Hostname: page-component-5f745c7db-8qdnt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-06T22:05:58.483Z Has data issue: true hasContentIssue false

From Eye Movement to Cognition: Toward a General Framework of Inference Comment on Liechty et al., 2003

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2025

Gary Feng*
Affiliation:
Duke University
*
Requests for reprints should be sent to Gary Feng, Department of Psychology, Social and Health Sciences, Box 90085, Durham NC 27708-0085. E-Mail:garyfeng@duke.edu

Abstract

Liechty, Pieters & Wedel (2003) developed a hidden Markov Model (HMM) to identify the states of an attentional process in an advertisement viewing task. This work is significant because it demonstrates the benefits of stochastic modeling and Bayesian estimation in making inferences about cognitive processes based on eye movement data. One limitation of the proposed approach is that attention is conceptualized as an autonomous random process that is affected neither by the overall layout of the stimulus nor by the visual information perceived during the current fixation. An alternative model based on the input-output hidden Markov model (IOHMM; Bengio, 1999) is suggested as an extension of the HMM. The need for further studies that validate the HMM classification results is also discussed.

Type
Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © 2003 The Psychometric Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Althoff, R.R., Cohen, N.J. (1999). Eye movement-based memory effect: A reprocessing effect in face perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 9971010.Google ScholarPubMed
Bengio, Y. (1999). Markovian models for sequential data. Neural Computing Surveys, 2, 129162.Google Scholar
Bengio, Y., Frasconi, P. (1996). Input/output HMMs for sequence processing. IEEE Transactions on Neural Networks, 7(5), 12311249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, C.S., Maglio, P.P. (2001). A robust algorithm for reading detection. Orlando, FL: Association for Computing Machinery.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feng, G., Miller, K.F., Zhang, H., Shu, H. (2001). Rowed to recovery: The use of phonological and orthographic information in reading Chinese and English. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 10791100.Google ScholarPubMed
Ghahramani, Z. (2001). An introduction to hidden Markov models and Bayesian networks. International Journal of Pattern Recognition and Artificial Intelligence, 15, 942.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henderson, J.M., Hollingworth, A. (1998). Eye movements during scene viewing: An overview. In Underwood, G. (Eds.), Eye guidance in reading and scene perception (pp. 269293). Oxford, UK: Anonima Romana.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jordan, M., Ghahramani, Z., Saul, L.K. (1997). Hidden Markov decision trees. In Mozer, M.C., Jordan, M.I., Petsche, T. (Eds.), Advances in neural information processing systems (pp. 501507). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jordan, M.I., Ghahramani, Z., Jaakkola, T.S., Saul, L.K. (1998). An introduction to variational methods for graphical models. In Jordan, M. (Eds.), Learning in graphical models (pp. 105159). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legge, G.E., Klitz, T.S., Tjan, B.S. (1997). Mr. Chips: An ideal-observer model of reading. Psychological Review, 104, 524553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liechty, J., Pieters, R., Wedel, M. (2003). Global and local covert visual attention: Evidence from a Bayesian hidden Markov model. Psychmetrika, 68, xxxxxx.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murphy, K. (2001, June). The Bayes net toolbox for Matlab. Paper presented at the Interface of Computer Science and Statistics, Costa Mesa, CA.Google Scholar
Murphy, K. (2002). Dynamic Bayesian networks: Representation, inference and learning. Berkeley: University of California.Google Scholar
Murphy, K., & Paskin, M. (2001, December). Linear time inference in hierarchical HMMs. Paper presented at the Neural Information Processing Systems, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada.Google Scholar
Rabiner, L. (1989). A tutorial on hidden Markov models and selected applications in speech recognition. Proceedings of the IEEE, 77, 257285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 372422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reichle, E.D., Pollatsek, A., Fisher, D.L., Rayner, K. (1998). Toward a model of eye movement control in reading. Psychological Review, 105, 125157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reichle, E.D., Rayner, K., Pollatsek, A. (2003). The E-Z Reader model of eye movement control in reading: Comparisons to other models. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 26, 445476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reilly, R.G., O'Regan, J.K. (1998). Eye movement control during reading: A simulation of some word-targeting strategies. Vision Research, 38, 303317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reingold, E., Loschky, L.C. (2002). Saliency of peripheral targets in gaze-contingent multiresolutional displays. Behavior Research Methods & Instrumentation, 34, 491499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salvucci, D.D. (1999). Mapping eye movements to cognitive processes. Pittsburgh, PA.: Carnegie-Mellon University.Google Scholar
See Machines (2003). faceLab 3.0. Canberra, Australia: Author.Google Scholar
Sereno, S.C., Rayner, K., Posner, M.I. (1998). Establishing a time-line of word recognition: Evidence from eye movements and event-related potentials. Neuroreport: An International Journal for the Rapid Communication of Research in Neuroscience, 9, 21952200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wooding, D.S., Mugglestone, M.D., Purdy, K.J., Gale, A.G. (2002). Eye movements of large populations: I. Implementation and performance of an autonomous public eye tracker. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 34, 509517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed