Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-g4j75 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-07T18:32:55.808Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A Mathematical Model for Conditioning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2025

G. W. Boguslavsky*
Affiliation:
Cornell University

Abstract

It is postulated that occurrence of a conditioned response depends on recurrence of one of a finite number of specific vigilance reactions. Number of trial on which a conditioned response occurs is shown to be a sufficient statistic for estimating the number of such vigilance reactions. The hypothesis is tested by noting whether numbers of trials on which conditioned responses occur fall within confidence intervals determined on the basis of a selected sufficient statistic. Applications of the model to psychological research are suggested.

Type
Original Paper
Copyright
Copyright © 1955 The Psychometric Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

From a doctoral dissertation at Cornell University. The author wishes to acknowledge the invaluable advice and help of Professor H. S. Liddell, under whose direction this research was conducted. A special debt of gratitude is due to Dr. Jack Kiefer of the Cornell department of mathematics, whose skill and interest aided materially in the development of the mathematical portions of this paper.

References

Bridgman, P. W. Some general principles of operational analysis. Psychol. Rev., 1945, 52, 246249CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denny-Brown, D. Selected writings of Sir Charles Sherrington, New York: Hoeber, 1940Google Scholar
Feller, W. An introduction to probability theory and its applications, New York: Wiley, 1950Google Scholar
Fraenkel, G. S., and Gunn, D. L.. The orientation of animals, Oxford: Clarendon Press, 1940Google Scholar
Guthrie, E. R. The psychology of learning, New York: Harper, 1952Google Scholar
Halmos, P. R., and Savage, L. J.. Application of the Randon-Nikodym theorem to the theory of sufficient statistics. Ann. math. Statist., 1949, 20, 225241CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, H. E. Cardiac output: regulation and estimation. In Fulton, J. F. (Eds.), A textbook of physiology (pp. 660680). Philadelphia: Saunders, 1950Google Scholar
Krechevsky, I. “Hypotheses” in rats. Psychol. Rev., 1932, 39, 516532CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liddell, H. S. Some specific factors that modify tolerance for environmental stress. Proc. Ass. Res. nerv. ment. Dis., 1949, 29, 155171Google ScholarPubMed
Liddell, H. S. The influence of experimental neuroses on respiratory function. In Abramson, H. A. (Eds.), Somatic and psychiatric treatment of asthma, Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins, 1951Google Scholar
Mood, A. M. Introduction to the theory of statistics, New York: McGraw-Hill, 1950Google Scholar
Pavlov, I. P. Conditioned reflexes, London: Oxford Univ. Press, 1927Google Scholar
Pavlov, I. P. Lectures on conditioned reflexes, New York: International, 1928Google Scholar
Pavlov, I. P. The reply of a physiologist to psychologists. Psychol. Rev., 1932, 39, 91127CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pratt, C. C. Operationism in psychology. Psychol. Rev., 1945, 52, 262269CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sherrington, C. S. The integrative action of the nervous system, New Haven: Yale Univ. Press, 1947Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. Are theories of learning necessary?. Psychol. Rev., 1950, 57, 193216CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spence, K. W. The nature of discrimination learning in animals. Psychol. Rev., 1936, 43, 427449CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Voitonis, N. Predistoriia intellekta, Moscow: Akad. Nauk, 1949Google Scholar