Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-29T04:16:07.188Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A commentary on current practice in mediating variable analyses in behavioural nutrition and physical activity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 August 2009

Ester Cerin*
Affiliation:
Children’s Nutrition Research Center, Baylor College of Medicine, 1100 Bates Street, Houston, TX 77030, USA
David P MacKinnon
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, USA
*
*Corresponding author: Email cerin@bcm.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

To critique current practice in, and provide recommendations for, mediating variable analyses (MVA) of nutrition and physical activity behaviour change.

Strategy

Theory-based behavioural nutrition and physical activity interventions aim at changing mediating variables that are hypothesized to be responsible for changes in the outcome of interest. MVA are useful because they help to identify the most promising theoretical approaches, mediators and intervention components for behaviour change. However, the current literature suggests that MVA are often inappropriately conducted, poorly understood and inadequately presented. Main problems encountered in the published literature are explained and suggestions for overcoming weaknesses of current practice are proposed.

Conclusion

The use of the most appropriate, currently available methods of MVA, and a correct, comprehensive presentation and interpretation of their findings, is of paramount importance for understanding how obesity can be treated and prevented.

Type
Research Paper
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2008

There appears to be no better time to evaluate our current practices in analyses of mediators of dietary and physical activity behaviour than now. Following the modest performance of obesity prevention programmes(Reference Flynn, McNeil, Maloff, Mutasingwa, Wu, Ford and Tough1) and in recognition of the importance of identifying the most effective mechanisms and strategies for change(Reference Baranowski, Anderson and Carmack2), the amount of research output on potential mediators of dietary and physical activity behaviour is increasing rapidly. The promise of analysis of mediating variables is the objective testing of theoretical processes in diet and physical activity prevention programmes. If these critical mediating processes can be identified, programmes may be improved by identifying important components and the cost of the programmes may be reduced by removing ineffective components. Ideally, true theoretical mediating mechanisms will emerge across studies and across behavioural interventions. Although mediation analysis is promising, before potentially going too far in delivering less-than-optimal communications, we need to evaluate the informational value and soundness of our current analytical practices and their interpretation. The aim of the current paper is to present general strategies that will help enhance the current quality of reports on mechanisms of obesity-related behaviour change. The paper starts with a brief overview of Baron and Kenny’s(Reference Baron and Kenny3) method of mediating variables analysis (MVA), then summarizes recent criticisms of their approach, and finally proposes ways to improve the presentation and interpretation of results from MVA in obesity-related behaviour research.

Current practice: using the Baron and Kenny method

Baron and Kenny’s(Reference Baron and Kenny3) seminal paper on mediators and moderators is by far the most cited published piece of work within studies investigating mediators of nutrition and physical activity behaviour. According to their not always correctly interpreted conceptual framework, to function as a mediator, a variable must meet four criteria.

  1. 1. An independent variable (e.g. fat-intake intervention) must be significantly associated with the potential mediator (e.g. change in self-efficacy for a low-fat diet); this relationship is operationalized as the α regression coefficient (also termed ‘path coefficient’ in the language of structural equation modelling; see Fig. 1).

    Fig. 1 Diagram for analysis of the effect of a fat-intake intervention on changes in fat intake mediated by self-efficacy for a low-fat diet

  2. 2. The potential mediator (self-efficacy) must be significantly associated with the outcome (e.g. change in fat intake), after adjustment for the independent variable; this is represented by the β coefficient (see Fig. 1).

  3. 3. After adjustment for the potential mediator (change in self-efficacy), a previously significant relationship between the outcome (change in fat intake) and the independent variable (fat-intake intervention) is no longer significant or is significantly attenuated; this is represented by the τ′ coefficient (see Fig. 1).

  4. 4. The third condition implies a significant relationship between the outcome (change in fat intake) and the independent variable (fat-intake intervention), which constitutes the fourth criterion; this relationship is represented by the τ coefficient (see Fig. 1). All of these coefficients are best obtained using regression models or structural equation modelling.

In the domain of behavioural prevention programmes, an evaluation of the first relationship (α coefficient) is termed ‘action theory test’ and informs us on the extent to which we were successful in manipulating the hypothesized mechanisms of behaviour change. The second relationship (β coefficient) represents a ‘conceptual theory test’, i.e. the extent to which there is empirical support for our conceptual theory (e.g. Social Cognitive Theory) that changes in the hypothesized mechanisms cause changes in behaviour(Reference Chen4). The italicized text above highlights important aspects of MVA that are sometimes overlooked by researchers claiming to follow Baron and Kenny’s method.

Misinterpreting Baron and Kenny

It is not uncommon to see studies published in very respectable journals reporting a significant association between the mediator and the outcome, unadjusted for the effects of the independent variable, as one of the criteria for mediation(Reference Miller, Trost and Brown5Reference Prodaniuk, Plotnikoff, Spence and Wilson8). The source of confusion appears to be Baron and Kenny’s unfortunate omission of the words ‘controlling or adjusting for the independent variable’ in the first part of their well-cited article (p. 1176), while later (p. 1177) they clearly explain that the second criterion of mediation (see above) needs to be tested by ‘regressing the dependent variable on both the independent variable and on the mediator’(Reference Baron and Kenny3). The reason for this necessary adjustment is that the mediator and the outcome may be related because they are both caused by the independent variable (in which case, there is no mediation) and not because the independent variable influences the mediator, which then influences the outcome(Reference Kenny, Kashy and Bolger9). Consequently, our first take-home message is that to test mediation according to the Baron and Kenny method it is unnecessary to examine the unadjusted relationship between the mediator and the outcome. However, it is necessary to establish that the mediator is related to the outcome independently of the independent variable.

Beyond the four criteria of mediation: in search for a measure of mediated effect

Although a formal statistical test of significance of the mediated effect (sometimes called ‘indirect effect’) is not one of the explicit criteria of mediation proposed by Baron and Kenny, they nevertheless note that a significantly attenuated independent variable–outcome relationship, following adjustment for the potential mediator, is a necessary condition for mediation. The magnitude of the attenuation is a measure of the mediated effect. The question is: what do we mean by significant mediated effect (or attenuation) and why is this information important? We argue that truly ‘significant’ mediators are those that satisfy both requirements of statistical and substantive or clinical significance. While statistical significance ensures that we identify reliable mediators, substantive or clinical significance ensures that we focus our efforts on finding and manipulating mechanisms that produce the largest effects in terms of behaviour or health outcomes, thus maximizing the cost-effectiveness of our interventions. Here is where the vast majority of published work runs into problems.

Let us start with statistical significance. There are many ways in which we can test the statistical significance of a mediated effect(Reference MacKinnon10, Reference MacKinnon, Fairchild and Fritz11). In general, if a variable meets all four criteria of mediation as per Baron and Kenny, we can confidently conclude that it is a reliable mediator. In fact, simulation studies have shown Baron and Kenny’s method to be one the most conservative tests of mediation(Reference MacKinnon, Fairchild and Fritz11, Reference MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West and Sheet12). However, as explained later in more detail, the downside of this overly conservative approach is that, in some circumstances, it can fail to identify reliable and substantively meaningful mediators(Reference Cerin, Taylor, Leslie and Owen13). On the other hand, large-scale studies may produce statistically significant but substantively or clinically trivial mediating effects. This is why it is important for studies adopting this particular MVA framework to go beyond the four criteria of mediation and explicitly report the observed mediated effect for each examined mediator. This needs to be done using simple, single-mediator as well as complex, multiple-mediator models evaluating the independent contribution of each mediating variable. Importantly, such practice is essential if solid knowledge about mechanism and strategies of behaviour change is to be swiftly accumulated and synthesized with the aid of meta-analytical procedures. Any alternative course of action is bound to unnecessarily slow down the progress in this area of research.

A measure of a mediated effect can be obtained by computing the product of α coefficient (independent-variable effect on the mediator) and β coefficient (mediator effect on the outcome adjusted for the independent variable; see Fig. 1). This is termed ‘product-of-coefficient estimate’ of a mediated effect(Reference MacKinnon10Reference MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West and Sheet12). When using ordinary least squares regression in single-mediator models, it is algebraically equivalent to computing the reduction in the independent-variable effect after inclusion of the mediator in the regression model(Reference MacKinnon10Reference MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West and Sheet12). This is named a ‘difference-of-coefficients estimate’. These estimates of mediated effect represent measures of the effect of the independent variable on the outcome via the mediator (the proposed mechanism) in the units used to measure the outcome (which implies that unstandardized regression coefficients have been used to compute them); hence their appeal and informative value.

For instance, if an intervention produced a decrease in fat intake of 10 g/d, and after including self-efficacy for a low-fat diet in the model this effect is reduced to 4 g/d, we would state that the effect of the intervention mediated by self-efficacy was a decrease in fat intake of 6 g/d. In other words, the intervention yielded a reduction in fat intake of 6 g/d likely because of its effect on self-efficacy. In terms of product-of-coefficient estimate, if the intervention has an average effect on self-efficacy of 2 units on a 5-point scale (α coefficient) and the independent effect of self-efficacy is a decrease in fat of 3 g/d per unit increase in self-efficacy (β coefficient; conceptual theory test), by multiplying these estimates we obtain a mediated effect of the intervention through self-efficacy of 2 × (−3) g/d=−6 g/d (i.e. a decrease in fat intake of 6 g/d). These values inform us of the extent to which self-efficacy is important in changing the target behaviour (3 g/d per unit change in self-efficacy; conceptual theory test), our ability to change self-efficacy (a 2 unit change on a 5 point-scale; action theory test), and the expected benefit of targeting self-efficacy with a specific type of intervention within a specific target population (a decrease in fat intake of 6 g/d). Hence, the second take-home message of the present paper, especially relevant to intervention studies, is that it is important to report estimates of mediated effects as well as action and conceptual theory tests (see Haerens et al.(Reference Haerens, Cerin, Maes, Cardon, Deforche and De Bourdeaudhuij14) for an example).

Beyond the four criteria of mediation: a special note on the ‘dichotomous’ case

Dichotomous outcome variables, such as being overweight v. being of normal weight or meeting v. not meeting the physical activity recommendations for general health, are often used in behavioural nutrition and physical activity research. In such cases, assuming that the mediator is a continuous variable, a mediating effect can be computed using a product-of-coefficient test based on the estimate of α from ordinary least squares regression and β from logistic or probit regression(Reference MacKinnon10, Reference MacKinnon, Fairchild and Fritz11). This method has been found to be robust against departures from the logistic or probit assumptions as well as normality assumptions for the distribution of the mediator(Reference MacKinnon, Lockwood, Brown, Wang and Hoffman15). Importantly, for dichotomous outcomes, the difference-of-coefficient method yields incorrect results and, hence, its use is not recommended(Reference MacKinnon, Fairchild and Fritz11, Reference MacKinnon, Lockwood, Brown, Wang and Hoffman15). The same applies to the commonly used approach to MVA based on an examination of the presence of a reduction in the regression coefficient of the independent variable (e.g. intervention) after inclusion of the mediator in the logistic regression model (criterion 3 of the Baron–Kenny method). Specifically, when using these methods, larger actual mediated effects may paradoxically result in smaller or nil estimates of the mediated effect or a reduction in coefficients. This effect is due to the difference in scales of the logistic regression coefficients of the outcome on the independent variable adjusting and not adjusting for the mediator, since these coefficients are derived from separate logistic regression equations (with a fixed error variance). Hence, the potential negative consequences of using Baron and Kenny’s method to conduct MVA with a dichotomous outcome are particularly serious. The third take-home message of the paper is to use the product-of-coefficient method when dealing with dichotomous outcome variables.

Beyond the four criteria of mediation: confidence intervals

Any statistical estimate of effect is accompanied by a measure of its accuracy (i.e. a standard error), which allows for the computation of the range of plausible values (i.e. the confidence interval) of the population parameter (i.e. mediated effect) supported by the data. Confidence intervals tell us how confident we can be that the observed estimate of effect corresponds to the effect in the target population. They provide clearer information than the usual P values because the latter are a confounded mixture of effect size and sample size, while the width of the former is determined by sample size and variability in effect of interest (determining accuracy). This also applies to the estimate of the mediated effect. Using the fat-intake example, a 95 % confidence interval of fat intake ranging from −1 to −11 g/d would indicate that the plausible population effects of the intervention on fat intake through self-efficacy are a decrease of fat intake from 1 to 11 g/d.

The information given by the point estimate and confidence limits of the mediated effect is very useful in guiding efforts to enhance theory and practice of behaviour change. For instance, a clinically meaningful mediated effect accompanied by large confidence limits indicates four main possible sources of problems: (i) insufficient sample size; (ii) large measurement error; (iii) collinearity between the independent variable and the mediator affecting the accuracy of the β coefficient estimate(Reference Baron and Kenny3); (iv) large inter-individual variations in the effects of the intervention on the mediator or/and the effects of the mediator on the outcome. In contrast, a clinically trivial but accurate (with a narrow confidence interval) estimate of mediated effect points directly at problems with action (the effectiveness of our intervention in changing the mediator) and conceptual theory (theoretical mechanisms of change). Thus our fourth take-home message is: report confidence intervals of mediated effects and interpret them conjointly with their point estimates.

How do we obtain confidence limits of mediated effects? Baron and Kenny(Reference Baron and Kenny3) provided a formula for the standard error of the mediated effect based on Sobel(Reference Sobel16). The use of the formula assumes that the mediated effect is approximately normally distributed. This solution is less than optimal because, unless the ratio of the α and/or β coefficients to their standard errors is equal or greater than 6, mediated effects are not normally distributed(Reference MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West and Sheet12, Reference Bollen and Stine17Reference Shrout and Bolger19). However, it is very easy to implement by those less versed in more sophisticated methods of MVA. All it requires are the point estimates and standard errors of α and β coefficients (see Preacher’s (2003) online program at http://www.psych.ku.edu/preacher/sobel/sobel.htm and associated ‘warnings’).

More optimal and recommended solutions involve the computation of asymmetric confidence intervals of mediated effects using a recent program developed by MacKinnon et al.(Reference MacKinnon, Fritz, Williams and Lockwood20) (which can be found at http://www.public.asu.edu/~davidpm/ripl/Prodclin) and the use of bootstrapping resampling techniques(Reference Bollen and Stine17, Reference Shrout and Bolger19, Reference Preacher and Hayes21). Bootstrapping allows the distribution of the mediated effect to be estimated empirically by using information from the original sample (treated as a pseudo population)(Reference Efron and Tibshirani22). It is particularly recommended for non-normally distributed variables, such as mediated effects, for which parametric inferential statistics may produce biased estimates. This method consists in drawing with replacement a large number of bootstrap samples (e.g. 1000) from the original sample; estimating mediated effects for each of these samples; averaging the effect estimates across all of the bootstrap samples; and computing the 95 % percentile confidence intervals for the mediated effects across all of the bootstrap samples(Reference Efron and Tibshirani22). Mediators whose confidence limits do not include zero are considered reliable (statistically significant). It is also recommended that the more accurate bias-corrected bootstrap confidence intervals, corrected for the difference between the original estimate and the bootstrap mean estimate, be computed(Reference Pituch, Stapleton and Kang23) (see Cerin and Leslie(Reference Cerin and Leslie24) for examples in the field of physical activity).

Is it really necessary to test the association between the independent variable and the outcome in mediating variable analysis?

The ultimate aim of MVA is to establish whether an independent variable influences the outcome through its effect on a mediating variable. If this is the case, do we really need to show that the independent variable is related to the outcome (fourth criterion of mediation as per Baron and Kenny)? The answer is no. We might be legitimately interested in the overall association of, for instance, socio-economic status and physical activity behaviour. However, a significant association between the two is not a requirement for mediation to occur. Actually, this particular criterion can hinder the discovery of substantive mechanisms of influence.

There are three major reasons for this. The first reason, discussed by Judd and Kenny(Reference Judd and Kenny25) and more recently explained by Kraemer et al.(Reference Kraemer, Wilson, Fairburn and Agras26), relates to the fact that the effect of the mediator on the outcome may depend on the values of the independent variable (interaction effect). It is theoretically possible for an intervention to leave unaltered the average level of the outcome (τ coefficient) and mediator (β coefficient) but change the direction of the effect of the mediator on the outcome (see Fig. 2a; β C and β T coefficients). In such a case, mediation would occur without observing a significant intervention effect on the outcome. As a matter of fact, there might not even be any significant intervention effects on the mediator or overall independent effect of the mediator on the outcome (Fig. 2a).

Fig. 2 Examples of mediation in the absence of a significant association between the independent variable and the outcome: (a) interaction between an intervention and the mediator; (b) inconsistent mediation

The second reason regards the possibility that the effect of an independent variable on the outcome be explained by multiple competing mechanisms whose influences are of opposite direction and cancel out(Reference Shrout and Bolger19, Reference MacKinnon, Krull and Lockwood27). For instance, it has been shown that an intervention-induced increase in physical activity (mediator of intervention effect, if the aim is to reduce BMI by increasing energy expenditure) in normal-weight adults does not generally result in a decrease in BMI (outcome) because it is also accompanied by an increase in energy intake (counterproductive or inconsistent mediator of intervention effect)(Reference Meltzer, Kayser, Saris and Pichard28). The intervention triggers two different mechanisms exerting opposite effects on BMI. It is clear that here mediation occurs even in lack of a significant overall effect of the intervention on the outcome (see Fig. 2b).

Inconsistent mediation, also called suppression, can sometimes be counterintuitive (see MacKinnon et al.(Reference MacKinnon, Krull and Lockwood27) for a discussion on inconsistent mediators). In such cases, assuming a robust effect, it is a warning that there may be serious problems with: (i) our action and conceptual theories; or (ii) our mediating variables measures. For example, unexpected inconsistent mediation effects have been recently reported in physical activity and nutrition intervention studies(Reference Haerens, Cerin, Maes, Cardon, Deforche and De Bourdeaudhuij14, Reference Haerens, Cerin, Deforche, Maes and De Bourdeaudhuij29). Self-efficacy and perceived barriers were found to be independently associated, in the theoretically predicted direction, with changes in physical activity and fat intake, respectively. However, the intervention was found to have a negative effect on self-efficacy and perceived barriers, resulting in inconsistent mediation effects. In other words, the intervention would have been more successful had not it exerted a negative effect on self-efficacy and perceived barriers as mechanisms of behaviour change. Changes in self-efficacy and perceived barriers suppressed the intervention effectiveness. The authors concluded that their strategies to manipulate mediators of behaviour change in adolescents needed to be reconsidered (problems with action theory) and/or that an intervention by measurement effect might have occurred, whereby exposure to the intervention affected the interpretation of the items in relation to the underlying construct (change of location of the items on the underlying construct). These explanations can be verified by employing differential item functioning (DIF) techniques in combination with having objective behavioural markers of self-efficacy and perceived barriers to contrast with the DIF of the scale items(Reference Baranowski, Allen, Masse and Wilson30).

The third reason for not considering a significant relationship between an independent variable and the outcome as a necessary criterion of mediation is low statistical power(Reference MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West and Sheet12, Reference Fritz and MacKinnon18, Reference Shrout and Bolger19). This argument is particularly relevant if we believe that the effect of the independent variable on the outcome is temporally or causally distal(Reference Shrout and Bolger19). In their simulation studies, Fritz and MacKinnon(Reference Fritz and MacKinnon18) have shown that, under conditions of complete mediation and small effect size, we would need a sample size of approximately 21 000 cases to achieve acceptable levels of statistical power. In contrast, the sample size required to detect the same mediated effect using product-of-coefficient tests (e.g. Sobel test, bootstrap estimates and asymmetric confidence intervals) would range between 490 and 660 cases.

Conclusion

To recap, the main points to remember in improving our current practices in MVA are:

  1. 1. Examine potential mediating effects even in the absence of a significant association between the independent and outcome variables.

  2. 2. Examine whether there is an interaction effect of the independent variable and the mediator on the outcome (applicable to prospective studies).

  3. 3. Report point estimates of the mediated effects, preferably in the units of the outcome measure.

  4. 4. Report confidence intervals of the mediated effects, preferably using accurate computational methods (asymmetric confidence intervals, bootstrapping).

  5. 5. When examining multiple mediators, aim at evaluating the independent contribution of each of them using complex, multiple-mediator models.

  6. 6. In intervention studies, test action (the extent to which the intervention affected the mediator) and conceptual theories (the extent to which changes in the mediator likely caused changes in the outcome).

  7. 7. Do not use Baron and Kenny’s method and the difference-of-coefficients method of MVA with dichotomous outcome variables.

Technical, methodological and conceptual issues related to MVA are complex and cannot be satisfactorily discussed in a single paper. Our aim was to highlight a few fundamental issues and strategies related to current practice in MVA in the research fields of behavioural nutrition and physical activity that is hoped will help us improve our understanding of how we can combat the obesity problem in our societies.

Acknowledgement

Conflicts of interest: There are no conflicts of interest.

Authorship responsibilities: E.C. conceptualized and wrote the manuscript. D.P.M. critically reviewed and approved the manuscript.

References

1.Flynn, MA, McNeil, DA, Maloff, B, Mutasingwa, D, Wu, M, Ford, C & Tough, SC (2006) Reducing obesity and related chronic disease risk in children and youth: a synthesis of evidence with ‘best practice’ recommendations. Obes Rev 7, Suppl. 1, 766.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2.Baranowski, T, Anderson, C & Carmack, C (1998) Mediating variable framework in physical activity interventions: how are we doing? How might we do better? Am J Prev Med 15, 266297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3.Baron, RM & Kenny, DA (1986) The moderator–mediator distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 51, 11731182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4.Chen, TH (1990) Theory-Driven Evaluations. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
5.Miller, Y, Trost, S & Brown, W (2002) Mediators of physical activity behavior among women with young children. Am J Prev Med 23, 2 Suppl., 98103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6.Moore, GF, Tapper, K, Murphy, S, Lynch, R, Raisanen, L, Pimm, C & Moore, L (2007) Associations between deprivation, attitudes towards eating breakfast and breakfast eating behaviours in 9–11-year-olds. Public Health Nutr 10, 582589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7.Pinto, BM, Lynn, H, Marcus, BH, DePue, J & Goldstein, MG (2001) Physician-based activity counseling: intervention effects on mediators of motivational readiness for physical activity. Ann Behav Med 23, 210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8.Prodaniuk, TR, Plotnikoff, RC, Spence, JC & Wilson, PM (2004) The influence of self-efficacy and outcome expectations on the relationship between perceived environment and physical activity in the workplace. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 1, 7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9.Kenny, DA, Kashy, DA & Bolger, N (1998) Data analysis in social psychology. In The Handbook of Social Psychology, vol. 1, pp 233265 [DT Gilbert, ST Fiske and G Lindzey, editors]. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
10.MacKinnon, DP (2008) Introduction to Statistical Mediation Analysis. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
11.MacKinnon, DP, Fairchild, AJ & Fritz, MS (2007) Mediation analysis. Annu Rev Psychol 58, 593614.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12.MacKinnon, DP, Lockwood, CM, Hoffman, JM, West, SG & Sheet, V (2002) A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychol Methods 7, 83103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13.Cerin, E, Taylor, LM, Leslie, E & Owen, N (2006) Small-scale randomized controlled trials need more powerful methods of mediational analysis than the Baron–Kenny method. J Clin Epidemiol 59, 457464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
14.Haerens, L, Cerin, E, Maes, L, Cardon, G, Deforche, B & De Bourdeaudhuij, I (2008) Explaining the effect of a 1-year intervention promoting physical activity in middle schools: a mediation analysis. Public Health Nutr 11, 501512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
15.MacKinnon, DP, Lockwood, CM, Brown, CH, Wang, W & Hoffman, JM (2007) The intermediate endpoint effect in logistic and probit regression. Clin Trials 4, 499513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16.Sobel, ME (1982) Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. In Sociological Methodology, pp. 290–312 [S Leinhardt, editor]. Washington, DC: American Sociological Association.Google Scholar
17.Bollen, KA & Stine, R (1990) Direct and indirect effects: classical and bootstrap estimates of variability. Sociol Methodol 20, 115140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
18.Fritz, MS & MacKinnon, DP (2007) Required sample size to detect the mediated effect. Psychol Sci 18, 233239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19.Shrout, PE & Bolger, N (2002) Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommendations. Psychol Methods 7, 422445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20.MacKinnon, DP, Fritz, MS, Williams, J & Lockwood, CM (2007) Distribution of the product confidence limits for the indirect effect: program PRODCLIN. Behav Res Methods 39, 384389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21.Preacher, KJ & Hayes, AF (2004) SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 36, 717731.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22.Efron, B & Tibshirani, TJ (1993) An Introduction to the Bootstrap. New York: Chapman & Hall.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
23.Pituch, KA, Stapleton, LM & Kang, JY (2006) A comparison for single sample and bootstrap methods to assess mediation in cluster randomized trials. Multivariate Behav Res 41, 367400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24.Cerin, E & Leslie, E (2008) How socio-economic status contributes to participation in leisure-time physical activity. Soc Sci Med 66, 25962609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25.Judd, CM & Kenny, DA (1981) Process analysis: estimating mediation in treatment evaluations. Eval Rev 5, 602619.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
26.Kraemer, H, Wilson, G, Fairburn, C & Agras, W (2002) Mediators and moderators of treatment effects in randomized clinical trials. Arch Gen Psychiatry 59, 877883.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27.MacKinnon, DP, Krull, JL & Lockwood, CM (2000) Equivalence of mediation, confounding and suppression effects. Prev Sci 1, 173181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
28.Meltzer, K, Kayser, B, Saris, WHM & Pichard, C (2005) Effects of physical activity on food intake. Clin Nutr 24, 885895.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
29.Haerens, L, Cerin, E, Deforche, B, Maes, L & De Bourdeaudhuij, I (2007) Explaining the effects of a 1-year intervention promoting a low fat diet in adolescent girls: a mediation analysis. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 4, 55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30.Baranowski, T, Allen, DD, Masse, LC & Wilson, M (2006) Does participation in an intervention affect responses on self-report questionnaires? Health Educ Res 21, 98109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Diagram for analysis of the effect of a fat-intake intervention on changes in fat intake mediated by self-efficacy for a low-fat diet

Figure 1

Fig. 2 Examples of mediation in the absence of a significant association between the independent variable and the outcome: (a) interaction between an intervention and the mediator; (b) inconsistent mediation