Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-hvd4g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-07T18:17:34.274Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

On the Construction of Weak Orders from Fragmentary Information

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2025

Peter C. Fishburn*
Affiliation:
The Pennsylvania State University

Abstract

An iterative method is proposed for constructing a weak order from a partial order on a set of stimuli that is based on individual pairwise comparison data. The method generalizes Duncan Luce's construction of the weak order induced by a semiorder. Various aspects of the iterative procedure are discussed, including its rationale, the number of iterations required to obtain a weak order, and the extent to which the data support additions to the initial partial order as a function of the number of iterations performed before the additions occur.

Type
Original Paper
Copyright
Copyright © 1973 The Psychometric Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

This work was supported in part by a grant from the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation to The Institute for Advanced Study.

The computer simulation programs for this study were written and run by William Gehrlein. I am grateful to him for his assistance.

References

Coombs, C. H. A theory of data, 1964, New York: WileyGoogle Scholar
Fishburn, P. C. Utility theory with inexact preferences and degrees of preference. Synthese, 1970, 21, 204221CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fishburn, P. C. Intransitive indifference with unequal indifference intervals. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 1970, 7, 144149CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galanter, E. An axiomatic and experimental study of sensory order and measure. Psychological Review, 1956, 63, 1628CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, N. The structure of appearance, 1951, Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
Hodson, F. R., Kendall, D. G. and TĂutu, P. Mathematics in the archaeological and historical sciences, 1971, Edinburgh: Edinburgh Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
Luce, R. D. Semiorders and a theory of utility discrimination. Econometrica, 1956, 24, 178191CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luce, R. D. and Galanter, E. Discrimination. In Luce, R. D., Bush, R. R., Galanter, E. (Eds.), Handbook of mathematical psychology, 1963, New York: WileyGoogle Scholar
Luce, R. D. and Suppes, P. Preference, utility, and subjective probability. In Luce, R. D., Bush, R. R., and Galanter, E. (Eds.), Handbook of mathematical psychology, 1965, New York: WileyGoogle Scholar
Scott, D. Measurement structures and linear inequalities. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 1964, 1, 233247CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, D. and Suppes, P. Foundational aspects of theories of measurement. Journal of Symbolic Logic, 1958, 23, 113128CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shepard, R. N. Circularity in judgments of relative pitch. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1964, 36, 23462353CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegel, S. A method for obtaining an ordered metric scale. Psychometrika, 1956, 21, 207216CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szpilrajn, E. Sur l'extension de l'ordre partiel. Fundamenta Mathematicae, 1930, 16, 386389CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A. Intransitivity of preferences. Psychological Review, 1969, 76, 3148CrossRefGoogle Scholar