Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T01:42:13.398Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Rurality and dietary patterns: associations in a UK cohort study of 10-year-old children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 September 2014

Tim T Morris*
Affiliation:
School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Oakfield House, Oakfield Grove, Bristol BS8 2BN, UK
Kate Northstone
Affiliation:
School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Oakfield House, Oakfield Grove, Bristol BS8 2BN, UK
*
* Corresponding author: Email Tim.Morris@bristol.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

Despite differences in obesity and ill health between urban and rural areas in the UK being well documented, very little is known about differences in dietary patterns across these areas. The present study aimed to examine whether urban/rural status is associated with dietary patterns in a population-based UK cohort study of children.

Design

Dietary patterns were obtained using principal components analysis and cluster analysis of 3 d diet records collected from children at 10 years of age. Rurality was obtained from the 2001 UK Census urban/rural indicator at the time of dietary assessment. General linear models were used to examine the relationship between rurality and dietary pattern scores from principal components analysis; multinomial logistic regression was used to assess the association between rurality and dietary clusters.

Setting

The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC), South West England.

Subjects

Children (n 5677) aged 10 years (2817 boys and 2860 girls).

Results

After adjustment, increases in rurality were associated with increased scores on the ‘health awareness’ dietary pattern (β=0·35; 95 % CI 0·14, 0·56; P<0·001 for the most rural compared with the most urban group) and lower scores on the ‘packed lunch/snack’ dietary pattern (β=−0·39; 95 % CI −0·59, −0·19; P<0·001 for the most rural compared with the most urban group). The odds ratio for participants being in the ‘healthy’ compared with the ‘processed’ dietary cluster for the most rural areas was 1·61 (95 % CI 1·05, 2·49; P=0·02) compared with those in the most urban areas.

Conclusions

There is evidence to suggest that differences exist in dietary patterns between rural and urban areas. Similar results were found using two different methods of dietary pattern analysis, showing that children residing in rural households were more likely to consume healthier diets than those in urban households.

Type
Research Papers
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2014 

Obesity continues to rise in the UK( Reference Sassi 1 ), posing a significant threat to the health of the nation. While there are many facets to the obesity epidemic( Reference Parsons, Power and Logan 2 ), multiple interventions and policy changes have failed to halt the continued rise over the past few years. Rates of obesity( Reference Mahmood and Arulkumaran 3 ) (and poor health in general( Reference Jones and Lake 4 )) in the UK vary considerably between rural and urban areas, whereby people living in rural areas are consistently healthier than their urban-dwelling counterparts. A greater understanding of this marked geographical difference may better inform policy interventions that are more effective at tackling the underlying causes and reducing the prevalence of obesity in the UK.

Diet plays a vital role in the development of obesity and numerous other health problems( Reference Rayner and Scarborough 5 ). Poor diet quality is cited as a major driver behind the obesity epidemic and is a highly modifiable risk factor for obesity and ill health( Reference Hensrud 6 ). This is particularly important for the development of children given that dietary habits formed in childhood and adolescence have been shown to track into adulthood( Reference DiPietro, Mossberg and Stunkard 7 , Reference Mikkilä, Räsänen and Raitakari 8 ). The role of diet in obesity development therefore makes it a sensible policy intervention target for improving the health of the nation in an attempt to halt and potentially reverse the obesity epidemic. For effective policy intervention though, it may be necessary to understand not only to whom health interventions can be most effectively targeted, but also geographically where they would be most effective; for instance, areas where dietary patterns tend to be of poorer quality. It is therefore important that geographical variations in diet – as an underlying driver of obesity and ill health – are well understood.

A number of studies have identified the influence that space and place have on dietary intake, both internationally( Reference Diez-Roux, Nieto and Caulfield 9 Reference House, Lepkowski and Williams 11 ) and in the UK( Reference Cummins and Macintyre 12 Reference Ellaway and Macintyre 14 ). However, research has focused largely on one of two factors: (i) the effects of neighbourhood-level deprivation( Reference Shohaimi, Welch and Bingham 15 Reference Thornton, Crawford and Ball 18 ); and (ii) the role that distance or access to food outlets plays in diet( Reference Wrigley and Warm 19 Reference Moore, Roux and Nettleton 24 ). Results from UK studies have been mixed( Reference Cummins, Smith and Taylor 25 ), indicating that geographical differences observed in neighbourhood diet quality may not be due to deprivation or affluence once individual-level variables such as income, education and class are accounted for and may not be due to accessibility or price either. This is understandable, as an increased choice of food outlets in the immediate local vicinity selling cheap healthy foods does not necessarily translate to a change in dietary habits. Habits tend to be formed over long periods of time from early-life experiences( Reference DiPietro, Mossberg and Stunkard 7 , Reference Mikkilä, Räsänen and Raitakari 8 ).

There has been very limited research dedicated to examining differences in dietary patterns between different rural/urban profiles of the UK. Given the documented differences in obesity and ill health between rural and urban residents in the UK, whereby residents of urban areas suffer poorer health than those from rural areas( Reference Mahmood and Arulkumaran 3 , Reference Jones and Lake 4 ), one may expect that diet quality varies geographically in a similar manner between urban and rural areas. Recent research from both the USA( Reference Asirvatham 26 , Reference Savoca, Arcury and Leng 27 ) and Canada( Reference Downs, Fraser and Storey 28 ) shows that in a North American context increasing rurality is associated with poorer nutrient profiles consisting of higher consumption of high-fat and high-sugar foods. To our knowledge, however, little research has been conducted examining dietary intake and rurality in the UK, where urban/rural profiles clearly differ from Canada and the USA in terms of population demographics, income disparity and service provision. UK-based evidence shows a higher prevalence of more ‘traditional’ dietary patterns (based on core foods such as bread, potatoes, vegetables) in rural areas( Reference Barker, McClean and Thompson 29 ), and that adults in rural households and students in rural schools have higher fruit and vegetable intakes than their urban counterparts( Reference Morgan, Armstrong and Huppert 30 , Reference Levin, Kirby and Currie 31 ) despite poorer access to food in rural areas( Reference Smith, Cummins and Taylor 17 ). Research from other European countries also suggests a greater prevalence of higher-quality diets in rural households( Reference Naska, Fouskakis and Oikonomou 32 ).

The use of dietary patterns that reflect the total diet, as an alternative or complementary approach to studying the associations between diet and health, has increased considerably over recent years. These methods recognise that foods and drinks are consumed in combination and enable the study of the whole diet, rather than individual foods or nutrients. Two common methods for deriving dietary patterns are principal components analysis (PCA) and cluster analysis. PCA uses the correlations that exist between different food groups to identify linear combinations of foods that are frequently consumed together and gives each individual a score for every component. Cluster analysis, however, combines individuals into non-overlapping groups based on similarity of dietary intakes. To our knowledge, only two studies have been performed specifically examining UK differences in dietary patterns with a focus on rurality( Reference Barker, McClean and Thompson 29 , Reference Morgan, Armstrong and Huppert 30 ), but neither of these focused on children. The dietary patterns used in the present paper had already been obtained using both PCA and cluster analysis from dietary data collected from the study cohort using diet diaries at 10 years of age( Reference Northstone, Smith and Newby 33 , Reference Smith, Emmett and Newby 34 ).

Given the lack of UK-specific research on differences in dietary patterns between urban and rural areas, the aim of the present study was to examine whether the urban/rural indicator is associated with dietary patterns in a population-based UK cohort study of children.

Methods

Subjects were children from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). Pregnant women were eligible to join the study if they had an expected date of delivery between April 1991 and December 1992 and were resident in the (former) Avon Health Authority area in South West England( Reference Boyd, Golding and Macleod 35 ). A total sample of 14 701 children was used, including 13 988 children from the originally recruited sample who were still alive at 1 year of age and an additional 713 children who were recruited at later ages. The ALSPAC cohort is largely representative of the UK when compared with 1991 Census data, with slight under-representation in ethnic minorities, single-parent families and those living in rented accommodation. Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee and the Local Research Ethics Committees. Please note that the study website contains details of all the data that are available through a fully searchable data dictionary( 36 ).

The dietary assessment at 10 years of age has been described in detail elsewhere( Reference Cribb, Jones and Rogers 37 ). Briefly, children were invited to attend a hands-on dietary assessment in a clinic setting. Prior to the visit children were asked to record all foods and drinks that had been consumed over three full days (two weekdays and one weekend day). At the clinic children were interviewed by a nutrition fieldworker to allow the inclusion of any food that had been missed out and obtain a greater description of any portion sizes and cooking methods. Twenty-four-hour recalls were conducted by the fieldworkers in the event that a child had attended a clinic without a complete diet diary. Coding was completed by the fieldworker using the DIDO (Diet In, Data Out) software program developed by the MRC Human Nutrition Research Unit (Cambridge, UK).

Exposure

The 2001 Census urban/rural indicator (URI) was applied to ALSPAC geographic data; month and year of the clinic visit date were used to extract the relevant household residence from the ALSPAC address table by census Output Area (OA). 2001 OA contain a minimum of forty households and 100 people and are built from clusters of neighbouring postcode units; they avoid urban/rural mixes where possible and are designed to be socially homogeneous while maintaining similar population counts( 38 ). The URI was designed to permit research analysis ‘according to different types of rural and urban area’( 39 ) and as such separates English, Welsh and Scottish addresses by OA into four categories based upon population density: (i) ‘urban >10 000’, (ii) ‘town and fringe’, (iii) ‘village’ and (iv) ‘hamlet and isolated dwelling’, before splitting each of these categories into whether surrounding areas are either ‘sparse[ly]’ or ‘less sparse[ly]’ populated, to give a total of eight categories( 39 ). Few children fell into the ‘sparse’ categories (n 30), so URI values were recoded to include both sparse and less sparse in each rurality category to give a total of four categories for analysis.

Outcomes

Outcomes of interest were dietary patterns obtained through both PCA and cluster analysis. The patterns were derived from 3 d diet record data using gram weight (g/d) as input variables and have been described in detail elsewhere( Reference Smith, Emmett and Newby 34 ). Three dietary patterns were derived from PCA; (i) one contrasting more healthy (high-fibre bread, vegetables, water) with less healthy/processed foods (processed foods, chips, soft drinks; labelled ‘health awareness’); (ii) one with high loadings on traditional British foods (meat, potatoes and vegetables; labelled ‘traditional’); and (iii) the last with high loadings on foods that are often found in a child’s packed lunch such as white bread, sandwich fillings and snacks such as crisps and chocolate (labelled ‘packed lunch/snack’).

Four clusters were derived from the same data using k-means cluster analysis( Reference Northstone, Smith and Newby 33 ); these were labelled ‘processed’, ‘healthy’, ‘traditional’ and ‘packed lunch’ according to the average intake of foods consumed by the children falling into each cluster.

Potential confounders

Potential confounders were obtained from self-completion questionnaires and included highest maternal education at pregnancy (‘low’ (less than O-level, where O-level represents qualifications obtained at 16 years of age), ‘middle’ (O-level) and ‘high’ (higher than O-level, i.e. education beyond 16 years of age)); maternal age at delivery (<25 years, 25–29 years, 30–34 years and 35+ years); highest parental social class based on occupation( 40 ) as reported by the mother in pregnancy (with social class I as ‘high’ and social classes IV and V as ‘low’ (social classes IV and V were grouped together due to low cell counts in the most rural areas)); gender; and home ownership status reported at 10 years (categorised as owned/mortgaged, rented from council or housing association, and rented privately/other).

Statistical methods

Differences in mean PCA scores according to URI were examined using ANOVA while a χ 2 test was used to establish any difference between dietary cluster membership and URI. Multivariable linear regression analysis was used to estimate the effects of URI on each of the scores from PCA separately; first unadjusted and then adjusted for all potential confounders. Finally, multinomial logistic regression was used to determine relationships between level of rurality and dietary cluster membership, again employing both unadjusted models and those adjusted for confounders to obtain odds ratios and 95 % confidence intervals. Throughout ‘urban’ was used as the reference category. All statistical analyses were performed using the statistical software package STATA version 12.

Results

Of the 11 868 children invited to attend the clinic visit at 10 years of age, 7557 (64 %) attended, of whom 7473 provided dietary data (3702 boys and 3771 girls). Of those with dietary data, a further 1796 (24 %) children were excluded due to missing data on the exposure variable (n 61) or from one of the confounder variables (n 1735), leaving a total of 5677 cases for analysis. Missing data from the exposure variable was due to minor historical database migration errors resulting in no valid address and therefore no URI value attached. Twenty-four-hour recalls were conducted for 610 children (10·7 %) with no difference observed between children from different areas in terms of rurality (P=0·954).

Table 1 displays the differences in baseline characteristics of the cohort between those from different areas in terms of rurality. Differences were observed between rurality categories for maternal education, maternal age, social class and home ownership (all P<0·004). These differences indicate that children from rural areas were more likely to come from higher class and better educated families who owned/mortgaged their homes, and have older mothers than their more urban counterparts.

Table 1 Differences in baseline characteristics for ALSPAC children with complete data by URI categories

ALSPAC, Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children; URI, urban/rural indicator.

It can be seen in Table 2 that increasing rurality was associated with increases in mean ‘health awareness’ and ‘traditional’ dietary pattern scores and a decrease in mean ‘packed lunch/snack’ score. Table 3 shows that children living in increasingly urban areas were more likely to belong to the ‘processed’ and ‘packed lunch’ clusters. Conversely, children living in increasingly rural areas were more likely to belong to the ‘healthy’ and ‘traditional’ clusters.

Table 2 Dietary pattern scores (mean and standard deviation) obtained from PCA at 10 years of age among ALSPAC children by URI category

PCA, principal components analysis; ALSPAC, Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children; URI, urban/rural indicator.

Table 3 Associations between clusters of dietary pattern at 10 years of age among ALSPAC children and URI (χ 2=76·10, P<0·001)

ALSPAC, Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children; URI, urban/rural indicator.

Table 4 shows that unadjusted parameter estimates increased for the ‘health awareness’ dietary pattern with increasing rurality. Although the estimates were attenuated, the trend was still evident after adjustment (P<0·001). Conversely, the ‘packed lunch/snack’ dietary pattern showed a negative trend both before and after adjustment with increasing rurality (P<0·001). There appeared to be little difference in ‘traditional’ dietary pattern scores for children living in different areas.

Table 4 Unadjusted and adjustedFootnote * parameter estimates (and 95 % confidence intervals) for the associations between URI and dietary pattern scores obtained from PCA at 10 years of age among ALSPAC children

URI, urban/rural indicator; PCA, principal components analysis; ALSPAC, Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children.

* Adjusted for gender, maternal education, maternal age at delivery, highest parental social class and home ownership status.

Table 5 presents the results from the multinomial logistic regression presenting the odds ratios for being in the ‘healthy’, ‘traditional’ or ‘packed lunch’ cluster compared with the ‘processed’ cluster. The odds of being in the ‘healthy’ cluster compared with the ‘processed’ cluster increased with increasing rurality, with children living in hamlets/isolated dwellings being more than twice as likely to be in the ‘healthy’ cluster (OR=2·24; 95 % CI 1·48, 3·37). This association was attenuated after adjustment but a trend remained with increasing rurality (hamlet v. urban: adjusted OR=1·61; 95 % CI 1·05, 2·49). Results showed a small association between increased rurality and belonging to the ‘traditional cluster’ but the strength of these associations was weak (P=0·183). There was no evidence to suggest that URI was associated in any way with being in the ‘packed lunch’ cluster compared with the ‘processed’ cluster.

Table 5 Unadjusted and adjustedFootnote * odds ratios (and 95 % confidence intervals) for the associations between URI and clusters of dietary pattern at 10 years of age among ALSPAC children, using the ‘processed’ cluster as base outcome

URI, urban/rural indicator; ALSPAC, Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children.

* Adjusted for gender, maternal education, maternal age at delivery, highest parental social class and home ownership status. Note: 340 children with dietary data were unassigned to food clusters (290 from ‘urban’, 20 from ‘town’, 25 from ‘village’, 5 from ‘hamlet’).

Discussion

The present study explored urban/rural differences in dietary patterns obtained through both PCA and cluster analysis. The results indicate that rurality is associated with dietary patterns assessed using two distinct methods, even after adjustment for potential confounding factors. We have shown that rurality is associated with diet whereby children living in the most rural areas are more likely to consume what may be considered to be a healthier diet.

Results using the PCA scores showed that children living in the most rural areas were more likely to consume a healthy diet than those in urban areas, with increased health awareness scores and lower packed lunch/snack scores. There was evidence of a dose–response relationship between both health awareness and packed lunch/snack scores with increasing rurality.

Cluster analysis identified similar patterns to those from the PCA and the associations with rurality were mirrored. Children residing in rural areas were more likely to belong to the healthy cluster than those in urban areas. As with the PCA results, the likelihood of being in the healthy category increased with increasing rurality. Cluster analysis results from the traditional cluster were weak after adjustment for socio-economic characteristics but there was evidence to suggest that children in the most rural areas were more likely to fall into that cluster than children in more urban areas.

More informed knowledge on differences in dietary intake is of critical importance if we are to accurately and reliably identify the underlying mechanisms that may drive such differences between areas of differing rural profiles and help to further explain those that have been seen between diet and deprivation or access to food. Our findings show the opposite trends to those shown by Downs et al. in Canada( Reference Downs, Fraser and Storey 28 ) but they are consistent with UK findings investigating aspects of diet quality and rurality( Reference Morgan, Armstrong and Huppert 30 , Reference Levin, Kirby and Currie 31 ). These latter studies reported a higher prevalence of healthy dietary patterns in rural households and school areas. This contrast in international results demonstrates the clear differences in geographical profiles between the UK and North America and the resulting influence of geographical spaces determined by rurality on diet quality. While it is important that policy makers consider rurality and the potential effect that it may have on diet and therefore obesity and ill health, it is also important to remember that interventions may not be successfully transferred internationally due to cultural and geographical dissimilarities between countries.

While further research is required to support these findings in geographically diverse areas of the UK, our findings imply that interventions in the UK may have more impact in urban areas where poorer-quality dietary patterns are more prevalent and as such there may be more barriers to healthy diets. The increased association between rurality and nutrient-rich diets and the decreased association with snack foods (such as chocolate and crisps that are energy-dense and contribute few nutrients) suggest greater dietary health awareness in more rural areas. Our results also suggest that access to food in terms of geographical distance to food retail outlets (as observed by Smith et al. ( Reference Smith, Cummins and Taylor 17 )) may not be a major influence on diet quality, as we would have expected to see consistent results between poorer access in rural areas and poorer diet quality if this was the case. It is plausible that other factors associated with families who live in rural areas (such as employment and income) are perhaps more relevant than access to food( Reference Dibsdall, Lambert and Bobbin 41 ). The results from our adjusted analyses suggest that previous arguments claiming neighbourhood characteristics play no significant role on diet quality after individual-level characteristics have been accounted for( Reference Cummins and Macintyre 42 Reference White 44 ) may not be valid. Although residual confounding may be an issue in our study, adjusting for individual factors such as maternal education and social class only slightly attenuated our effect sizes. Further research is needed to determine exactly what may be driving geographic variations in diet quality but our study indicates that differences do exist above the individual level.

The current study adds to the scant research examining associations between diet quality and rurality in the UK. The dietary variables used were well defined, identifying particular diet patterns known to have different nutrient profiles( Reference Northstone, Smith and Newby 33 , Reference Smith, Emmett and Newby 34 ), and the inclusion of both PCA scores and dietary clusters allowed us to examine associations on two different measures. The data were from a large, generally representative sample of children from the UK. However, a number of limitations also exist with the study which must be acknowledged. First, it is only cross-sectional and as such it is not possible to determine causality. However, we consider reverse causality to be unlikely. Second, all dietary data were self-reported by the participants and as such may not have been completely accurate. While the use of nutritionists and interviews may have minimised errors in the data this is something that we should remain aware of. Third, small numbers in sparse areas restricted us from examining the effects of sparsity on dietary patterns. There is a possibility that the true associations between URI and diet quality may be underestimated due to the higher levels of drop out observed among participants living in increasingly urban areas. Finally, there is the possibility that underlying values of child rearing may exist in which people with healthier dietary patterns migrate to rural areas prior to pregnancy. However, such data are not available in the ALSPAC cohort, so we could not take this into account.

Conclusion

In conclusion, we have shown that geographical location in terms of rurality is associated with dietary patterns. The associations we report suggest that being resident in increasingly rural areas is linked to a higher prevalence of what may be considered healthier diets and a lesser tendency towards snack and processed foods; even after taking into account individual-level socio-economic confounding factors. Further work is needed to understand the mechanisms behind these differences, but the results presented here suggest that policy interventions to improve diet would be better targeted in urban than rural areas. While the cross-sectional nature of the research does not allow us to infer causality or direction of associations, our results are important for directing public health messages as they suggest that associations in diet quality truly exist at the spatial level and that rurality is itself independently linked to diet quality.

Acknowledgements

Acknowledgements: The authors are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists and nurses. Financial support: The UK Medical Research Council and the Wellcome Trust (grant ref. 092731) and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. The UK Medical Research Council, the Wellcome Trust and the University of Bristol had no role in the design, analysis or writing of this article. Conflict of interest: None. Authorship: Both authors contributed to the formulation of research questions, study design, interpretation of results and the final manuscript. Analysis was conducted by T.T.M. Ethics of human subject participation: Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee and the Local Research Ethics Committees.

References

1. Sassi, F (2010) Obesity and the Economics of Prevention: Fit Not Fat. Paris: OECD Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
2. Parsons, TJ, Power, C, Logan, S et al. (1999) Childhood predictors of adult obesity: a systematic review. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 23, Suppl. 8, S1S107.Google ScholarPubMed
3. Mahmood, T & Arulkumaran, S (2012) Obesity: A Ticking Time Bomb for Reproductive Health, 1st ed. London: Elsevier.Google Scholar
4. Jones, N & Lake, I (2013) The combined impact of rural residence and socio-economic status on premature mortality. Health Place 24, 9096.Google Scholar
5. Rayner, M & Scarborough, P (2005) The burden of food related ill health in the UK. J Epidemiol Community Health 59, 10541057.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6. Hensrud, DD (2004) Diet and obesity. Current Opin Gastroenterol 20, 119124.Google Scholar
7. DiPietro, L, Mossberg, HO & Stunkard, AJ (1994) A 40-year history of overweight children in Stockholm: life-time overweight, morbidity, and mortality. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 18, 585590.Google Scholar
8. Mikkilä, V, Räsänen, L, Raitakari, OT et al. (2005) Consistent dietary patterns identified from childhood to adulthood: the cardiovascular risk in Young Finns Study. Br J Nutr 93, 923931.Google Scholar
9. Diez-Roux, AV, Nieto, FJ, Caulfield, L et al. (1999) Neighbourhood differences in diet: the atherosclerosis risk in communities (ARIC) study. J Epidemiol Community Health 53, 5563.Google Scholar
10. Van Hooijdonk, C, Droomers, M, Deerenberg, IM et al. (2008) Higher mortality in urban neighbourhoods in The Netherlands: who is at risk? J Epidemiol Community Health 62, 499505.Google Scholar
11. House, JS, Lepkowski, JM, Williams, DR et al. (2000) Excess mortality among urban residents: how much, for whom, and why? Am J Public Health 90, 18981904.Google Scholar
12. Cummins, S & Macintyre, S (2006) Food environments and obesity – neighbourhood or nation? Int J Epidemiol 35, 100104.Google Scholar
13. Black, JL & Macinko, J (2008) Neighborhoods and obesity. Nutr Rev 66, 220.Google Scholar
14. Ellaway, A & Macintyre, S (1996) Does where you live predict health related behaviours?: a case study in Glasgow. Health Bull (Edinb) 54, 443446.Google Scholar
15. Shohaimi, S, Welch, A, Bingham, S et al. (2004) Residential area deprivation predicts fruit and vegetable consumption independently of individual educational level and occupational social class: a cross sectional population study in the Norfolk cohort of the European Prospective Investigation into Cancer (EPIC-Norfolk). J Epidemiol Community Health 58, 686691.Google Scholar
16. Macintyre, S (2007) Deprivation amplification revisited; or, is it always true that poorer places have poorer access to resources for healthy diets and physical activity? Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 4, 32.Google Scholar
17. Smith, DM, Cummins, S, Taylor, M et al. (2010) Neighbourhood food environment and area deprivation: spatial accessibility to grocery stores selling fresh fruit and vegetables in urban and rural settings. Int J Epidemiol 39, 277284.Google Scholar
18. Thornton, LE, Crawford, DA & Ball, K (2010) Neighbourhood-socioeconomic variation in women’s diet: the role of nutrition environments. Eur J Clin Nutr 64, 14231432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19. Wrigley, N & Warm, D (2003) Deprivation, diet, and food-retail access: findings from the Leeds ‘food deserts’ study. Environ Plann A 35, 151188.Google Scholar
20. Rose, D & Richards, R (2004) Food store access and household fruit and vegetable use among participants in the US Food Stamp Program. Public Health Nutr 7, 10811088.Google Scholar
21. McEachern, MG & Warnaby, G (2006) Food shopping behaviour in Scotland: the influence of relative rurality. Int J Consum Stud 30, 189201.Google Scholar
22. Morland, K, Diez Roux, AV & Wing, S (2006) Supermarkets, other food stores, and obesity: the atherosclerosis risk in communities study. Am J Prev Med 30, 333339.Google Scholar
23. Cummins, S, Findlay, A, Higgins, C et al. (2008) Reducing inequalities in health and diet: findings from a study on the impact of a food retail development. Environ Plann A 40, 402422.Google Scholar
24. Moore, LV, Roux, AVD, Nettleton, JA et al. (2008) Associations of the local food environment with diet quality – a comparison of assessments based on surveys and geographic information systems. Am J Epidemiol 167, 917924.Google Scholar
25. Cummins, S, Smith, DM, Taylor, M et al. (2009) Variations in fresh fruit and vegetable quality by store type, urban–rural setting and neighbourhood deprivation in Scotland. Public Health Nutr 12, 20442050.Google Scholar
26. Asirvatham, J (2009) Examining diet quality and body mass index in rural areas using a Quantile Regression Framework. Rev Region Stud 39, 149169.Google Scholar
27. Savoca, MR, Arcury, TA, Leng, X et al. (2009) The diet quality of rural older adults in the South as measured by healthy eating index-2005 varies by ethnicity. J Am Diet Assoc 109, 20632067.Google Scholar
28. Downs, SM, Fraser, SN, Storey, KE et al. (2012) Geography influences dietary intake, physical activity and weight status of adolescents. Nutr Metab 2012, 816834.Google Scholar
29. Barker, ME, McClean, SI, Thompson, KA et al. (1990) Dietary behaviours and sociocultural demographics in Northern Ireland. Br J Nutr 64, 319329.Google Scholar
30. Morgan, K, Armstrong, GK, Huppert, FA et al. (2000) Healthy ageing in urban and rural Britain: a comparison of exercise and diet. Age Ageing 29, 341348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
31. Levin, KA, Kirby, J, Currie, C et al. (2012) Trends in adolescent eating behaviour: a multilevel cross-sectional study of 11–15 year olds in Scotland, 2002–2010. J Public Health 34, 523531.Google Scholar
32. Naska, A, Fouskakis, D, Oikonomou, E et al. (2006) Dietary patterns and their socio-demographic determinants in 10 European countries: data from the DAFNE databank. Eur J Clin Nutr 60, 181190.Google Scholar
33. Northstone, K, Smith, A, Newby, P et al. (2013) Longitudinal comparisons of dietary patterns derived by cluster analysis in 7 to 13-year-old children. Br J Nutr 109, 20502058.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34. Smith, AD, Emmett, PM, Newby, PK et al. (2013) Dietary patterns obtained through principal components analysis: the effect of input variable quantification. Br J Nutr 109, 18811891.Google Scholar
35. Boyd, A, Golding, J, Macleod, J et al. (2013) Cohort profile: the ‘Children of the 90s’ – the index offspring of the Avon longitudinal study of parents and children. Int J Epidemiol 42, 111127.Google Scholar
36. Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (2013) Data dictionary. http://www.bris.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/data-access/data-dictionary/ (accessed January 2014).Google Scholar
37. Cribb, VL, Jones, LR, Rogers, IS et al. (2011) Is maternal education level associated with diet in 10-year-old children? Public Health Nutr 14, 20372048.Google Scholar
38. Office for National Statistics (2013) Output Areas (OA). http://www.ons.gov.uk/ons/guide-method/geography/beginner-s-guide/census/output-area--oas-/index.html (accessed July 2013).Google Scholar
39. Office for National Statistics (2012) ONS Postcode Directory User Guide 2012 v4.1. https://geoportal.statistics.gov.uk/geoportal/catalog/main/home.page (accessed September 2013).Google Scholar
40. Office of Population Censuses and Surveys (1990) Standard Occupational Classification. London: HMSO.Google Scholar
41. Dibsdall, LA, Lambert, N, Bobbin, RF et al. (2003) Low-income consumers’ attitudes and behaviour towards access, availability and motivation to eat fruit and vegetables. Public Health Nutr 6, 159168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42. Cummins, S & Macintyre, S (2002) A systematic study of an urban foodscape: the price and availability of food in Greater Glasgow. Urban Studies 39, 21152130.Google Scholar
43. Giskes, K, Turrell, G, van Lenthe, FJ et al. (2006) A multilevel study of socio-economic inequalities in food choice behaviour and dietary intake among the Dutch population: the GLOBE study. Public Health Nutr 9, 7583.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
44. White, M (2007) Food access and obesity. Obes Rev 8, 99107.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Differences in baseline characteristics for ALSPAC children with complete data by URI categories

Figure 1

Table 2 Dietary pattern scores (mean and standard deviation) obtained from PCA at 10 years of age among ALSPAC children by URI category

Figure 2

Table 3 Associations between clusters of dietary pattern at 10 years of age among ALSPAC children and URI (χ2=76·10, P<0·001)

Figure 3

Table 4 Unadjusted and adjusted* parameter estimates (and 95 % confidence intervals) for the associations between URI and dietary pattern scores obtained from PCA at 10 years of age among ALSPAC children

Figure 4

Table 5 Unadjusted and adjusted* odds ratios (and 95 % confidence intervals) for the associations between URI and clusters of dietary pattern at 10 years of age among ALSPAC children, using the ‘processed’ cluster as base outcome