Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-s22k5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-07T16:57:32.722Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The applicability of deadline models: Comment on Glickman, Gray, and Morales (2005)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2025

Jeffrey N. Rouder*
Affiliation:
University of Missouri-Columbia
*
Requests for reprints should be sent to Jeffrey N. Rouder, Department of Psychological Sciences, 210 McAlester Hall, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211, USA. E-mail: rouderj@missouri.edu

Abstract

Glickman, Gray, and Morales (this issue) propose a statistical model for measuring the unobserved latency of stimulus-controlled processes. The model accounts for both speed and accuracy and does so by assuming that participants set an internal deadline. If a stimulus-controlled response is not produced by the deadline, the participant then guesses. The applicability of the model is discussed in this comment. The deadline model yields specific predictions for the case in which stimulus difficulty is manipulated in a within-block manner. In this case, it is reasonable to assume that stimulus difficulty does not affect the deadline. It is shown that in common perceptual and cognitive domains, extant data do not fully meet these predictions. Hence, practitioners need be aware of the possibility and consequences of model misspecification.

Type
Original Paper
Copyright
Copyright © 2005 The Psychometric Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

This research is supported by NSF grant SES - 0095919 to J. Rouder, D. Sun, and P. Speckman.

References

Anderson, J.A. (1991). Why, having so many neurons, do we have so few thoughts?. In Hockley, W.E., Lewandowsky, S. (Eds.), Relating Theory and Data: Essays on Human Memory in Honor of Bennet B Murdock (pp. 477507). Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Brown, S., Heathcote, A. (in press). A ballistic model of choice response time. Psychological Review.Google Scholar
Espinoza-Varas, B., Watson, C. (1994). Effects of decision criterion on latencies of binary decisions. Perception Psychophysics, 55, 190203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luce, R.D. (1986). Response Times. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ollman, R.T., Billington, M.J. (1972). The deadline model for simple reaction times. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 311336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ratcliff, R., Rouder, J.N. (1998). Modeling response times for decisions between two choices. Psychological Science, 9, 347356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rouder, J.N., Sun, D., Speckman, P.L., Lu, J., Zhou, D. (2003). A hierarchical Bayesian statistical framework for skewed variables with an application to response time distributions. Psychometrika, 68, 587604.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rouder, J., Lu, J., Speckman, P.L., Sun, D., Jiang, Y. (in press). A hierarchical model for estimating response time distributions. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review.Google Scholar
Ruthruff, E. (1996). A test of the deadline model for speed-accuracy tradeoffs. Perception Psychophysics, 55, 5664.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, P.L., Vickers, D. (1988). The accumulator model of two choice discrimination. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 32, 135168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Usher, M., McClelland, J.L. (2001). On the time course of perceptual choice: The leaky competing accumulator model. Psychological Review, 108, 550592.Google Scholar