, E, Stewart, C, Hollifield, M, Tasman, A (2008). Event-related potential study of executive dysfunctions in a speeded reaction task in cocaine addiction. Journal of Neurotherapy 12, 185204.Google Scholar
Sokhadze, EM, Baruth, JM, Sears, L, Sokhadze, GE, El-Baz, AS, Williams, E, Klapheke, R, Casanova, MF (2012). Event-related potential study of attention regulation during illusory figure categorization task in ADHD, autism spectrum disorder, and typical children. Journal of Neurotherapy 16, 1231.Google Scholar
Soomro, GM, Altman, D, Rajagopal, S, Oakley-Browne, M (2008). Selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitors (SSRIs) versus placebo for obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD). Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, CD001765.Google Scholar
South, M, Larson, MJ, Krauskopf, E, Clawson, A (2010). Error processing in high-functioning Autism Spectrum Disorders. Biological Psychology 85, 242251.Google Scholar
Spielberger, CD, Gorsuch, RL, Lushene, R, Vagg, PR, Jacobs, GA (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press: Palo Alto, CA.Google Scholar
Steele, VR, Fink, BC, Maurer, JM, Arbabshirani, MR, Wilber, CH, Jaffe, AJ, Sidz, A, Pearlson, GD, Calhoun, VD, Clark, VP, Kiehl, KA (2014). Brain potentials measured during a Go/NoGo task predict completion of substance abuse treatment. Biological Psychiatry 76, 7583.Google Scholar
Stern, ER, Liu, Y, Gehring, WJ, Lister, JJ, Yin, G, Zhang, J, Fitzgerald, KD, Himle, JA, Abelson, JL, Taylor, SF (2010). Chronic medication does not affect hyperactive error responses in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychophysiology 47, 913920.Google Scholar
Swedo, SE, Pietrini, P, Leonard, HL, Schapiro, MB, Rettew, DC, Goldberger, EL, Rapoport, SI, Rapoport, JL, Grady, CL (1992). Cerebral glucose metabolism in childhood-onset obsessive-compulsive disorder. Revisualization during pharmacotherapy. Archives of General Psychiatry 49, 690694.Google Scholar
Swedo, SE, Schapiro, MB, Grady, CL, Cheslow, DL, Leonard, HL, Kumar, A, Friedland, R, Rapoport, SI, Rapoport, JL (1989). Cerebral glucose metabolism in childhood-onset obsessive-compulsive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry 46, 518523.Google Scholar
Swets, M, Dekker, J, van Emmerik-van Oortmerssen, K, Smid, GE, Smit, F, de Haan, L, Schoevers, RA (2014). The obsessive compulsive spectrum in schizophrenia, a meta-analysis and meta-regression exploring prevalence rates. Schizophrenia Research 152, 458468.Google Scholar
Tang, Y, Zhang, X, Simmonite, M, Li, H, Zhang, T, Guo, Q, Li, C, Fang, Y, Xu, Y, Wang, J (2013). Hyperactivity within an extensive cortical distribution associated with excessive sensitivity in error processing in unmedicated depression: a combined event-related potential and sLORETA study. International Journal of Psychophysiology 90, 282289.Google Scholar
Thase, ME, Larsen, KG, Kennedy, SH (2011). Assessing the ‘true’ effect of active antidepressant therapy v. placebo in major depressive disorder: use of a mixture model. British Journal of Psychiatry 199, 501507.Google Scholar
Torres, AR (2016). The road to improvement in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Lancet Psychiatry 3, 695697.Google Scholar
Torres, AR, Fontenelle, LF, Shavitt, RG, Ferrão, YA, do Rosário, MC, Storch, EA, Miguel, EC (2016). Comorbidity variation in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder according to symptom dimensions: results from a large multicentre clinical sample. Journal of Affective Disorders 190, 508516.Google Scholar
van den Heuvel, OA, Remijnse, PL, Mataix-Cols, D, Vrenken, H, Groenewegen, HJ, Uylings, HB, van Balkom, AJ, Veltman, DJ (2009). The major symptom dimensions of obsessive-compulsive disorder are mediated by partially distinct neural systems. Brain 132, 853868.Google Scholar
van der Wee, NJ, Ramsey, NF, van Megen, HJ, Denys, D, Westenberg, HG, Kahn, RS (2007). Spatial working memory in obsessive-compulsive disorder improves with clinical response: a functional MRI study. European Neuropsychopharmacology 17, 1623.Google Scholar
van Grootheest, DS, Boomsma, DI, Hettema, JM, Kendler, KS (2008). Heritability of obsessive-compulsive symptom dimensions. American Journal of Medical Genetics B Neuropsychiatric Genetics 147B, 473478.Google Scholar
van Meel, CS, Heslenfeld, DJ, Oosterlaan, J, Sergeant, JA (2007). Adaptive control deficits in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): the role of error processing. Psychiatry Research 151, 211220.Google Scholar
Vlamings, PH, Jonkman, LM, Hoeksma, MR, van Engeland, H, Kemner, C (2008). Reduced error monitoring in children with autism spectrum disorder: an ERP study. European Journal of Neuroscience 28, 399406.Google Scholar
Voon, V, Derbyshire, K, Rück, C, Irvine, MA, Worbe, Y, Enander, J, Schreiber, LR, Gillan, C, Fineberg, NA, Sahakian, BJ, Robbins, TW, Harrison, NA, Wood, J, Daw, ND, Dayan, P, Grant, JE, Bullmore, ET (2014). Disorders of compulsivity: a common bias towards learning habits. Molecular Psychiatry 20, 345352.Google Scholar
Weinberg, A, Klein, DN, Hajcak, G (2012). Increased error-related brain activity distinguishes generalized anxiety disorder with and without comorbid major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 121, 885896.Google Scholar
Weinberg, A, Kotov, R, Proudfit, GH (2015). Neural indicators of error processing in generalized anxiety disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder, and major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 124, 172185.Google Scholar
Weinberg, A, Olvet, DM, Hajcak, G (2010). Increased error-related brain activity in generalized anxiety disorder. Biological Psychology 85, 472480.Google Scholar
Wiersema, JR, van der Meere, JJ, Roeyers, H (2005). ERP correlates of impaired error monitoring in children with ADHD. Journal of Neural Transmission 112, 14171430.Google Scholar
Wiersema, JR, van der Meere, JJ, Roeyers, H (2009). ERP correlates of error monitoring in adult ADHD. Journal of Neural Transmission 116, 371379.Google Scholar
Wild-Wall, N, Oades, RD, Schmidt-Wessels, M, Christiansen, H, Falkenstein, M (2009). Neural activity associated with executive functions in adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). International Journal of Psychophysiology 74, 1927.Google Scholar
Xiao, Z, Wang, J, Zhang, M, Li, H, Tang, Y, Wang, Y, Fan, Q, Fromson, JA (2011). Error-related negativity abnormalities in generalized anxiety disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 35, 265272.Google Scholar
Yeung, N, Botvinick, MM, Cohen, JD (2004). The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review 111, 931959.Google Scholar
Zambrano-Vazquez, L, Allen, JJ (2014). Differential contributions of worry, anxiety, and obsessive compulsive symptoms to ERN amplitudes in response monitoring and reinforcement learning tasks. Neuropsychologia 61, 197209.Google Scholar
Zhang, JS, Wang, Y, Cai, RG, Yan, CH (2009). The brain regulation mechanism of error monitoring in impulsive children with ADHD–an analysis of error related potentials. Neuroscience Letters 460, 1115.Google Scholar
Zhou, Z, Li, C, Zhu, H (2013). An error-related negativity potential investigation of response monitoring function in individuals with internet addiction disorder. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 7, 131.Google Scholar
Zirnheld, PJ, Carroll, CA, Kieffaber, PD, O'Donnell, BF, Shekhar, A, Hetrick, WP (2004). Haloperidol impairs learning and error-related negativity in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 16, 10981112.Google Scholar
Zung, WW (1965). A self-rating depression scale. Archives of General Psychiatry 12, 6370.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Data from 706 obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) patients re-plotted with permission from Nestadt and colleagues (2009). Disorders assessed were obsessive-compulsive personality disorder (OCPD), generalized anxiety disorder (GAD), major depression (recurrent), tics, pathological skin picking, separation anxiety disorder, pathological nail biting, panic disorder, alcohol dependence, body dysmorphic disorder, substance dependence, trichotillomania, hypochondriasis, bipolar disorder. (a) Co-morbidity rates in a sample of OCD cases – 87.3% of patients met the criteria for another DSM diagnosis (note: this figure is probably higher as only select diagnoses were assessed). (b) The prevalence of specific co-morbid disorders within the OCD cohort.

Figure 1

Table 1. Summary of results for studies examining error-related negativity (ERN) across psychiatric disorders

Figure 2

Table 2. Summary of results for studies examining goal-directed learning

Figure 3

Fig. 2. Validating a trans-diagnostic dimension. (a) The strength of the association between self-report symptoms of various DSM disorders and deficits in goal-directed control. The pattern is strikingly non-specific. (b) The association between goal-directed deficits and three ‘trans-diagnostic symptom dimensions identified in a data-driven factor analysis. Factor 1 corresponds to ‘Anxious Depression’, Factor 2 is ‘Compulsive Behavior and Intrusive Thought’, and Factor 3 is ‘Social Withdrawal’. The association between Factor 2 (‘Compulsive Behavior and Intrusive Thought’) and deficits in goal-directed learning is greater than that of any of the nine DSM-inspired questionnaires and crucially, the relationship exhibits excellent specificity with respect to ‘non-compulsive’ aspects of psychopathology, i.e. Factor 1 (‘Anxious Depression’) and Factor 3 (‘Social Withdrawal’). Data reproduced with permission from Gillan et al. (2016b). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Figure 4

Fig. 3. A trans-diagnostic approach to obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). The predominant symptoms of OCD may result from complex interaction between independent trans-diagnostic dimensions, of which anxiety and compulsivity are good (but not the only) candidates. In this simplified schematic, we show just two putative trans-diagnostic psychiatric dimensions, there are of course many others. The task for basic research, outlined in the Research Domain Criteria initiative is to understand these dimensions at units of analysis, linking self-report symptoms (such as anxiety and compulsivity) to paradigms, physiology, circuits, cells, molecules and genes. GAD, Generalized anxiety disorder.

Supplementary material: File

Gillan supplementary material

Gillan supplementary material

Download Gillan supplementary material(File)
File 26.4 KB