Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-mzp66 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-07T18:39:04.315Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A Truncated-Probit Item Response Model for Estimating Psychophysical Thresholds

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2025

Richard D. Morey*
Affiliation:
University of Groningen
Jeffrey N. Rouder
Affiliation:
University of Missouri
Paul L. Speckman
Affiliation:
University of Missouri
*
Requests for reprints should be sent to Richard D. Morey, DPMG, University of Groningen, Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712TS Groningen, The Netherlands. E-mail: r.d.morey@rug.nl
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

Human abilities in perceptual domains have conventionally been described with reference to a threshold that may be defined as the maximum amount of stimulation which leads to baseline performance. Traditional psychometric links, such as the probit, logit, and t, are incompatible with a threshold as there are no true scores corresponding to baseline performance. We introduce a truncated probit link for modeling thresholds and develop a two-parameter IRT model based on this link. The model is Bayesian and analysis is performed with MCMC sampling. Through simulation, we show that the model provides for accurate measurement of performance with thresholds. The model is applied to a digit-classification experiment in which digits are briefly flashed and then subsequently masked. Using parameter estimates from the model, individuals’ thresholds for flashed-digit discrimination is estimated.

Type
Original Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NC
This article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Noncommercial License which permits any noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and source are credited.
Copyright
Copyright © 2009 The Psychometric Society

Footnotes

This research is part of the first author’s Ph.D. thesis from the University of Missouri. We thank Mike Pratte and Andrew Kent for help in running the reported experiment. This research is supported by NSF grant SES-0351523 and NIMH grant R01-MH071418 and an Adeline Hoffman Fellowship from the University of Missouri.

References

Abrams, R., Klinger, M., Greenwald, A. (2002). Subliminal words activate semantic categories (not automated motor responses). Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 100106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albert, J.H., Chib, S. (1993). Bayesian analysis of binary and polychotomous response data. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 88, 669679.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andersen, E.B. (1973). Conditional inference for multiple-choice questionnaires. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 26, 3144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breitmeyer, B.G. (1984). Visual masking: An integrative approach, London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le Clech, G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G. (1998). Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature, 395, 597600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eimer, M., Schlaghecken, F. (2002). Links between conscious awareness and response inhibition: Evidence from masked priming. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 514520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelfand, A., Smith, A.F.M. (1990). Sampling based approaches to calculating marginal densities. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 85, 398409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelman, A., Carlin, J.B., Stern, H.S., Rubin, D.B. (2004). Bayesian data analysis, (2nd ed.). London: Chapman and Hall.Google Scholar
Geman, S., Geman, D. (1984). Stochastic relaxation. Gibbs distribution, and the Bayesian restoration of images. IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence, 6, 721741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geweke, J. (1992). Evaluating the accuracy of sampling-based approaches to the calculation of posterior moments. In Bernardo, J.M., Berger, J.O., Dawid, A.P., Smith, A.F.M. (Eds.), Bayesian statistics 4, London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Liu, S.J., Sabatti, C. (2000). Generalised Gibbs sampler and multigrid Monte Carlo for Bayesian computation. Biometrika, 87, 353369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lord, F.M., Novick, M.R. (1968). Statistical theories of mental test scores, Reading: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Morey, R.D., Rouder, J.N., Speckman, P.L. (2008). A statistical model for discriminating between subliminal and near-liminal performance. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 52, 2136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rasch, G. (1960). Probabilistic models for some intelligence and attainment tests, Copenhagen: Danish Institute for Educational Research.Google Scholar
Reingold, E.M., Merikle, P.M. (1988). Using direct and indirect measures to study perception without awareness. Perception & Psychophysics, 44, 563575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rouder, J.N., Lu, J. (2005). An introduction to Bayesian hierarchical models with an application in the theory of signal detection. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12, 573604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rouder, J.N., Morey, R.D., Speckman, P.L., Pratte, M.S. (2007). Detecting chance: A solution to the null sensitivity problem in subliminal priming. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14, 597605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snodgrass, M., Bernat, E., Shevrin, H. (2004). Unconscious perception: A model-based approach to method and evidence. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 888895.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spiegelhalter, D.J., Best, N.G., Carlin, B.P., van der Linde, A. (2002). Bayesian measures of model complexity and fit (with discussion). Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B (Statistical Methodology), 64, 583639.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swaminathan, H., Gifford, J.A. (1982). Bayesian estimation in the Rasch model. Journal of Educational Statistics, 7, 175191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, M.M., Creelman, C.D. (1967). PEST: Efficient estimates on probability functions. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 41, 782787.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tierney, L. (1994). Markov chains for exploring posterior distributions. Annals of Statistics, 22, 17011728.Google Scholar
Verhelst, N.D., Glas, C.A.W. (1995). The one parameter logistic model. In Fischer, G.H., Molenaar, I.W. (Eds.), Rasch models: Foundations, recent developments, and applications, New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Vorberg, D., Mattler, U., Heinecke, A., Schmidt, T., Schwarzbach, J. (2003). Different time course for the visual perception and action priming. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100, 62756280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, A.B., Pelli, D.G. (1983). QUEST: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method. Perception & Psychophysics, 33, 113120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar