Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T14:45:41.068Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Parental considerations during complementary feeding in higher income countries: a systematic review of qualitative evidence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 April 2021

Eleni Spyreli*
Affiliation:
Centre for Public Health, Queen’s University Belfast, Institute of Clinical Science, Block A, Grosvenor Road, BelfastBT12 6BA, UK Institute for Global Food Security, Queen’s University Belfast, Chlorine Gardens, BelfastBT9 5DL, UK
Michelle C McKinley
Affiliation:
Centre for Public Health, Queen’s University Belfast, Institute of Clinical Science, Block A, Grosvenor Road, BelfastBT12 6BA, UK Institute for Global Food Security, Queen’s University Belfast, Chlorine Gardens, BelfastBT9 5DL, UK
Moira Dean
Affiliation:
Institute for Global Food Security, Queen’s University Belfast, Chlorine Gardens, BelfastBT9 5DL, UK
*
*Corresponding author: Email eleni.spyreli@qub.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

Worldwide data suggest a clash between parental complementary feeding practices and recommendations. Understanding the circumstances under which parents form their feeding practices is a crucial step to improve such practices. This paper aimed to systematically review the existing qualitative literature and synthesise the factors that parents take into consideration in relation to complementary feeding.

Design:

A systematic review was undertaken. Four electronic databases were searched for qualitative studies published after 2001 exploring parental experiences during complementary feeding. A framework that included authors’ outcomes of interest was used to extract and synthesise study findings. The Standards for Reporting Qualitative Research were used to critically assess the included studies.

Setting:

Upper-middle- and high-income countries.

Participants:

Parents with a child below the age of 3 years.

Results:

A total of forty-seven studies met the eligibility criteria and were included in this systematic review. The themes were organised into three main categories: (1) factors related to introduction of complementary foods; (2) factors related to the type of complementary foods and (3) factors related to both timing and type. The selected literature highlights: prevalent baby cues that prompt parents to introduce solid foods; parents’ views on the recommended timing of complementary feeding; factors that drive the choice of complementary foods and perceived value in advice received from health professionals and grandmothers.

Conclusions:

This systematic review indicates factors that can be barriers to complying with the complementary feeding guidelines, and therefore, its findings are pertinent to improving parental feeding practices through intervention studies and through infant feeding education in a primary care setting.

Type
Systematic Review
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

In the past two decades, research has considerably advanced our knowledge of infant nutrition by elucidating the rapid physical and neurological development taking place in the first year of life and the consequent nutritional requirements(Reference Gibney, Macdonald, Lanham-New, Macdonald and Roche1). Around the age of 6 months, milk is no longer sufficient to meet infant’s needs in energy, protein, most vitamins and minerals(Reference Castenmiller, De Henauw and Hirsch-Ernst2), and the addition of complementary foods is required to maintain healthy growth, a process known as complementary feeding(3). Complementary foods are often referred to as solid foods, even though they can be any nutrient-containing foods with a solid, semi-solid or liquid consistency excluding breastmilk or breastmilk substitutes.

In 2001, WHO published a set of infant feeding recommendations advising that infants should be introduced to nutritious complementary foods from 6 months(4). Parents are encouraged to offer a variety of flavours and textures and to include Fe-rich foods, as infant stores in Fe start to diminish from 6 months. It is recommended that added salt is avoided(Reference Joppich, Kollmann and Ingrisch5), and that parents should abstain from giving sugar- or honey-containing foods(Reference Bowen, Pearson and Rosalen6). In addition, the WHO guidelines advise parents on portion sizes and frequency of meals, on a feeding environment with no distractions, as well as on food safety when preparing and storing food(7). Since the advent of the WHO guidelines, new research evidence has emerged in relation to the appropriate timing of introduction of some foods in children with a risk of food allergy, as well on parental approaches that can help increase food acceptance and encourage children to regulate their food intake(Reference Castenmiller, De Henauw and Hirsch-Ernst2,Reference Nicklaus8,Reference DiSantis, Hodges and Johnson9) . The WHO recommendations, however, remain the basis for complementary feeding guidelines in most countries around the world.

Parental complementary feeding practices show considerable variation across higher income countries and often deviate greatly from the recommendations in terms of the timing of complementary feeding and the nutritional value of the first foods offered. In the UK, the 2011 Diet and Nutrition Survey of Infants and Young Children with a nationally representative sample of 2683 children aged between 4 and 18 months highlighted feeding practices that compromised infants’ dietary quality and diversity(Reference Lennox, Somerville and Ong10). Results reveal frequent consumption of nutrient-poor foods (e.g. sugar-containing foods and drinks), along with a low intake of protein- and Fe-rich foods. In South Africa, a cross-sectional analysis of data from 316 children between 4 and 18 months revealed frequent consumption of carbonated drinks, as well as low dietary variety (i.e. consumption of less than 4 food groups daily) and insufficient intake of key micronutrients including Zn and Fe(Reference Faber, Laubscher and Berti11). In the USA, the latest Feeding Infants and Toddler Survey shows that in a sample of 902 children between 6 and 12 months, a third of them consumed sugar-containing foods and half of them did not consume protein-rich foods daily (meat, fish, egg)(Reference Roess, Jacquier and Catellier12). Additionally, the same data indicate that, although rates of early introduction of solid foods are improving through the years, 17 % infants were introduced to complementary foods by 4 months. In Australia, this proportion was 28 % according to the national infant feeding survey in 2010(13). In Brazil, a prospective study which assessed feeding practices and dietary intake of 179 healthy infants showed that the median age of introducing solid foods was 4 months and concluded that insufficient intake in both macro- and micronutrients in young infants was very frequent with 45 % being Fe deficient(Reference Caetano, Ortiz and Silva14). Introducing solid foods early has been shown to be prevalent in European countries too(Reference Schiess, Grote and Scaglioni15,Reference Castro, Layte and Kearney16) .

Understanding the circumstances under which parents form their feeding practices, particularly practices that are in conflict with the recommendations, is a crucial initial step to improve them. Soaring interest has therefore focused on utilising qualitative methodologies(Reference Swift and Tischler17), which have been useful in providing insights into how and why parents shape their feeding practices. In the past two decades, new evidence exploring parental experiences in relation to the introduction of solid foods has emerged, and two systematic reviews have provided syntheses of this evidence(Reference Harrison, Brodribb and Hepworth18,Reference Matvienko-Sikar, Kelly and Sinnott19) . However, given the ever-growing number of relevant qualitative studies published since the last systematic review, it was considered essential to cast a view in the literature to gain a greater understanding of the underlying factors that contribute to the formation of parents’ feeding practices and their adherence to complementary feeding guidelines. In doing so, attention was drawn to the fact that although the WHO guidelines are relevant for all countries across the globe, circumstances in low-income countries (e.g. high rates of child stunting, insufficient health information services and limited access to clean water) represent unique and distinct challenges for infant feeding, when compared with higher income countries(Reference Dewey and Adu-Afarwuah20). This paper aimed to systematically review the existing qualitative literature in order to synthesise all the factors that are taken into consideration by parents and primary caregivers in relation to complementary feeding in upper-middle-income and high-income countries. Findings of this review will enable the design and implementation of effective education initiatives that will encourage parent feeding practices in line with complementary feeding recommendations.

Methods

The protocol of this review has been registered in the PROSPERO database (registration ID: CRD42017067091). The selection of the reviewed studies was based on a number of eligibility criteria detailed in Table 1. The studies that satisfied the eligibility criteria were incorporated into the synthesis; no further considerations were included (e.g. data saturation).

Table 1 Eligibility criteria for inclusion and exclusion of reviewed studies

Information sources and search

Four electronic databases were searched for this review: PubMed, EMBASE, Scopus and Web of Science. The papers retrieved from all databases were published in the same reference period (January 2001 – 6 March 2021). The reference lists of all included studies were hand-searched for additional papers that meet the inclusion criteria. In a few cases, where a full text could not be retrieved in English, the authors of these papers were contacted to confirm that a paper in English did not exist.

The same terms were used to search all databases after appropriate adjustment for database-specific operators. A list of search terms can be found in Table 2. Limits for the exclusion of papers published before January 2001 and animal studies were applied when possible. No limit was applied for language of publication.

Table 2 Keywords used in search of electronic databases

The asterisk (*) was used as the wildcard character in the search.

Study selection

All search results were imported from each database to Refworks citation management software (ProQuest). They were then exported into a unified Excel spreadsheet. All duplicate records were identified and removed. Titles were screened, and the records with irrelevant titles were removed. Two reviewers independently reviewed all abstracts of the remaining records against the eligibility criteria (ES, MMK). Disagreements were due to simple oversights and were resolved through discussion. When a definite decision could not be made based on the studies’ abstract alone, the full paper was obtained for a thorough assessment against the inclusion criteria. The reasons for exclusion were documented for each study that was screened on a full-text basis.

Data collection and synthesis

The information that was obtained and integrated in this review included key characteristics and reported findings of the included papers. In order to extract and synthesise study findings, a framework approach was followed as described by Barnett-Pate and Thomas(Reference Barnett-Page and Thomas21). An initial framework (i.e. a list of themes) was conceived following review of the background literature and discussions among authors. After selection of relevant studies and during careful consideration of the available data, additional themes emerged and incorporated into the initial framework. However, during this process, the newly emerging themes were prioritised based on frequency of occurrence within the selected literature. In this way, themes that appeared in only few papers and that were not extensively discussed were not presented in this review, an approach named as Qualitative Meta-summary(Reference Sandelowski22).

Quality assessment in individual studies

The ‘Standards for Reporting Qualitative Research’ tool (SRQR) was used to assess the quality of the included studies(Reference O’Brien, Harris and Beckman23). The SRQR checklist includes twenty-one criteria and provides a critical appraisal of the design and reporting of findings for the eligible papers. The SRQR scoring was utilised to underline areas of methodological rigor and clarity of reporting, as well as key elements that were inadequately reported. In this way, the quality assessment of the selected studies could enable comparisons among them and inform qualitative researchers of common omissions. Studies were not excluded based on the quality assessment, and their scores were not taken into consideration during data synthesis.

Results

Study selection

Figure 1 provides a flowchart of the selection process based on the PRISMA protocol(Reference Moher, Liberati and Tetzlaff24). Forty-seven studies were eligible and considered for this review. Characteristics of each of these studies can be found in Appendix – Table 1. Additionally, a list of all studies that were reviewed on a full-text basis and were not included is provided in Appendix – Table 2 with the reason for exclusion.

Fig. 1 PRISMA flowchart of study selection

Quality assessment of included studies

All the manuscripts were evaluated against the twenty-one items of the SRQR checklist (Fig. 2). All included studies sufficiently reported the problem formulation and the research question. All but one(Reference Horodynski and Mills25) adequately covered their results and their interpretation. The data analysis process was not well reported in five studies. Links to empirical data (e.g. quotes) were reported in 91 % of studies (43/47) and source of funding in 89 % of studies (42/47).

Fig. 2 Scoring of individual studies according to the ‘Standards of Reporting Qualitative Research’ assessment tool. , Number of items sufficiently covered in paper; , number of items partially covered in paper; , number of items for which no information was provided

Incomplete information was apparent within the included papers for a number of areas, one of which was describing the methodological approach followed within the title. Furthermore, there was a lack of information on the level of participation in each study and why. Finally, in twenty-four studies there was insufficient integration with prior work and inadequate descriptions of the transferability of studies’ conclusions and contribution to the field. The most frequently omitted information was regarding authors’ conflict of interest, followed by techniques enhancing credibility and study limitations. A detailed comparison of the studies against the SRQR checklist is provided in Appendix – Table 3.

Study findings

The themes were classified into (i) factors related to the timing of complementary feeding; (ii) factors related to the type of complementary foods and (iii) factors related to both timing and type of complementary foods. Figure 3 shows in detail the classification of themes and sub-themes discussed under each category. The themes were addressed across the literature with the majority of included papers discussing all themes. The frequency with which the themes and sub-themes were reported is summarised in Appendix – Table 4.

Fig. 3 Themes and sub-themes emerging from reviewing the qualitative research on complementary feeding. Themes are classified into (i) factors related to the timing of complementary feeding; (ii) factors related to the type of complementary foods and (iii) factors related to both timing and type of complementary foods

Factors related to timing of introduction of solid foods

Prompts for the introduction of solid foods

Prompts were infant- and mother-related.

Baby-related prompts

Mothers discussed how cues from their babies guided them to introduce solid foods. These cues included perceived hunger, perceived interest in food, changes in weight and physical cues indicating their infant’s readiness for solids.

The idea that infants were no longer satisfied by a milk-only diet was often cited by mothers and fathers as a compelling cue to give the baby’s first solid foods(Reference Horodynski and Mills25Reference Swanepoel, Henderson and Maher44). Signs that indicated hunger were crying, increased consumption of milk and disturbed sleep. In some cases, the feeling of obligation to attend to baby’s hunger led parents to introduce solids earlier than indicated by their country’s guidelines(Reference Horodynski and Mills25,Reference Heinig, Follett and Ishii29,Reference Lindsay, Machado and Sussner33Reference Savage, Neshteruk and Balantekin36,Reference Garcia, Looby and McLean-Guthrie43) . Seeing baby settle or sleep longer after providing food justified early complementary feeding.

Excitement triggered by the sight of food was reported as another sign of baby’s readiness for solids(Reference Anderson, Guthrie and Alder26Reference Heinig, Follett and Ishii29,Reference Kavanagh, Habibi and Anderson31,Reference Savage, Neshteruk and Balantekin36,Reference Synnott, Bogue and Edwards38Reference Walsh, Kearney and Dennis40,Reference Begley, Ringrose and Giglia42,Reference Arden and Abbott45Reference Monterrosa, Pelto and Frongillo48) . Parents described that their baby was reaching out for food and grabbing it from their plates, which encouraged them to start complementary feeding. A refusal for the breast, as reported in the study by Abu Shosha et al., also urged mothers to introduce solids(Reference Abu Shosha41).

Baby’s weight was also discussed in relation to the timing of introduction of solid foods(Reference Anderson, Guthrie and Alder26,Reference Arden28,Reference Heinig, Follett and Ishii29,Reference Lee and Brann32,Reference Lindsay, Machado and Sussner33,Reference Tully, Allen-Walker and Spyreli39Reference Begley, Ringrose and Giglia42,Reference Jessri, Farmer and Olson49) . The purpose of complementary feeding was quoted to sustain a steady growth rate for babies with slow weight gain(Reference Arden28,Reference Heinig, Follett and Ishii29,Reference Lindsay, Machado and Sussner33,Reference Walsh, Kearney and Dennis40,Reference Begley, Ringrose and Giglia42,Reference Jessri, Farmer and Olson49) . On the other hand, infants whose weight picked up in the first months of life were perceived to need solid foods earlier than their lighter peers(Reference Anderson, Guthrie and Alder26,Reference Tully, Allen-Walker and Spyreli39,Reference Abu Shosha41) .

Mothers explained that a number of physical milestones signified physiological maturation for a smooth transition to a mixed diet, such as losing the tongue thrust, being able to sit up, good hand-eye coordination and teething(Reference Anderson, Guthrie and Alder26,Reference Arden28Reference Horodynski, Olson and Arndt30,Reference Savage, Neshteruk and Balantekin36,Reference Begley, Ringrose and Giglia42,Reference Arden and Abbott45,Reference Brown and Lee46,Reference Monterrosa, Pelto and Frongillo48) .

Mother-related prompts

Certain mother-related considerations also contributed to the decision to introduce complementary foods(Reference Anderson, Guthrie and Alder26,Reference Arden28,Reference Tully, Allen-Walker and Spyreli39Reference Abu Shosha41,Reference Swanepoel, Henderson and Maher44,Reference Jessri, Farmer and Olson49Reference McNally, Hugh-Jones and Hetherington51) . Mothers who breastfed conveyed the need for babies to be fed by other people too, so that they could return to work(Reference Arden28,Reference Tully, Allen-Walker and Spyreli39,Reference Abu Shosha41,Reference Swanepoel, Henderson and Maher44,Reference Rodriguez-Oliveros, Bisogni and Frongillo50,Reference McNally, Hugh-Jones and Hetherington51) . Additionally, some mothers shared that they were excited to see their children eat family foods(Reference Anderson, Guthrie and Alder26,Reference Walsh, Kearney and Dennis40) . One study highlighted that breast-feeding in public was felt as shameful for Middle Eastern mothers living in Canada(Reference Jessri, Farmer and Olson49) and introducing solid foods was seen as the solution.

Knowledge of and attitudes towards guidelines on timing of complementary feeding

Parental views on the recommended age for introducing solid foods was an important theme across papers in this review.

Awareness of the recommended age of complementary feeding

Even though the recommended age of complementary feeding varied slightly based on location (e.g. after 4 months in the USA and from 6 months in the UK and Australia), findings from the selected studies reflected good awareness of the guidance(Reference Anderson, Guthrie and Alder26,Reference Horodynski, Olson and Arndt30,Reference Russell, Taki and Azadi35,Reference Synnott, Bogue and Edwards38,Reference Walsh, Kearney and Dennis40,Reference Garcia, Looby and McLean-Guthrie43,Reference Arden and Abbott45,Reference Brown and Lee46) . In contrast, few studies revealed poor awareness of the appropriate time to start complementary feeding(Reference Abu Shosha41,Reference Dogan, Sert and Topcu52Reference Yue, Marsh and Zhou54) . Indicatively, mothers interviewed by Abu Shosha et al. and Yue et al. believed that milk is sufficient for the first year of life and hence the introduction of solids foods is not necessary(Reference Abu Shosha41,Reference Yue, Marsh and Zhou54) .

Attitudes towards the guidelines for the appropriate age for complementary feeding

A wide range of views was expressed(Reference Anderson, Guthrie and Alder26,Reference Arden28,Reference Horodynski, Olson and Arndt30,Reference Redsell, Atkinson and Nathan34Reference Walsh, Kearney and Dennis40,Reference Begley, Ringrose and Giglia42,Reference Garcia, Looby and McLean-Guthrie43,Reference Arden and Abbott45Reference Cameron, Heath and Taylor47,Reference Caton, Ahern and Hetherington55,Reference Nielsen, Michaelsen and Holm56) . There were mothers who questioned their credibility and disregarded them, those who were sceptical but still considered them as a guide and those who expressed an effort and desire to follow them. Some of the reasons of dissatisfaction quoted were that current guidelines are perceived as too rigid and that they put unrealistic restrictions on a new mother. In addition, a ‘one-size-fits-all’ approach of the guidelines is often not viewed as relevant, since all babies develop differently.

Understanding the rationale

Despite mothers’ good awareness of the recommended age for complementary feeding, mothers revealed an uncertainty about the health implications of early introduction of solid foods(Reference Anderson, Guthrie and Alder26,Reference Russell, Taki and Azadi35) and about the need for reviewing and updating the guidelines(Reference Tully, Allen-Walker and Spyreli39,Reference Begley, Ringrose and Giglia42,Reference Caton, Ahern and Hetherington55) .

Factors related to type of complementary foods

Factors that influence the choice of complementary foods

Factors taken into consideration by parents to determine the type of complementary foods included health properties of the food; infants’ food preferences; culture; cost and parental considerations.

Actual and perceived health properties of the food

Providing a healthy diet was voiced as a priority in most studies that addressed the choice of complementary foods(Reference Anderson, Guthrie and Alder26,Reference Kavanagh, Habibi and Anderson31,Reference Lindsay, Machado and Sussner33Reference Russell, Taki and Azadi35,Reference Schwartz, Madrelle and Vereijken37Reference Abu Shosha41,Reference Garcia, Looby and McLean-Guthrie43Reference Brown and Lee46,Reference Monterrosa, Pelto and Frongillo48,Reference Rodriguez-Oliveros, Bisogni and Frongillo50,Reference McFarren, Vazquez and Jacobs53Reference Wrottesley, Prioreschi and Slemming64) . Parents shared a desire to provide a balanced diet with foods from all groups that will secure a sufficient intake of nutrients. Encouraging fruit and vegetable consumption was often discussed as the foundation of providing a healthy diet. A preference for organic foods was expressed by parents in a number of studies(Reference Redsell, Atkinson and Nathan34,Reference Schwartz, Madrelle and Vereijken37Reference Tully, Allen-Walker and Spyreli39,Reference Swanepoel, Henderson and Maher44,Reference Caton, Ahern and Hetherington55,Reference Nielsen, Michaelsen and Holm56,Reference Merriman, Doyle and McDaid61) , even though some admitted that cost was considerably higher than conventionally sourced foods.

On the contrary, certain foods were perceived as ‘unhealthy’ due to their nutritional content (e.g. sugar and salt) and level of processing and were avoided(Reference Schwartz, Madrelle and Vereijken37,Reference Arden and Abbott45,Reference McFarren, Vazquez and Jacobs53,Reference Nielsen, Michaelsen and Holm56,Reference Beck, Hoeft and Takayama57,Reference Merriman, Doyle and McDaid61,Reference Wrottesley, Prioreschi and Slemming64) . Parents also discussed complementary feeding as an opportunity for the family to adopt a healthier diet that is more suitable for the child’s nutritional needs. However, offering unsuitable solid foods, such as sugar-sweetened drinks and snacks, was discussed in some of the selected studies(Reference Monterrosa, Pelto and Frongillo48,Reference Jessri, Farmer and Olson49,Reference Cheney, Nieri and Davis59,Reference Horodynski, Calcatera and Carpenter65,Reference Van der Merwe, Bourne and Marais66) .

Furthermore, foods were avoided for complementary feeding because parents believed that they would cause allergy (e.g. dairy, nuts, eggs and fish)(Reference Schwartz, Madrelle and Vereijken37,Reference Walsh, Kearney and Dennis40,Reference Garcia, Looby and McLean-Guthrie43,Reference Rodriguez-Oliveros, Bisogni and Frongillo50) or that an infants’ gastrointestinal system was not mature enough to process them (pulses, meat and some types of vegetables)(Reference Kavanagh, Habibi and Anderson31,Reference Schwartz, Madrelle and Vereijken37,Reference Monterrosa, Pelto and Frongillo48,Reference Rodriguez-Oliveros, Bisogni and Frongillo50,Reference Beck, Hoeft and Takayama57) . Beck and Yue reported that parents waited for children to reach 12 months before introducing meat, as they believed their infants could not digest it. Gaps in parental understanding of what healthy diet means were evident, but also in the nutritional content of foods and in the nutrient-health links. In some cases, individual foods have been arbitrarily linked to healthy growth and well-being due to their nutritional content(Reference Monterrosa, Pelto and Frongillo48,Reference Rodriguez-Oliveros, Bisogni and Frongillo50,Reference Dutta, Sywulka and Frongillo60,Reference Chakona63) .

Baby’s preferences and aversions

Parents observed that their infants had distinct food likes and dislikes which guided the choice of complementary foods(Reference Anderson, Guthrie and Alder26,Reference Tully, Allen-Walker and Spyreli39,Reference Begley, Ringrose and Giglia42,Reference Arden and Abbott45,Reference Monterrosa, Pelto and Frongillo48,Reference Rodriguez-Oliveros, Bisogni and Frongillo50,Reference Yue, Marsh and Zhou54,Reference Nielsen, Michaelsen and Holm56Reference Boak, Virgo-Milton and Hoare58,Reference Dutta, Sywulka and Frongillo60,Reference Spyreli, McKinley and Allen-Walker62) . Rodriguez et al. and Yue et al. report that parents might have even prioritised offering foods that baby was thought to like over others that were considered to be more nutritious.

Cultural background

Studies explored the values of mothers whose culture is strongly linked to dietary choices and traditional foods automatically featured in infant diet(Reference Lindsay, Machado and Sussner33,Reference Schwartz, Madrelle and Vereijken37,Reference Monterrosa, Pelto and Frongillo48,Reference Rodriguez-Oliveros, Bisogni and Frongillo50,Reference Dogan, Sert and Topcu52,Reference Beck, Hoeft and Takayama57,Reference Boak, Virgo-Milton and Hoare58,Reference Chakona63,Reference Wrottesley, Prioreschi and Slemming64,Reference Van der Merwe, Bourne and Marais66) . They also highlighted experiences whereby religion was associated with food(Reference Lee and Brann32,Reference Jessri, Farmer and Olson49,Reference Leung67) , such as Islamic beliefs requiring halal complementary foods(Reference Jessri, Farmer and Olson49). Some studies discussed how cultural traditions encourage the consumption of foods that are not recommended during infancy, i.e. herbal infusions and sugar-sweetened desserts(Reference Jessri, Farmer and Olson49,Reference Dogan, Sert and Topcu52,Reference Chakona63,Reference Wrottesley, Prioreschi and Slemming64) .

Food cost

For low-income families, the price of certain foods (fresh fruit, vegetables and meat) was a barrier to providing a complete nutrition during complementary feeding(Reference Redsell, Atkinson and Nathan34,Reference Schwartz, Madrelle and Vereijken37,Reference Synnott, Bogue and Edwards38,Reference Monterrosa, Pelto and Frongillo48,Reference Jessri, Farmer and Olson49,Reference Yue, Marsh and Zhou54,Reference Boak, Virgo-Milton and Hoare58,Reference Dutta, Sywulka and Frongillo60,Reference Chakona63) . Due to the reported high cost and perishability of these foods, parents described replacing them by cheaper alternatives of poorer nutritional quality or offering them less frequently than ideally (e.g. daily). The cost of nutritional supplements was an additional financial burden for families with limited budget(Reference Jessri, Farmer and Olson49).

Parental considerations

Additional factors that guided the decision on the type of first foods were food consistency, parents’ own food preferences and cooking skills(Reference Anderson, Guthrie and Alder26,Reference Kavanagh, Habibi and Anderson31,Reference Redsell, Atkinson and Nathan34,Reference Schwartz, Madrelle and Vereijken37Reference Tully, Allen-Walker and Spyreli39,Reference Begley, Ringrose and Giglia42,Reference Brown and Lee46,Reference Monterrosa, Pelto and Frongillo48Reference Rodriguez-Oliveros, Bisogni and Frongillo50,Reference Boak, Virgo-Milton and Hoare58,Reference Merriman, Doyle and McDaid61Reference Wrottesley, Prioreschi and Slemming64,Reference Thullen, Majee and Davis68) . Foods with a soft texture were considered appropriate for infants, whereas parents discussed how hard foods, or foods with seeds raised concerns of choking and were, therefore, avoided(Reference Anderson, Guthrie and Alder26,Reference Schwartz, Madrelle and Vereijken37,Reference Monterrosa, Pelto and Frongillo48,Reference Rodriguez-Oliveros, Bisogni and Frongillo50) . Food choices were also influenced by what mothers saw as tasty and discussed how infants are exposed to food they preferred(Reference Kavanagh, Habibi and Anderson31,Reference Schwartz, Madrelle and Vereijken37,Reference Brown and Lee46,Reference Monterrosa, Pelto and Frongillo48,Reference Boak, Virgo-Milton and Hoare58,Reference Merriman, Doyle and McDaid61,Reference Wrottesley, Prioreschi and Slemming64) . Similarly, foods mothers disliked were not available and were not provided to children(Reference Rodriguez-Oliveros, Bisogni and Frongillo50,Reference Spyreli, McKinley and Allen-Walker62) . Limited confidence in cooking and lack of cooking equipment were also quoted to impact choice of complementary foods(Reference Redsell, Atkinson and Nathan34,Reference Begley, Ringrose and Giglia42,Reference Jessri, Farmer and Olson49,Reference Rodriguez-Oliveros, Bisogni and Frongillo50,Reference Boak, Virgo-Milton and Hoare58,Reference Dutta, Sywulka and Frongillo60,Reference Chakona63) .

Perceptions of commercial baby foods

A general preference for home-made foods over ready-prepared baby foods emerged from the studies included. Even though commercial foods were seen in some cases as convenient and time-saving(Reference Schwartz, Madrelle and Vereijken37Reference Tully, Allen-Walker and Spyreli39,Reference Swanepoel, Henderson and Maher44,Reference Nielsen, Michaelsen and Holm56,Reference Chakona63) , mothers voiced mistrust for products marketed for infants, because their label ‘suitable from 4 months’ contradicted the 6-month recommendation and could contribute to maternal confusion over timing of complementary feeding(Reference Synnott, Bogue and Edwards38Reference Walsh, Kearney and Dennis40,Reference Begley, Ringrose and Giglia42) . Other negative aspects of marketed baby foods, as perceived by parents, included uncertainty regarding presence of potentially harmful ingredients and nutritional value, inferior flavour and cost.

Ingredients and nutritional value

A common idea about ready-prepared baby foods was that they underwent processing and contained unknown ingredients or artificial additives that could be dangerous for infant’s health(Reference Begley, Ringrose and Giglia42,Reference Swanepoel, Henderson and Maher44,Reference Rodriguez-Oliveros, Bisogni and Frongillo50,Reference Beck, Hoeft and Takayama57,Reference Boak, Virgo-Milton and Hoare58,Reference Dutta, Sywulka and Frongillo60,Reference Merriman, Doyle and McDaid61) . Their nutritional quality was an additional consideration, and parents were uncertain whether commercial baby foods provided the necessary nutrients and whether they were void from ingredients that should be avoided during infancy, e.g. salt and sugar(Reference Synnott, Bogue and Edwards38,Reference Tully, Allen-Walker and Spyreli39,Reference Rodriguez-Oliveros, Bisogni and Frongillo50,Reference Caton, Ahern and Hetherington55,Reference Nielsen, Michaelsen and Holm56,Reference Dutta, Sywulka and Frongillo60) .

Taste and cost

Four studies reported that ready-made infant foods were perceived to lack taste and therefore children did not enjoy eating them(Reference Schwartz, Madrelle and Vereijken37,Reference Synnott, Bogue and Edwards38,Reference Caton, Ahern and Hetherington55,Reference Van der Merwe, Bourne and Marais66) . In addition, parents felt it was cheaper to make their own baby foods at home than to buy those on the market(Reference Jessri, Farmer and Olson49,Reference Boak, Virgo-Milton and Hoare58,Reference Dutta, Sywulka and Frongillo60,Reference Van der Merwe, Bourne and Marais66) .

Parental strategies to establish healthy dietary behaviours

Parents talked about the positive effects of offering a variety of foods, the repeated exposure to foods, baby-led weaning and modelling with regard to future food habits.

Providing variety of foods

Mothers were mindful that their feeding approaches would have a lasting effect on their children’s eating behaviours and that ensuring food variety would have a number of benefits, like nutritional adequacy, improved acceptance of foods in infancy and later in life and low risk of fussy eating behaviours(Reference Anderson, Guthrie and Alder26,Reference Arden28,Reference Schwartz, Madrelle and Vereijken37,Reference Brown and Lee46,Reference Monterrosa, Pelto and Frongillo48,Reference McFarren, Vazquez and Jacobs53,Reference Caton, Ahern and Hetherington55,Reference Nielsen, Michaelsen and Holm56,Reference Boak, Virgo-Milton and Hoare58,Reference Merriman, Doyle and McDaid61,Reference Spyreli, McKinley and Allen-Walker62) .

Repeated exposure to foods

In a number of studies, mothers discussed how their infants’ acceptance of unfamiliar foods grew over time through their repeated exposure to them(Reference Schwartz, Madrelle and Vereijken37,Reference Caton, Ahern and Hetherington55,Reference Beck, Hoeft and Takayama57,Reference Boak, Virgo-Milton and Hoare58,Reference Spyreli, McKinley and Allen-Walker62) . Mothers recounted cases of re-offering foods disliked by their infants (mostly vegetables) like re-presenting them after a few days, hiding them within dishes or giving them side by side with a previously liked food. A coercive repeated exposure was presented by Wrottesley and team, whereby mothers reported force-feeding foods infants refused to eat(Reference Wrottesley, Prioreschi and Slemming64).

Baby-led weaning

Six studies discussed baby-led weaning (BLW) (i.e. infant being offered foods in their whole form and self-feeding rather than being spoon-fed(Reference Rapley69) as another method to foster healthy eating habits in the future), three of which exclusively recruited BLW mothers in the UK and New Zealand(Reference Arden28,Reference Walsh, Kearney and Dennis40,Reference Swanepoel, Henderson and Maher44Reference Cameron, Heath and Taylor47) . Through this approach, it was envisaged that children would develop an ability to develop good appetite control, lower their chances of becoming fussy-eaters and improved the experience of family meals.

Modelling

Mothers considered themselves role models for their children and used this quality in an effort to shape good food habits(Reference Synnott, Bogue and Edwards38,Reference Caton, Ahern and Hetherington55,Reference Spyreli, McKinley and Allen-Walker62) . This was achieved by eating healthy, nutritious foods (e.g. vegetables) and abstaining from unsuitable snacks (e.g. sweets) in shared mealtimes.

Factors related to both timing of introduction and type of solid foods

Views on the available sources of feeding advice

Discussions in relation to sources of influence on parents’ feeding practices were a common thread in the included studies. Advice from ‘important others’ (healthcare professionals, grandmothers and peers) was described as a prompt to introduce complementary foods and also a guide to the choice of foods offered to their children.

Healthcare professionals

A range of professionals who give postpartum feeding advice were the most-frequently-discussed source of information. These ranged from paediatricians and nurses (European countries) to health visitors (UK) and other healthcare professionals (e.g. Special Supplemental Nutrition Program staff in USA)(Reference Horodynski and Mills25Reference Heinig, Follett and Ishii29,Reference Savage, Neshteruk and Balantekin36,Reference Abu Shosha41,Reference Garcia, Looby and McLean-Guthrie43,Reference Arden and Abbott45,Reference Cameron, Heath and Taylor47,Reference Dogan, Sert and Topcu52,Reference McFarren, Vazquez and Jacobs53,Reference Caton, Ahern and Hetherington55,Reference Cheney, Nieri and Davis59,Reference Wrottesley, Prioreschi and Slemming64,Reference Leung67,Reference Griauzde, Kieffer and Domoff70) .

In nine studies, medical professionals were discussed in a positive light due to their experience and knowledge and parents recognised that their decision on when to start complementary feeding and what foods to feed was based on their advice(Reference Anderson, Guthrie and Alder26,Reference Arden28,Reference Heinig, Follett and Ishii29,Reference Synnott, Bogue and Edwards38,Reference Walsh, Kearney and Dennis40,Reference Abu Shosha41,Reference Cameron, Heath and Taylor47,Reference Monterrosa, Pelto and Frongillo48,Reference McFarren, Vazquez and Jacobs53,Reference Caton, Ahern and Hetherington55,Reference Leung67,Reference Griauzde, Kieffer and Domoff70) . However, in eleven studies the information from healthcare professionals was met with scepticism(Reference Horodynski and Mills25,Reference Anderson, Nicklas and Spence27Reference Kavanagh, Habibi and Anderson31,Reference Redsell, Atkinson and Nathan34Reference Savage, Neshteruk and Balantekin36,Reference Tully, Allen-Walker and Spyreli39,Reference Garcia, Looby and McLean-Guthrie43,Reference Arden and Abbott45,Reference Wrottesley, Prioreschi and Slemming64) . Two separate samples of mothers and fathers agreed that advice from medical staff who had not experienced parenthood was less valued than those with children(Reference Anderson, Nicklas and Spence27,Reference Tully, Allen-Walker and Spyreli39,Reference Walsh, Kearney and Dennis40) . The level of adherence or scepticism towards advice from healthcare staff seemed to depend on the nature of contact with them and number of previous children. For instance, Yue et al. explored a setting where caregivers expressed mistrust for the advice of medical professionals, but had limited opportunities to consult them due to the absence of a routine postnatal care pathway(Reference Yue, Marsh and Zhou54). Moreover, a sample of Danish mothers who had previous experience with complementary feeding were less willing to consult with a health provider(Reference Nielsen, Michaelsen and Holm56).

Grandmothers

Parents in many studies expressed trust for their mothers’ feeding advice, and in some cases, grandmothers were a preferred source of advice over healthcare professionals(Reference Horodynski and Mills25,Reference Horodynski, Olson and Arndt30,Reference Kavanagh, Habibi and Anderson31,Reference Lindsay, Machado and Sussner33,Reference Tully, Allen-Walker and Spyreli39,Reference Abu Shosha41,Reference Begley, Ringrose and Giglia42,Reference Swanepoel, Henderson and Maher44,Reference Jessri, Farmer and Olson49,Reference Yue, Marsh and Zhou54,Reference Caton, Ahern and Hetherington55,Reference Cheney, Nieri and Davis59,Reference Wrottesley, Prioreschi and Slemming64,Reference Horodynski, Calcatera and Carpenter65,Reference Bramhagen, Axelsson and Hallstrom71) . Mothers-in-law were also influential, though to a lesser extent, and particularly for participants who viewed them as a ‘motherly figure’(Reference Anderson, Guthrie and Alder26,Reference Anderson, Nicklas and Spence27,Reference Kavanagh, Habibi and Anderson31,Reference Russell, Taki and Azadi35,Reference Cheney, Nieri and Davis59,Reference Merriman, Doyle and McDaid61) . Perceived value in grandmothers’ advice was due to their own experience with feeding children(Reference Kavanagh, Habibi and Anderson31,Reference Abu Shosha41,Reference Swanepoel, Henderson and Maher44,Reference Caton, Ahern and Hetherington55,Reference Cheney, Nieri and Davis59,Reference Wrottesley, Prioreschi and Slemming64,Reference Horodynski, Calcatera and Carpenter65) . Teenage mums and women living in a different country from their home country relied heavily on their mothers’ advice which shaped their infant feeding practices(Reference Lee and Brann32,Reference Jessri, Farmer and Olson49,Reference Horodynski, Calcatera and Carpenter65) . On the other hand, parents reported that the advice from their mothers often came without being requested, as it was considered outdated(Reference Anderson, Nicklas and Spence27,Reference Arden28,Reference Russell, Taki and Azadi35,Reference Synnott, Bogue and Edwards38,Reference Tully, Allen-Walker and Spyreli39,Reference Abu Shosha41) . It was often pointed out that grandmothers applied pressure for an early introduction of solids(Reference Horodynski and Mills25,Reference Anderson, Guthrie and Alder26,Reference Russell, Taki and Azadi35,Reference Walsh, Kearney and Dennis40,Reference Cheney, Nieri and Davis59,Reference Merriman, Doyle and McDaid61,Reference Wrottesley, Prioreschi and Slemming64) and offered or encouraged foods that were not suitable for infants, e.g. sweet treats(Reference Horodynski and Mills25,Reference Redsell, Atkinson and Nathan34,Reference Swanepoel, Henderson and Maher44,Reference Jessri, Farmer and Olson49,Reference Boak, Virgo-Milton and Hoare58,Reference Cheney, Nieri and Davis59,Reference Merriman, Doyle and McDaid61) .

Peer influence

Friends and peers acted as an important influence and contributed both positively and negatively to parents’ complementary feeding decisions(Reference Horodynski and Mills25,Reference Anderson, Nicklas and Spence27Reference Kavanagh, Habibi and Anderson31,Reference Redsell, Atkinson and Nathan34,Reference Russell, Taki and Azadi35,Reference Schwartz, Madrelle and Vereijken37,Reference Tully, Allen-Walker and Spyreli39Reference Swanepoel, Henderson and Maher44,Reference Cameron, Heath and Taylor47,Reference Jessri, Farmer and Olson49,Reference Dogan, Sert and Topcu52,Reference Yue, Marsh and Zhou54,Reference Caton, Ahern and Hetherington55,Reference Boak, Virgo-Milton and Hoare58,Reference Horodynski, Calcatera and Carpenter65,Reference Griauzde, Kieffer and Domoff70,Reference Bramhagen, Axelsson and Hallstrom71) . Both mothers and fathers highlighted that parenthood and recent experience with infant feeding qualified friends as credible sources of advice(Reference Anderson, Nicklas and Spence27,Reference Walsh, Kearney and Dennis40,Reference Boak, Virgo-Milton and Hoare58) . The ease of getting in touch with them and their experience in dealing with practical difficulties of feeding contributed to the parental trust in peers’ advice. It was recognised, however, that the influence of peers sometimes manifested as peer pressure to introduce solid foods early(Reference Arden28,Reference Redsell, Atkinson and Nathan34,Reference Walsh, Kearney and Dennis40,Reference Begley, Ringrose and Giglia42) .

Receiving external advice was very often expressed as vague, contradictory and confusing(Reference Horodynski and Mills25,Reference Arden28,Reference Kavanagh, Habibi and Anderson31,Reference Russell, Taki and Azadi35Reference Begley, Ringrose and Giglia42,Reference Jessri, Farmer and Olson49,Reference Dogan, Sert and Topcu52,Reference Yue, Marsh and Zhou54,Reference Boak, Virgo-Milton and Hoare58,Reference Bramhagen, Axelsson and Hallstrom71) . The plethora of available sources of information made it difficult for parents to filter out the unreliable information. In general, parents who had previous experience with complementary feeding felt they were less influenced by external advice or had a better ability to gauge if the advice was correct(Reference Anderson, Nicklas and Spence27,Reference Horodynski, Olson and Arndt30,Reference Synnott, Bogue and Edwards38,Reference Walsh, Kearney and Dennis40,Reference Abu Shosha41,Reference Swanepoel, Henderson and Maher44,Reference Dogan, Sert and Topcu52,Reference Yue, Marsh and Zhou54) . Receiving conflicting complementary feeding advice was quoted as a rationale for not adhering to infant feeding guidelines(Reference Arden28,Reference Savage, Neshteruk and Balantekin36,Reference Walsh, Kearney and Dennis40) .

Feedback from mothers on improved complementary feeding education

Parents suggested improvements for areas where they felt they needed more information or support, as well as healthcare professionals’ approach to providing feeding information.

Areas of inadequate information

In several studies, mothers pointed out that early introduction of solid foods is prevalent, as they felt there is insufficient education on its health implications and on the benefits of waiting until 6 months(Reference Horodynski and Mills25,Reference Arden28,Reference Horodynski, Olson and Arndt30) . Furthermore, it was felt that more information was required on the specific foods that are associated with increased risk of allergies(Reference Horodynski, Olson and Arndt30,Reference Tully, Allen-Walker and Spyreli39,Reference Horodynski, Calcatera and Carpenter65) . Other areas where more information was required included the appropriate frequency and size of infant’s daily meals(Reference Horodynski and Mills25,Reference Horodynski, Olson and Arndt30,Reference Schwartz, Madrelle and Vereijken37,Reference Abu Shosha41,Reference Garcia, Looby and McLean-Guthrie43,Reference McFarren, Vazquez and Jacobs53,Reference Van der Merwe, Bourne and Marais66) , as new mothers found it difficult to conceptualise the amount of solid foods that would replace milk feeds. More guidance was also sought after in relation to the appropriate age of introducing different food textures(Reference Abu Shosha41,Reference Begley, Ringrose and Giglia42) . Finally, in two recent studies, social media were suggested as an accessible way to communicate infant feeding information to younger mothers(Reference Garcia, Looby and McLean-Guthrie43,Reference Griauzde, Kieffer and Domoff70) .

Healthcare professionals’ role

The ability to establish good rapport was seen as an essential quality that health professionals that deal with parents should have. Participants explained that healthcare staff need to be approachable, inspire trust and avoid passing judgement for mothers’ feeding practices(Reference Redsell, Atkinson and Nathan34,Reference Tully, Allen-Walker and Spyreli39,Reference Abu Shosha41,Reference Swanepoel, Henderson and Maher44,Reference Horodynski, Calcatera and Carpenter65) . In terms of language used, parents highlighted the need for medical staff to translate the dietary guidelines into easy-to-understand practical information(Reference Boak, Virgo-Milton and Hoare58,Reference Horodynski, Calcatera and Carpenter65) . Participants in studies by Jessri et al. and Horodynski et al. stressed the importance of receiving complementary feeding advice that is sensitive and appropriate to their ethnic identity and religion(Reference Jessri, Farmer and Olson49,Reference Horodynski, Calcatera and Carpenter65) . Receiving information that does not take into consideration mothers’ cultural background was described as an important barrier to adhering to the complementary feeding recommendations(Reference Jessri, Farmer and Olson49). Finally, mothers in the UK voiced disillusion with advice from healthcare professionals that were not in alignment with the updated feeding recommendations(Reference Garcia, Looby and McLean-Guthrie43).

Experiences of baby-led-weaning mothers

Three of the included studies set out to explore the experiences of mothers who employed a baby-led approach to complementary feeding(Reference Arden and Abbott45Reference Cameron, Heath and Taylor47). Among BLW mothers, it was generally agreed that the infant feeding guidelines were an important guide that prompted them to wait for 6 months to introduce solid foods. The studies, two from the UK(Reference Arden and Abbott45,Reference Brown and Lee46) and one from New Zealand(Reference Cameron, Heath and Taylor47), dealt with the advantages of the method, as well as the practical difficulties.

Positive experiences

Good appetite and portion control was described by mothers as one of the advantages over traditional feeding methods. According to BLW mums, their children developed the ability to recognise their own signals of hunger and satiety and demonstrated good control of the amount of food needed to satisfy their hunger(Reference Arden and Abbott45,Reference Brown and Lee46) . Additionally, McNally and colleagues observed a reduced maternal concern over the amount of food eaten by BLW infants and whether it is enough, when compared with spoon-feeding mothers(Reference McNally, Hugh-Jones and Hetherington51). Participants in the BLW studies viewed spoon-feeding as a forceful process that was associated with a certain level of pressure, whereas BLW was viewed as a more fun way of eating(Reference Arden and Abbott45,Reference Brown and Lee46) . With baby-led weaning, infants were seen to actively take part in family mealtimes and overall made feeding a more enjoyable experience for them and their parents(Reference Swanepoel, Henderson and Maher44,Reference Brown and Lee46,Reference Cameron, Heath and Taylor47) . Finally, some mothers who used BLW explained that the approach saved money and time, as infants have whatever the family eats with no need for particular adaptations such as preparation of purees(Reference Brown and Lee46,Reference Cameron, Heath and Taylor47) .

Negative aspects

Mothers admitted that BLW was not suitable for all times and occasions, and the method was linked with increased mealtime mess(Reference Arden and Abbott45Reference Cameron, Heath and Taylor47). However, mothers discussed that mess was gradually reduced as infant motor skills improved. Although some mothers described BLW as economical, others described the method as wasteful due to the mess made, and this could discourage mothers from offering expensive foods(Reference Brown and Lee46). Allowing infants to have control over the food consumed was not always discussed positively, and some mothers were unable to measure the amount eaten which raised concerns around the adequacy of child’s food intake(Reference Arden and Abbott45). Finally, although mothers admitted that the risk of choking might put parents off of BLW(Reference Garcia, Looby and McLean-Guthrie43,Reference Swanepoel, Henderson and Maher44,Reference Brown and Lee46,Reference Cameron, Heath and Taylor47) , only a small minority reported dealing with individual cases of choking which were eventually managed easily.

Fathers’ role in complementary feeding

Two of the included studies set out to explore fathers’ experiences in relation to complementary feeding(Reference Anderson, Nicklas and Spence27,Reference Thullen, Majee and Davis68) . Specifically, Anderson and colleagues recruited exclusively male participants, and Thullen et al. recruited couples with both males and females being present during each couple’s interview. Two additional studies included fathers in their sample(Reference Merriman, Doyle and McDaid61,Reference Bramhagen, Axelsson and Hallstrom71) and another two recruited an exclusively female sample, but included mothers’ reports on their male partners’ involvement with complementary feeding(Reference Begley, Ringrose and Giglia42,Reference Wrottesley, Prioreschi and Slemming64) .

Overall, fathers’ role was described to be supportive to a great extent, since mothers were mainly responsible for feeding and took decisions regarding the timing and the type of food(Reference Anderson, Nicklas and Spence27,Reference Begley, Ringrose and Giglia42,Reference Wrottesley, Prioreschi and Slemming64,Reference Thullen, Majee and Davis68,Reference Bramhagen, Axelsson and Hallstrom71) . Moreover, it was highlighted that fathers had a more relaxed approach to feeding and were more inclined towards learning through experience rather than implementing information previously sought from important sources. Fathers reported to be less concerned about feeding their child foods that mothers would consider not appropriate for infants(Reference Merriman, Doyle and McDaid61,Reference Thullen, Majee and Davis68) . Additionally, a sample of fathers described offering specific portions, pre-determined by their partners, rather than responding to infant’s satiety cues(Reference Anderson, Nicklas and Spence27).

Discussion

This systematic review offers a comprehensive synthesis of the qualitative evidence in relation to factors that parents take into consideration during complementary feeding in higher income countries. Findings of this paper provide novel insights into parental experiences and attitudes and can be grouped into three overarching categories: (1) themes that relate to the age of introduction of solids foods; (2) themes that relate to the type of first solid foods and (3) themes that relate to both age of introduction and type. Themes with important public health implications include prevalent baby-driven cues to introduce solids; parental views on the recommendations for the age of complementary feeding; factors that drive the choice of complementary foods; perceived value in advice received from health professionals and grandmothers and the nature of fathers’ role during complementary feeding. The findings and their public health implications are discussed in detail below.

Factors in relation to the age of introduction of solids

Studies included in this systematic review indicated that the circumstances that prompt the introduction of solid foods have been extensively explored among parents from various socio-economic backgrounds. Perceived infant hunger made mothers think that milk alone fails to nourish their babies and was reported to be a compelling prompt for mothers to introduce solids. This has been previously reported in two previous systematic reviews on parental infant feeding practices(Reference Harrison, Brodribb and Hepworth18,Reference Matvienko-Sikar, Kelly and Sinnott19) . It is also consistent with cross-sectional data, as the UK Diet and Nutrition Survey for Infants and Young Children revealed that the most prevalent reason for introducing solid foods was the perception that the baby is no longer satisfied with milk feeds(Reference Lennox, Somerville and Ong10). One of the signs indicating infant hunger, as reported in this review, was disrupted sleep. Contrary to common maternal beliefs, an observational study has shown that increasing the calorific content of infants’ diet by offering more milk or by commencing complementary foods does not improve disrupted sleep(Reference Brown and Harries72). Parents need to be more aware of official guidance that warns that hunger and lack of sleep alone do not mark an appropriate timing for introducing solids and encourages them to be mindful of developmental milestones too(Reference Agostoni, Decsi and Fewtrell73,74) . Moreover, the qualitative literature, as reviewed in this paper and in the review by Harrison et al. (Reference Harrison, Brodribb and Hepworth18), has demonstrated that having a heavy infant can encourage early introduction of solids, which is in agreement with data from a longitudinal study by Rogers and Blisset(Reference Rogers and Blissett75). Recent research has also addressed infant temperament as a potential contributing factor to the timing of introduction of solid foods, but with no conclusive evidence so far(Reference Rogers and Blissett75Reference Wasser, Bentley and Borja77). In this review, although not explicitly discussed, mothers did talk about every baby being different indicating awareness of baby’s personality. It will be interesting for future qualitative researchers to explore whether parents are conscious of introducing solids earlier or later because of their child’s temperament.

In relation to parental perceptions of the recommendations on the appropriate age for complementary feeding, there was good awareness, but there was scepticism as to whether introducing solids at an age specified by universal guidelines is best practice; this has formerly been demonstrated by the systematic reviews on this topic as well(Reference Harrison, Brodribb and Hepworth18,Reference Matvienko-Sikar, Kelly and Sinnott19) . In addition, findings indicate that there was a substantial gap in maternal knowledge of the health risks related to early introduction and similarly the benefits of timely introduction, which seem to reinforce the general dismissal of the recommended timing for complementary feeding. Poor compliance to the recommendation on timing of complementary feeding is prevalent across higher income countries and is illustrated by national survey data in the USA, Australia and UK, where the officially recommended timing of complementary feeding is 6 months(13,Reference Kuo, Inkelas and Slusser78,Reference McAndrew, Thompson and Fellows79) .

Factors in relation to the type of first solid foods

Foods’ nutritional content and their impact on infant’s health were the most influential factors when choosing complementary foods. Placing emphasis on offering fruit and vegetables was seen as an important determinant of choice of complementary foods, and this was shown by previous systematic reviews in this topic(Reference Harrison, Brodribb and Hepworth18,Reference Matvienko-Sikar, Kelly and Sinnott19) . Indeed, the value of introducing a wide variety of fruits and vegetables early in life has been highlighted in previous research(Reference Nicklaus8). Furthermore, purchasing organic was seen in some cases as a way of securing the best possible diet for their children. In contrast to the beliefs of many parents, the latest systematic review on the nutritional quality of organic foods by Dangour and colleagues concluded that currently there is no evidence suggesting significant differences in the nutrient content between organic and conventionally produced food(Reference Dangour, Dodhia and Hayter80).

Among the factors that influenced the type of complementary foods offered, discussions revealed parental concerns regarding the consumption of certain foods during the first years of life. Some of these concerns were passed down from older family members or were established on an empirical basis. Fear of developing food allergies was quoted as another reason to avoid a number of foods. Similarly, in the most recent UK Infant Feeding Survey, 12 % and 11 % of mums with an infant aged between 8 and 10 months reported that they avoided giving to their children any eggs or dairy products, respectively, due to fear of allergy(Reference McAndrew, Thompson and Fellows79). Food avoidance can restrict nutritional variety and compromise nutrient sufficiency(Reference Galloway, Fiorito and Lee81). Therefore, current findings in conjunction with previous observations regarding food avoidance indicate that parents can benefit from receiving clear and evidence-based advice on food transitioning during infancy, as well as advice on the risk of food allergy based on family history by health professionals with relevant expertise.

Factors in relation to both age of introduction and type of solid foods

Among a number of sources of advice on complementary feeding, grandmothers and health professionals generated most discussion. Findings showed that views on the information given by medical professionals were polarised. Their advice was better received when it was incorporated through scheduled visits relative to other settings where caregivers had limited opportunities to consult a health professional. Another contributing factor of the diverging views might be the varying levels of health professionals’ knowledge of current evidence on complementary feeding. A survey by Chouraqui et al. revealed a discordance between parents’ statements of the infant feeding advice received and the statements of paediatricians who offered this advice(Reference Chouraqui, Delmas and Le Bris82). Hence, an assessment of healthcare staff’s knowledge of current complementary feeding guidelines and their perceived barriers to an effective nutritional education is required to complement parental perspectives and give directions for improved access to feeding recommendations. When it came to parental perspectives on grandmothers’ advice, mothers with great respect for older generations and teenage mums put a lot of trust in their mothers’ advice. However, such advice can be regarded as outdated and diverging from the official complementary feeding guidelines. Cross-sectional data confirm that infant feeding advice received by grandmothers can be undermining of official feeding guidelines by applying pressure to add cereal in the infant’s bottle(Reference Ashida, Lynn and Williams83). Adolescent mothers have been shown to be particularly influenced by their mothers’ advice, as they are more likely to live and have daily contact with them compared with adult mums(Reference Black, Siegel and Abel84). Furthermore, findings from observational studies suggest that grandmothers may have a negative influence on breast-feeding duration, but support provided by healthcare professionals has been associated with longer breast-feeding duration and postponed introduction of solid foods(Reference de Oliveira, Giugliani and Santo85Reference Pilkauskas88). Therefore, including grandmothers in counselling sessions with health professionals on complementary feeding could enhance grandmothers’ knowledge and support of recommended feeding practices. More studies are needed in order to assess the potential benefits and negative effects of external feeding advice from healthcare providers and grandmothers during the first years of life.

Even though the present review aimed to explore parental feeding experiences of both genders, it was observed that mothers represent the vast majority of research participants. This was not surprising, as fathers’ underrepresentation in research on childhood dietary behaviours has been highlighted in the past(Reference Davison, Gicevic and Aftosmes-Tobio89). The male-derived data included in the present review indicated that fathers played an assisting role during complementary feeding whilst allowing their female partners to lead in the decision-making process regarding timing of introduction and type of complementary foods. The quantitative literature that looked at both parents’ feeding practices also suggests that mothers and fathers have distinct interaction with children regarding food with fathers being less likely to monitor children’s food intake and apply restrictions on food compared with mothers(Reference Davison, Haines and Garcia90). In general, literature on fathers’ feeding practices is limited and focuses primarily on childhood nutrition as opposed to infancy. However, mealtime interactions between fathers and children are important and fathers can be influential role models of dietary behaviours, as observational studies demonstrate(Reference Harris and Ramsey91,Reference Walsh, Cameron and Crawford92) . Further qualitative studies would be greatly valuable in elucidating paternal perceptions of their role in shaping their children’s dietary behaviours during the first years of life.

Critical appraisal of included studies

This systematic review also highlights areas lacking information within the included qualitative literature with the main ones being the limitations of findings and authors’ conflicts of interest. Reporting on the limitations enables other researchers to critically appraise the trustworthiness of study findings; as for including a declaration of competing interests, it clarifies whether there are potential sources of influence on study conduct and conclusions. Inclusion of these elements therefore contributes to the clarity and completeness of reporting. Overall, adhering to standards of reporting can contribute to the clarity of qualitative manuscripts and help their authors disseminate them successfully.

Limitations

Like in all systematic reviews of qualitative evidence, the final synthesis is prone to vary between researchers and to be driven by their individual research priorities and scientific background(Reference Sandelowski, Docherty and Emden93). In order to deal with this, great care went to achieve a methodological interpretation and aggregation of study findings, and the use of an explicit protocol offered transparency to this process. Additionally, alongside the first author, two additional reviewers monitored the synthesis process and offered guidance.

The majority of qualitative research on parental feeding practices during complementary feeding currently comes from the UK, USA and Australia. Hence, the views of parents living in other upper-middle- and high-income countries, particularly in Europe and Asia, were underrepresented limiting the generalisability of the findings of this systematic review. Moreover, the information available on the socio-economic and family status of studies’ participants was limited. As a result, it was difficult to differentiate between feeding experiences of parents living in distinct social and family circumstances.

When reading this review, one also needs to consider that the feeding practices may be falsely self-reported if parents feel that they are being evaluated by the researcher. For instance, parents may disguise their poor compliance with feeding guidelines, which can become a barrier to an in-depth exploration of the underlying factors of poor compliance with the guidelines.

Despite its limitations, the process of undertaking this systematic review was characterised by transparency with its protocol having been published before the literature search. This paper was also structured according to the PRISMA guidance to ensure clear presentation of design and findings(Reference Moher, Liberati and Tetzlaff24).

Two previous systematic reviews by Harrison et al. and Matvienko-Sikar et al. have explored parental perceptions of and experiences during complementary feeding(Reference Harrison, Brodribb and Hepworth18,Reference Matvienko-Sikar, Kelly and Sinnott19) . The present paper, however, provides an updated review of the evidence and provides an in-depth exploration of topics that were not previously addressed (e.g. experiences of baby-led-weaning mothers, paternal involvement in complementary feeding and an overview of all factors that influence choice of complementary foods). Consequently, the additional studies that were incorporated in the present review contributed to a more comprehensive synthesis of the factors in relation to parental feeding practices.

Conclusions and future recommendations

This systematic review of qualitative evidence provided a comprehensive report on factors that parents take into consideration during complementary feeding in upper-middle- and high-income countries. Early introduction of complementary feeding was often reported, and misinterpreting baby cues as readiness for solids was quoted as the main reason. Parents demonstrated poor understanding of the evidence base of the complementary feeding guidelines, which were perceived to be a one-size-fits-all approach. The choice of complementary foods was mainly based on the perceived health properties of foods, though there was often uncertainty as to which foods need to be avoided at what stage.

These findings indicate a number of factors that can be barriers to complying with the complementary feeding guidelines and therefore are pertinent to policy makers, researchers and health practitioners that aim to help parents improve their feeding practices. Committees involved in developing national guidelines and recommendations around complementary feeding need to be mindful of the value of practical and easy-to-understand guidelines that reflect current research and acknowledge the cultural background of caregivers living in that area. The complementary feeding recommendations for every country should be discussed with parents with flexibility and without disregarding their children’s unique characteristics and needs. Attention needs to be drawn to vulnerable groups of parents (teenage mums or mothers living remotely with limited access to healthcare staff) who, due to their personal circumstances, are more prone to seek and take on board advice that may be outdated or not evidence based. Widening complementary feeding education to include fathers and other family members (e.g. grandmothers) may help to increase mothers’ feeling of being supported in feeding their child by ensuring a supportive environment and reducing pressure to go against guidelines. Fathers’ role during the period of complementary feeding is currently under-researched.

Acknowledgements

Acknowledgements: The authors would like to thank Dr Caroline McGirr who assisted with abstract screening after the first literature search conducted for this systematic review. Financial support: This study was conducted as part of a doctorate programme funded by Department of Education, Northern Ireland. Conflict of interest: There are no conflicts of interest. Authorship: All authors conceptualised the present study and significantly contributed to creating the present manuscript. E.S. designed the search strategy, conducted the literature search, data extraction and synthesis, performed the quality appraisal of included studies and drafted the manuscript. M.C.M.K. performed independent abstract screening, ensured that methods comply with transparency and reproducibility standards, provided advice in interpretation of findings and reviewed the manuscript. M.D. supervised all stages, provided advice in interpretation of findings and reviewed the manuscript. Ethics of human subject participation: Not applicable.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980021001749

References

Gibney, MJ & Macdonald, IA (2003) Core concepts of nutrition. In Nutrition and Metabolism, 2nd ed., pp. 16 [Lanham-New, SA, Macdonald, IA & Roche, HM, editors]. West Sussex, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Castenmiller, J, De Henauw, S, Hirsch-Ernst, K-I et al. (2019) Appropriate age range for introduction of complementary feeding into an infant’s diet. EFSA J 17, e05780.Google ScholarPubMed
World Health Organisation (2019) Complementary feeding. https://www.who.int/nutrition/topics/complementary_feeding/en (accessed October 2020).Google Scholar
World Health Organisation (2001) The optimal duration of exclusive breastfeeding: report of the expert consultation. https://www.who.int/nutrition/publications/infantfeeding/WHO_NHD_01.09/en (accessed October 2020).Google Scholar
Joppich, R, Kollmann, D, Ingrisch, U et al. (1977) Urinary cyclic AMP and renal concentrating capacity in infants. Eur J Pediatr 124, 113119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowen, WH, Pearson, SK, Rosalen, PL et al. (1997) Assessing the cariogenic potential of some infant formulas, milk and sugar solutions. J Am Dent Assoc 128, 865871.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panamerican Health Organisation & World Health Organisation (2003) Guiding principles for complementary feeding of the breastfed child. https://www.who.int/maternal_child_adolescent/documents/a85622/en (accessed October 2020).Google Scholar
Nicklaus, S (2011) Children’s acceptance of new foods at weaning. Role of practices of weaning and of food sensory properties. Appetite 57, 812815.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DiSantis, KI, Hodges, EA, Johnson, SL et al. (2011) The role of responsive feeding in overweight during infancy and toddlerhood: a systematic review. Int J Obes (Lond) 35, 480492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lennox, A, Somerville, J, Ong, K et al. (2013) Diet and nutrition survey of infants and young children, 2011. A survey carried out on behalf of the Department of Health and Food Standards Agency. https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/139572/DNSIYC_UK_report_ALL_chapters_DH_V10.0.pdf (accessed October 2020).Google Scholar
Faber, M, Laubscher, R & Berti, C (2016) Poor dietary diversity and low nutrient density of the complementary diet for 6- to 24-month-old children in urban and rural KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa. Matern Child Nutr 12, 528545.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roess, AA, Jacquier, EF, Catellier, DJ et al. (2018) Food consumption patterns of infants and toddlers: findings from the feeding infants and toddlers study (FITS) 2016. J Nutr 148, 1525s1535s.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Australian Institute of Health and Welfare (2011) 2010 Australian National Infant Feeding Survey: indicator results. https://www.aihw.gov.au/getmedia/af2fe025–637e-4c09-ba03–33e69f49aba7/13632.pdf.aspx?inline=true (accessed October 2020).Google Scholar
Caetano, MC, Ortiz, TT, Silva, SG et al. (2010) Complementary feeding: inappropriate practices in infants. J Pediatr (Rio J) 86, 196201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schiess, S, Grote, V, Scaglioni, S et al. (2010) Introduction of complementary feeding in 5 European countries. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 50, 9298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castro, PD, Layte, R & Kearney, J (2014) Ethnic variation in breastfeeding and complimentary feeding in the Republic of Ireland. Nutrients 6, 18321849.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swift, JA & Tischler, V (2010) Qualitative research in nutrition and dietetics: getting started. J Hum Nutr Diet 23, 559566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrison, M, Brodribb, W & Hepworth, J (2017) A qualitative systematic review of maternal infant feeding practices in transitioning from milk feeds to family foods. Matern Child Nutr 13, e12360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matvienko-Sikar, K, Kelly, C, Sinnott, C et al. (2018) Parental experiences and perceptions of infant complementary feeding: a qualitative evidence synthesis. Obes Rev 19, 501517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dewey, KG & Adu-Afarwuah, S (2008) Systematic review of the efficacy and effectiveness of complementary feeding interventions in developing countries. Matern Child Nutr 4, Suppl. 1, 2485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnett-Page, E & Thomas, J (2009) Methods for the synthesis of qualitative research: a critical review. BMC Med Res Methodol 9, 59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandelowski, MB (2006) Handbook for Synthesizing Qualitative Research. New York, USA: Springer Publishing Company.Google Scholar
O’Brien, BC, Harris, IB, Beckman, TJ et al. (2014) Standards for reporting qualitative research: a synthesis of recommendations. Acad Med 89, 12451251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moher, D, Liberati, A, Tetzlaff, J et al. (2009) Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. BMJ 339, b2535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horodynski, MA & Mills, KJ (2014) The voice of low-income adolescent mothers on infant feeding. J Ext 52, n6.Google Scholar
Anderson, AS, Guthrie, CA, Alder, EM et al. (2001) Rattling the plate--reasons and rationales for early weaning. Health Educ Res 16, 471479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, KE, Nicklas, JC, Spence, M et al. (2010) Roles, perceptions and control of infant feeding among low-income fathers. Public Health Nutr 13, 522530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arden, MA (2010) Conflicting influences on UK mothers’ decisions to introduce solid foods to their infants. Matern Child Nutr 6, 159173.Google ScholarPubMed
Heinig, MJ, Follett, JR, Ishii, KD et al. (2006) Barriers to compliance with infant-feeding recommendations among low-income women. J Hum Lact 22, 2738.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horodynski, M, Olson, B, Arndt, MJ et al. (2007) Low-income mothers’ decisions regarding when and why to introduce solid foods to their infants: influencing factors. J Commun Health Nurs 24, 101118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kavanagh, KF, Habibi, M, Anderson, K et al. (2010) Caregiver- v. infant-oriented feeding: a model of infant-feeding strategies among special supplemental nutrition program for women, infants, and children participants in rural east Tennessee. J Am Diet Assoc 110, 14851491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, A & Brann, L (2015) Influence of cultural beliefs on infant feeding, postpartum and childcare practices among Chinese-American mothers in New York City. J Community Health 40, 476483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindsay, AC, Machado, MT, Sussner, KM et al. (2008) Infant-feeding practices and beliefs about complementary feeding among low-income Brazilian mothers: a qualitative study. Food Nutr Bull 29, 1524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redsell, SA, Atkinson, P, Nathan, D et al. (2010) Parents’ beliefs about appropriate infant size, growth and feeding behaviour: implications for the prevention of childhood obesity. BMC Public Health 10, 711.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russell, CG, Taki, S, Azadi, L et al. (2016) A qualitative study of the infant feeding beliefs and behaviours of mothers with low educational attainment. BMC Pediatr 16, 69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savage, JS, Neshteruk, CD, Balantekin, KN et al. (2016) Low-income women’s feeding practices and perceptions of dietary guidance: a qualitative study. Matern Child Health J 20, 25102517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, C, Madrelle, J, Vereijken, CM et al. (2013) Complementary feeding and “donner les bases du gout” (providing the foundation of taste). A qualitative approach to understand weaning practices, attitudes and experiences by French mothers. Appetite 71, 321331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Synnott, K, Bogue, J, Edwards, CA et al. (2007) Parental perceptions of feeding practices in five European countries: an exploratory study. Eur J Clin Nutr 61, 946956.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tully, L, Allen-Walker, V, Spyreli, E et al. (2019) Solid advice: complementary feeding experiences among disadvantaged parents in two countries. Matern Child Nutr 15, e12801.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walsh, A, Kearney, L & Dennis, N (2015) Factors influencing first-time mothers’ introduction of complementary foods: a qualitative exploration. BMC Public Health 15, 939.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abu Shosha, GM (2020) Weaning practices of Jordanian mothers: a qualitative study. Health Soc Care Community. Published online: 17 September 2020. doi: 10.1111/hsc.13165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Begley, A, Ringrose, K, Giglia, R et al. (2019) Mothers’ understanding of infant feeding guidelines and their associated practices: a qualitative analysis. Int J Environ Res Public Health 16, 1141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garcia, AL, Looby, S, McLean-Guthrie, K et al. (2019) An exploration of complementary feeding practices, information needs and sources. Int J Environ Res Public Health 16, 4311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanepoel, L, Henderson, J & Maher, J (2020) Mothers’ experiences with complementary feeding: conventional and baby-led approaches. Nutr Diet 77, 373381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arden, MA & Abbott, RL (2015) Experiences of baby-led weaning: trust, control and renegotiation. Matern Child Nutr 11, 829844.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, A & Lee, M (2013) An exploration of experiences of mothers following a baby-led weaning style: developmental readiness for complementary foods. Matern Child Nutr 9, 233243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cameron, SL, Heath, AL & Taylor, RW (2012) Healthcare professionals’ and mothers’ knowledge of, attitudes to and experiences with, Baby-Led Weaning: a content analysis study. BMJ Open 2, e001542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monterrosa, EC, Pelto, GH, Frongillo, EA et al. (2012) Constructing maternal knowledge frameworks. How mothers conceptualize complementary feeding. Appetite 59, 377384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jessri, M, Farmer, AP & Olson, K (2015) A focused ethnographic assessment of Middle Eastern mothers’ infant feeding practices in Canada. Matern Child Nutr 11, 673686.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodriguez-Oliveros, MG, Bisogni, CA & Frongillo, EA (2014) Knowledge about food classification systems and value attributes provides insight for understanding complementary food choices in Mexican working mothers. Appetite 83, 144152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNally, J, Hugh-Jones, S & Hetherington, MM (2020) “An invisible map” – maternal perceptions of hunger, satiation and ‘enough’ in the context of baby led and traditional complementary feeding practices. Appetite 148, 104608.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dogan, S, Sert, ZE & Topcu, S (2019) Practices, beliefs and attitudes about complementary feeding among Turkish mothers: a qualitative study. Prog Nutr 21, 769775.Google Scholar
McFarren, H, Vazquez, C, Jacobs, EA et al. (2020) Understanding feeding practices of Latinx mothers of infants and toddlers at risk for childhood obesity: a qualitative study. Matern Child Nutr 16, e12983.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yue, A, Marsh, L, Zhou, H et al. (2016) Nutritional deficiencies, the absence of information and caregiver shortcomings: a qualitative analysis of infant feeding practices in rural China. PLoS One 11, e0153385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caton, SJ, Ahern, SM & Hetherington, MM (2011) Vegetables by stealth. An exploratory study investigating the introduction of vegetables in the weaning period. Appetite 57, 816825.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nielsen, A, Michaelsen, KF & Holm, L (2013) Parental concerns about complementary feeding: differences according to interviews with mothers with children of 7 and 13 months of age. Eur J Clin Nutr 67, 11571162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, AL, Hoeft, KS, Takayama, JI et al. (2018) Beliefs and practices regarding solid food introduction among Latino parents in Northern California. Appetite 120, 381387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boak, R, Virgo-Milton, M, Hoare, A et al. (2016) Choosing foods for infants: a qualitative study of the factors that influence mothers. Child Care Health Dev 42, 359369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheney, AM, Nieri, T, Davis, E et al. (2019) The sociocultural factors underlying latina mothers’ infant feeding practices. Glob Qual Nurs Res 6, 2333393618825253.Google ScholarPubMed
Dutta, T, Sywulka, SM, Frongillo, EA et al. (2006) Characteristics attributed to complementary foods by caregivers in four countries of Latin America and the Caribbean. Food Nutr Bull 27, 316326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merriman, BG, Doyle, S, McDaid, E et al. (2013) Report on the Qualitative Study of Infants and their Parents at Wave 1 (9 Months). Dublin: Department of Children and Youth Affairs.Google Scholar
Spyreli, E, McKinley, MC, Allen-Walker, V et al. (2019) “The one time you have control over what they eat”: a qualitative exploration of mothers’ practices to establish healthy eating behaviours during weaning. Nutrients 11, 562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chakona, G (2020) Social circumstances and cultural beliefs influence maternal nutrition, breastfeeding and child feeding practices in South Africa. Nutr J 19, 47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wrottesley, SV, Prioreschi, A, Slemming, W et al. (2020) Maternal perspectives on infant feeding practices in Soweto, South Africa. Public Health Nutr. Published online: 10 August 2020. doi: 10.1017/S1368980020002451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horodynski, MA, Calcatera, M & Carpenter, A (2012) Infant feeding practices: perceptions of Native American mothers and health paraprofessionals. Health Educ J 71, 327339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van der Merwe, J, Bourne, L & Marais, D (2007) An assessment of preliminary food-based dietary guidelines for infants 6–12 months of age in the Little Karoo area of the Western Cape Province of South Africa. Public Health Nutr 10, 869877.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leung, G (2017) Cultural considerations in postnatal dietary and infant feeding practices among Chinese mothers in London. Br J Midwifery 25, 1824.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thullen, M, Majee, W & Davis, AN (2016) Co-parenting and feeding in early childhood: reflections of parent dyads on how they manage the developmental stages of feeding over the first 3 years. Appetite 105, 334343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rapley, G (2011) Baby-led weaning: transitioning to solid foods at the baby’s own pace. Community Pract 84, 2023.Google Scholar
Griauzde, DH, Kieffer, EC, Domoff, SE et al. (2020) The influence of social media on child feeding practices and beliefs among Hispanic mothers: a mixed methods study. Eat Behav 36, 101361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bramhagen, AC, Axelsson, I & Hallstrom, I (2006) Mothers’ experiences of feeding situations - an interview study. J Clin Nurs 15, 2934.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, A & Harries, V (2015) Infant sleep and night feeding patterns during later infancy: association with breastfeeding frequency, daytime complementary food intake, and infant weight. Breastfeed Med 10, 246252.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Agostoni, C, Decsi, T, Fewtrell, M et al. (2008) Complementary feeding: a commentary by the ESPGHAN Committee on Nutrition. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 46, 99110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
NHS Start4Life (2019) Your pregnancy and baby guide: your baby’s first solid foods 2019. https://www.nhs.uk/conditions/pregnancy-and-baby/solid-foods-weaning/ (accessed October 2020).Google Scholar
Rogers, SL & Blissett, J (2019) Infant temperament, maternal feeding behaviours and the timing of solid food introduction. Matern Child Nutr 15, e12771.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doub, AE, Moding, KJ & Stifter, CA (2015) Infant and maternal predictors of early life feeding decisions. The timing of solid food introduction. Appetite 92, 261268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wasser, H, Bentley, M, Borja, J et al. (2011) Infants perceived as “fussy” are more likely to receive complementary foods before 4 months. Pediatrics 127, 229237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuo, AA, Inkelas, M, Slusser, WM et al. (2011) Introduction of solid food to young infants. Matern Child Health J 15, 11851194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McAndrew, F, Thompson, J, Fellows, L et al. (2012) Infant feeding survey 2010. Leeds: Health and Social Care Information Centre; available at https://sp.ukdataservice.ac.uk/doc/7281/mrdoc/pdf/7281_ifs-uk-2010_report.pdf (accessed October 2020).Google Scholar
Dangour, AD, Dodhia, SK, Hayter, A et al. (2009) Nutritional quality of organic foods: a systematic review. Am J Clin Nutr 90, 680685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galloway, AT, Fiorito, L, Lee, Y et al. (2005) Parental pressure, dietary patterns, and weight status among girls who are “picky eaters”. J Am Diet Assoc 105, 541548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chouraqui, JP, Delmas, B, Le Bris, M et al. (2019) Physicians advice, parental practice and adherence to doctor’s advice: an original survey on infant feeding. BMC Pediatr 19, 313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashida, S, Lynn, FB, Williams, NA et al. (2016) Competing infant feeding information in mothers’ networks: advice that supports v. undermines clinical recommendations. Public Health Nutr 19, 12001210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Black, MM, Siegel, EH, Abel, Y et al. (2001) Home and videotape intervention delays early complementary feeding among adolescent mothers. Pediatrics 107, E67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Oliveira, LD, Giugliani, ER, Santo, LC et al. (2012) Impact of a strategy to prevent the introduction of non-breast milk and complementary foods during the first 6 months of life: a randomized clinical trial with adolescent mothers and grandmothers. Early Hum Dev 88, 357361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giugliani, ER, do Espirito Santo, LC, de Oliveira, LD et al. (2008) Intake of water, herbal teas and non-breast milks during the first month of life: associated factors and impact on breastfeeding duration. Early Hum Dev 84, 305310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grassley, J & Eschiti, V (2008) Grandmother breastfeeding support: what do mothers need and want? Birth 35, 329335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pilkauskas, NV (2014) Breastfeeding initiation and duration in coresident grandparent, mother and infant households. Matern Child Health J 18, 19551963.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davison, KK, Gicevic, S, Aftosmes-Tobio, A et al. (2016) Fathers’ representation in observational studies on parenting and childhood obesity: a systematic review and content analysis. Am J Public Health 106, e14e21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davison, KK, Haines, J, Garcia, EA et al. (2020) Fathers’ food parenting: a scoping review of the literature from 1990 to 2019. Pediatr Obes 15, e12654.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, TS & Ramsey, M (2015) Paternal modeling, household availability, and paternal intake as predictors of fruit, vegetable, and sweetened beverage consumption among African American children. Appetite 85, 171177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walsh, AD, Cameron, AJ, Crawford, D et al. (2016) Dietary associations of fathers and their children between the ages of 20 months and 5 years. Public Health Nutr 19, 20332039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandelowski, M, Docherty, S & Emden, C (1997) Focus on qualitative methods. Qualitative metasynthesis: issues and techniques. Res Nurs Health 20, 365371.3.0.CO;2-E>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Eligibility criteria for inclusion and exclusion of reviewed studies

Figure 1

Table 2 Keywords used in search of electronic databases

Figure 2

Fig. 1 PRISMA flowchart of study selection

Figure 3

Fig. 2 Scoring of individual studies according to the ‘Standards of Reporting Qualitative Research’ assessment tool. , Number of items sufficiently covered in paper; , number of items partially covered in paper; , number of items for which no information was provided

Figure 4

Fig. 3 Themes and sub-themes emerging from reviewing the qualitative research on complementary feeding. Themes are classified into (i) factors related to the timing of complementary feeding; (ii) factors related to the type of complementary foods and (iii) factors related to both timing and type of complementary foods

Supplementary material: File

Spyreli et al. supplementary material

Spyreli et al. supplementary material 1

Download Spyreli et al. supplementary material(File)
File 113.6 KB
Supplementary material: File

Spyreli et al. supplementary material

Spyreli et al. supplementary material2

Download Spyreli et al. supplementary material(File)
File 26.9 KB