Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-mzp66 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-07T18:09:56.305Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Multivariate Stochastic Processes Compatible with “Aspect” Models of Similarity and Choice

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2025

A. A. J. Marley*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology McGill University
*
Requests for reprints should be addressed to A.A.J.Marley, Dept. of Psychology, McGill University, 1205 Avenue Dr. Penfield, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 1B1, Canada.

Abstract

Various recent works have developed “feature” or “aspect” models of similarity and preference. These models are more concerned with the fine detail of the judgment process than were prior models, but nevertheless they have not in general developed an underlying stochastic process compatible with the assumed structure. In this paper, we show that a particular class of multivariate stochastic processes, namely those associated with the Marshall-Olkin multivariate exponential distribution, generates several of these models. In particular, such stochastic processes (appropriately interpreted) yield Tversky's elimination by aspects model, Edgell and Geisler's (normal) additive random aspects model, and Shepard and Arabie's additive cluster model.

Type
Original Paper
Copyright
Copyright © 1981 The Psychometric Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

This work was supported by Natural Science and Engineering Research Council of Canada Grant A8124 to A.A.J. Marley.

References

Reference Note

Marley, A. A. J. The compatibility between random utility models of choice and competing causes models of reaction time. Manuscript, Department of Psychology, McGill University, 1976.Google Scholar

References

Arnold, B. C. Parameter estimation for a multivariate exponential distribution. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 1968, 63, 848852.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berman, S. M. Note on extreme values, competing risks and semi-markov processes. Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 1963, 34, 11041106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Block, H. D., & Marschak, J. Random orderings and stochastic theories of responses. In Olkin, I., Ghurye, S., Hoeffding, W., Madow, W., Mann, H. (Eds.), Contributions to probability and statistics, 1960, Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Edgell, S. E., & Geisler, W. S. A set-theoretic random utility model of choice behaviour. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 1980, 22, 265278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galambos, J., & Kotz, S. Characterizations of probability distributions, 1978, Berlin: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luce, R. D., & Green, D. M. A neural timing theory for response times and the psychophysics of intensity. Psychological Review, 1972, 79, 1457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luce, R. D., & Suppes, P. Preference, utility, and subjective probability. In Luce, R. D., Bush, R. R., & Galanter, E. (Eds.), Handbook of Mathematical Psychology, 1965, New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Petrusic, W. M., & Jamieson, D. G. Relation between probability of preferential choice and time to choose changes with practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1978, 4, 471482.Google Scholar
Shepard, R. N., & Arabie, P. Additive clustering: Representation of similarities as combinations of discrete overlapping properties. Psychological Review, 1979, 86, 87123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A. Elimination by aspects: A theory of choice. Psychological Review, 1972, 79, 281299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A. Choice by elimination. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 1972, 9, 341367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A. Features of similarity. Psychological Review, 1977, 84, 327352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar