Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-lrblm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-05T03:53:21.880Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Modeling the relationship between online L2 motivational self-system and EFL learners’ virtual exchange self-regulations: the mediator and moderator roles of L2 grit

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 February 2025

Amir Reza Rahimi
Affiliation:
Universitat de València, Spain (Arera2@alumni.uv.es)
Ana Sevilla-Pavón
Affiliation:
IULMA/Universitat de València, Spain (Ana.M.Sevilla@uv.es)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

While previous studies in computer-assisted language learning have extensively explored sociolinguistic factors, such as cultural competence, important psycholinguistic factors such as online L2 motivational self-system, L2 grit, and online self-regulation in relation to virtual exchange (VE) have remained widely unexplored. To address this gap, a study was conducted with 92 Spanish English as a foreign language learners who exchanged language and culture with Cypriot and Irish students and responded to questionnaires adapted for the study context, as part of the SOCIEMOVE (Socioemotional Skills Through Virtual Exchange) Project. The partial least squares structural equation modeling approach showed that language learners who set positive personal goals for the future and evaluate their current learning progress in VE can regulate their learning in it. Interestingly, the sign of authenticity gap was found in the study context, since learners’ motivation to learn in VE was higher compared to their previous language learning contexts, resulting in more effort and consistency of interest in setting their goals, evaluating their progress, and asking for help from others. Furthermore, learners’ L2 grit moderated and mediated the correlation between learners’ online motivation and online self-regulation, indicating that VE success requires long-term perseverance of effort and consistency of interest. Accordingly, a new conceptual framework for VE was developed. In addition, one of the main implications is that teachers who employ VE should focus more on learners’ current needs and the goals they wish to achieve when exchanging information rather than only focusing on their accomplishments based on the course syllabus.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of EUROCALL, the European Association for Computer-Assisted Language Learning

1. Introduction

Increasing trade tensions, political instability, polarization and populism have made connecting more critical than ever for people from different cultures. Virtual exchange (VE) is widely recognized as one of the most effective contexts for enhancing students’ intercultural awareness and proficiency in foreign languages. Researchers in computer-assisted language learning (CALL) have documented a variety of positive outcomes associated with VE, including the development of language skills (Luo & Gui, Reference Luo and Gui2021), pragmatic competence (Zhang, Reference Zhang2024), grammatical proficiency (Zhang, Reference Zhang2024), lexical competence (Liu, Zhang & Zhang, Reference Liu, Zhang and Zhang2024), and 21st-century digital competences (Sevilla-Pavón & Nicolaou, Reference Sevilla-Pavón and Nicolaou2022). Furthermore, its importance has been highlighted for fostering intercultural communicative competence (ICC; O’Dowd, Reference O’Dowd2021b).

Despite the steady growth of VE in the last two decades, several gaps have yet to be addressed. First, many VE studies rely on qualitative data gathering and analysis with relatively small sample sizes, this being one of the main issues found in such studies (Rienties & Rets, Reference Rienties and Rets2022). In line with this, Baroni et al., Reference Baroni, Dooly, Garcés García, Guth, Hauck, Helm, Lewis, Mueller-Hartmann, O’Dowd, Rienties and Rogaten2019) underlined that VE studies failed to implement evidence-based designs or use extensive samples to show its influence. This concern was also highlighted by Golonka, Bowles, Frank, Richardson and Freynik (Reference Golonka, Bowles, Frank, Richardson and Freynik2014), who reported similar findings in their critical review of 350 CALL studies, which were predominantly descriptive case studies with small sample sizes and poor variables.

Second, most studies reported that VE successfully developed learners’ ICC, language skills, or subskills (Luo & Gui, Reference Luo and Gui2021; Zhang, Reference Zhang2024), indicating its success from sociolinguistic and language teaching perspectives. However, in comparison to face-to-face settings, VE’s ecosystem has unique affordances and constraints that may influence learners’ psychology. According to Müller-Hartmann (Reference Müller-Hartmann2005), internet-mediated intercultural exchange is both highly complex and pedagogically challenging, as the “complex learning environments of telecollaborative exchanges” (Kurek, Reference Kurek2015: 27) and “the complexity of the actual telecollaborative classroom” (Grau & Turula, Reference Grau and Turula2019: 110) culminate in “instability and unpredictability” due to a host of different elements involved (i.e. tasks, participants, materials) (Kurek, Reference Kurek2015). A further layer of complexity is added by issues such as failed communication, technological difficulties and tensions, conflicts, and power issues arising from differences in expectations, motivations, and cultural backgrounds. Given this complexity, psychological traits like motivation, persistence, and self-regulation learning (SRL) are key to making sure that VE practices work, ensuring students can handle the difficulties and complexity involved, which could lead to better academic performance. Accordingly, students need to develop cognitive, affective, and behavioral factors to persevere and succeed. These include online motivation, online self-regulation, L2 grit, and emotional engagement (Dörnyei & Ryan, Reference Dörnyei and Ryan2015) and are among the dynamic complex systems present in second language acquisition (SLA; Freeborn, Andringa, Lunansky & Rispens, Reference Freeborn, Andringa, Lunansky and Rispens2023), which, in spite of their relevance, have rarely been explored in VE. In a nutshell, VE has received relatively little attention as a successful tool from a psycholinguistic perspective. In their recent integrative reviews, leading experts recommended that scholars focus on aspects of VE other than language skills and ICC (Colpaert, Reference Colpaert2020; Luo & Yang, Reference Luo and Yang2021; O’Dowd, Reference O’Dowd2021a). In order to bridge these gaps, the present study was guided by online L2 motivational self-system (OL2MSS), L2 grit, and online self-regulation (OSEL). The integration of these psychological traits and factors was deemed appropriate for a better understanding of the psycholinguistic mechanisms underlying VE learning processes. Thus, researchers developed the following research questions:

  • RQ1: What are the factorial structures of OL2MSS, online L2 grit, and OSEL in VE and Spanish English as a foreign language (EFL) contexts?

  • RQ2: To what extent can learners’ OL2MSS predict their online L2 grit and OSEL in VE?

  • RQ3: To what extent can online L2 grit mediate the relationship between learners’ OL2MSS and their OSEL in VE?

2. Theoretical frameworks

2.1 Online L2 motivational self-system

Extensive research into L2 motivation has emphasized motivation’s internal structure as well as its relationship to other concepts. Dörnyei’s (Reference Dörnyei2009) L2 motivational self-system has dominated L2 motivation research, replicating Gardner’s (Reference Gardner1985) socio-educational model for two reasons. First, there is evidence that integrative motivation is feasible, for instance, with French-speaking learners in bilingual contexts, such as Belgium. However, its explanatory power is limited when it comes to language motivational behavior in EFL contexts (Liu, Reference Liu2024). Furthermore, Dörnyei (Reference Dörnyei2009) claimed that integrativeness is not possible in EFL contexts because learners do not have direct contact with the target learning context. Thus, he developed the L2MSS theory, which posits that learners’ motivation is composed of three main constructs: the ideal L2 self, or the image of one’s future self that expresses their desire to become a competent L2 speaker; the ought-to L2 self, or the sense of obligation to meet expectations and avoid negative consequences; and the L2 learning experience, or the specific contextual factors that motivate students to engage in language learning.

L2MSS has successfully been applied to single and multiple target languages in monolingual and multilingual contexts (Rahimi et al., Reference Rahimi, Sheyhkholeslami and Mahmoudi Pour2025). Its outputs have consistently grown, and SLA meta-analyses have demonstrated the model’s utility (Yousefi & Mahmoodi, Reference Yousefi and Mahmoodi2022). Although Dörnyei’s model reframes L2 motivation as a self-system, he does not fully elaborate on its function or provide a clear justification for its inclusion within the self-system. This poses difficulties in measuring discrepancies between present and future selves (Thorsen, Henry & Cliffordson, Reference Thorsen, Henry and Cliffordson2020), and the concept of the self-system remains a puzzle. In response to these criticisms, SLA scholars have extended and adapted this model, particularly by incorporating information and communications technology (ICT). For clarification, they introduced the current L2 self factor and compared its predictive power with that of the ideal L2 self in traditional (Henry & Cliffordson, Reference Henry and Cliffordson2017) and online language learning (OLL) contexts (Rahimi & Mosalli, Reference Rahimi and Mosalli2024; Smith, Foster, Baffoe-Djan, Li & Yu, Reference Smith, Foster, Baffoe-Djan, Li and Yu2020). Considering that digital language learning is closer to learners’ needs in their private lives, they devote much more motivation and effort to it, leading Henry and Cliffordson (Reference Henry and Cliffordson2017) to add the authenticity gap to the original L2MSS and SLA, which was validated by recent studies in language massive open online course (LMOOC; Rahimi, Reference Rahimi2023), ChatGPT-assisted language learning (Rahimi, Sheyhkholeslami & Mahmoudi Pour, Reference Rahimi, Sheyhkholeslami and Mahmoudi Pour2025), and OLL (Rahimi & Mosalli, Reference Rahimi and Mosalli2024; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Foster, Baffoe-Djan, Li and Yu2020). Since the exploratory power of these constructs in VE has not yet been explored, they were operationalized for the current study:

  • Ideal L2 self: learners’ desirable self-image to exchange language in VE to reach their future personal objectives.

  • Ought-to L2 self: learners’ sense of obligation to use the target language in VE to avoid negative outcomes, such as failing a university course.

  • Current L2 self: learners’ self-assessment of their current language proficiency and ability to use the language in VE contexts.

  • Digital self-authenticity: the comparison between students’ motivation in VE and their previous experiences with traditional language learning.

2.2 L2 grit

Grit is a relatively newly established individual trait defined as “perseverance and passion for long-term goals,” which comprises two constructs: perseverance of effort and consistency of interest (Duckworth, Peterson, Matthews & Kelly, Reference Duckworth, Peterson, Matthews and Kelly2007: 1087). Consistency of interest refers to an individual’s consistency of passion for a goal regardless of challenges, obstacles, or failures encountered along the way. Meanwhile, perseverance of effort refers to an individual’s ability to invest durable energy over a long period, which enables them to achieve incredible personal growth through more demanding challenges and interests (Duckworth et al., Reference Duckworth, Peterson, Matthews and Kelly2007). Nevertheless, a recent meta-analysis questioned the predictive power of general grit in language learning (Cred, Tynan & Harms, Reference Credé, Tynan and Harms2017), leading Teimouri, Plonsky and Tabandeh (Reference Teimouri, Plonsky and Tabandeh2022) to develop language-specific grit. Since then, exploring L2 grit and how it behaves in various language learning contexts has become an active research topic for both traditional face-to-face classrooms (Zhang, Reference Zhang2023), OLL (Paradowski & Jelińska, Reference Paradowski and Jelińska2024), and CALL environments (Zarrinabadi, Rezazadeh & Mohammadzadeh Mohammadabadi, Reference Zarrinabadi, Rezazadeh and Mohammadzadeh Mohammadabadi2024). It has, however, not been addressed in terms of its effect on learners’ behaviors in VE contexts, particularly in correlation with online motivation and self-regulation. Therefore, it is important to study these factors given that they are among complex dynamic systems in SLA that have a significant impact on L2 proficiency development (Oxford, Reference Oxford and Gałajda, D.2016).

2.3 Virtual exchange self-regulation

Self-regulation is derived from educational psychology and is a multifaceted and process-oriented concept (Dörnyei & Ryan, Reference Dörnyei and Ryan2015). It refers to “self-generated thoughts, feelings, and actions that are planned and cyclically adapted to attaining personal goals” (Zimmerman, Reference Zimmerman, Boekaerts, Pintrich and Zeidner2000: 14). Learning to regulate one’s own behavior becomes a cornerstone of successful communication, which is one of the most critical requirements of VE success. It serves as the internal conductor that guides students in translating their mental abilities into tangible language skills, as highlighted by Zimmerman (Reference Zimmerman, Boekaerts, Pintrich and Zeidner2000). This dynamic process encompasses motivation, metacognition, and behavioral control (Oxford, Reference Oxford and Gałajda, D.2016), and equips learners with tools that enable them to effectively manage emotions, thoughts, and behaviors (Zimmerman, Reference Zimmerman, Zimmerman and Schunk2001). This self-directed process culminates in the development of one’s capacity for continuous, conscious cognitive management, which includes self-regulation within complex dynamic systems in SLA (Oxford, Reference Oxford and Gałajda, D.2016).

Due to the context-specific behavior of psychological factors in various language learning contexts and the evolution and integration of ICT into education, where learners require more self-direction and control over their learning process, SLA researchers have started to explore the predictive power of self-regulation in digital language learning contexts. In this line, a six-factor questionnaire for assessing learners’ online self-regulation (OSEL) was developed by Barnard, Lan, To, Paton & Lai (Reference Barnard, Lan, To, Paton and Lai2009), which included goal setting, time management, task strategies, environment structuring, help-seeking, and self-evaluation. Furthermore, Fuchs et al. (Reference Fuchs, Pösse, Bedenlier, Gläser-Zikuda, Kammerl, Kopp, Ziegler and Händel2022) classified these six psychosocial factors into metacognitive strategies (goal setting and self-evaluation), resource management strategies (environment structuring, time management, and help-seeking), and cognitive strategies (task strategies).

OSEL has been applied or adapted to measure learners’ online self-regulatory strategies in different CALL contexts, such as ChatGPT-assisted language learning (Rahimi et al., Reference Rahimi, Sheyhkholeslami and Mahmoudi Pour2025) LMOOCs (Rahimi & Cheraghi, Reference Rahimi and Cheraghi2022) and OLL (Rahimi & Sevilla-Pavón, Reference Rahimi and Sevilla-Pavón2024b; Zheng, Liang, Chai, Chen & Liu, Reference Zheng, Liang, Chai, Chen and Liu2023). Having been designed to measure students’ OSEL for general purposes, the questionnaire needs more explanatory power when discussing learners’ OSEL strategies relevant to specific academic subfields. Although there has been substantial research on OSEL and technology-supported SRL (Fuchs et al., Reference Fuchs, Pösse, Bedenlier, Gläser-Zikuda, Kammerl, Kopp, Ziegler and Händel2022; Rahimi & Cheraghi, Reference Rahimi and Cheraghi2022; Zheng, Liang, Li & Tsai, Reference Zheng, Liang, Li and Tsai2018), there are limited empirical reports about learners’ OSEL in the CALL field, specifically in VE contexts such as the one in the present study.

3. Literature review and literature gap

CALL studies have indicated that motivation is one of the most critical factors for promoting learners’ psychological strengths, including emotional, cognitive, and behavioral factors (Lamb & Arisandy, Reference Lamb and Arisandy2020; Rahimi, Reference Rahimi2023). There is evidence that the ideal L2 self has a more significant impact on learners’ intended effort in commonly taught languages than in less commonly taught languages (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Foster, Baffoe-Djan, Li and Yu2020). Moreover, Gu and Cheung (2016) found both a direct and indirect impact of the ideal L2 self on intended effort through acculturation to the mainstream culture. Additionally, Rahimi (Reference Rahimi2023) reported that both future ideal and ought-to L2 self-images shaped Iranian EFL learners’ behavioral intentions to learn a language in LMOOCs. This may be due to the fact that motivational factors exhibit different behaviors depending on the environment and course (Rahimi, Reference Rahimi2023; You & Dörnyei, Reference You and Dörnyei2016). To clarify, Thompson & Liu (Reference Thompson and Liu2018) found that Chinese female language learners exhibited stronger ideal selves than male students and that high motivation was related to the ideal L2 self. Furthermore, lower-proficiency students displayed stronger ought-to L2 selves, possibly due to external pressures. In their study, You and Dörnyei (Reference You and Dörnyei2016) found that ideal and ought-to L2 selves significantly influenced Chinese learners’ attitudes and efforts to learn English. Additionally, motivation differed between more developed and less developed regions. These differences, which may have been influenced by social class and resource allocations, resulted in the finding that Global South learners were more motivated than Global North learners to maintain their efforts and attitudes toward learning. In Eastern and European countries, out-of-class digital learning environments negatively impacted learners’ motivation in their traditional language learning context (Henry, Reference Henry and Ushioda, E.2013). In fact, students may be motivated to seek similar self-congruent and aesthetically powerful experiences elsewhere due to the precious cognitive, emotional, and aesthetic experiences gained in digital environments (Rahimi et al., Reference Rahimi, Sheyhkholeslami and Mahmoudi Pour2025). Learners are expected to engage in very different social practices in digital learning formats, as opposed to those in the regimented and often mundane language classroom environments. This, caused by the lack of opportunities for autonomy, personal expression, and creativity (Henry & Cliffordson, Reference Henry and Cliffordson2017), led Henry and Cliffordson (Reference Henry and Cliffordson2017) to the observation of the “authenticity gap,” relating to traditional learning environments, which negatively affected learners’ efforts and motivation in classroom settings. They concluded that learners’ current L2 self and authenticity gap negatively affected their efforts in traditional learning settings. In a replication study conducted by Smith et al. (Reference Smith, Foster, Baffoe-Djan, Li and Yu2020), the authenticity gap and current L2 self were determined as predictors of Chinese language learners’ efforts in an online language context. Meanwhile, Lamb and Arisandy (Reference Lamb and Arisandy2020) did not observe any authenticity gaps in the Indonesian EFL context. Nevertheless, this gap was recently identified in the Iranian EFL context, and it was found to shape language learners’ intention to learn language in LMOOCs (Rahimi, Reference Rahimi2023) and OLL (Rahimi & Mosalli, Reference Rahimi and Mosalli2024). Although the role of L2 self-identities and digital self-authenticity has been studied in correlation with learners’ efforts and attitudes in various digital language learning contexts, their influence on learners’ self-direction strategies, efforts, and interests in other Western countries, such as Spain, has not been examined, resulting in the exploration of these correlations with learners’ L2 grit and virtual exchange self-regulation (VESRL) in VE in the current study.

L2 self-identities have been cited as one of the prerequisite factors in shaping learners’ self-regulation in both traditional (Kormos & Csizér, Reference Kormos and Csizér2013; Zimmerman & Kitsantas, Reference Zimmerman and Kitsantas2014) and digital language learning contexts (Rahimi & Cheraghi, Reference Rahimi and Cheraghi2022; Rahimi et al., Reference Rahimi, Sheyhkholeslami and Mahmoudi Pour2025). Rahimi and Cheraghi (Reference Rahimi and Cheraghi2022) and Zheng et al. (Reference Zheng, Liang, Li and Tsai2018) provided further supportive evidence that the cultural and instrumental interests of Chinese and Iranian English language learners affected their metacognitive, management, and cognitive strategies in asking for help from others and for evaluating their progress in OLLs and LMOOCs. In an effort to continue exploring the influence of the L2 motivational self-system on learners’ OSEL in digital language learning, the current study will use VE as a means to do so.

Alongside learners’ motivation and self-regulation, L2 grit has been recognized as a critical psychological factor that can influence learners’ language learning performance. However, due to its recent integration, only a few studies have examined its relationship with language learners’ motivation. It should be clarified that after developing the concept of L2 grit, Teimouri et al. (Reference Teimouri, Plonsky and Tabandeh2022) explored its correlation with EFL learners’ motivation, incorporating learners’ willingness to communicate (WTC), intended effort, mindset, and attention. They found that language learners’ motivation and their level of L2 grit were positively correlated. Furthermore, Paradowski and Jelińska (Reference Paradowski and Jelińska2024) reported similar findings regarding OLL, where learners were motivated to preserve their effort and devote more interest to their online language course due to their autonomy and online readiness. Furthermore, Fathi and Hejazi (Reference Fathi and Hejazi2024) found that Iranian EFL learners’ ideal L2 self was related to their L2 grit and that both language learners’ perseverance of effort and consistency of interest played a mediation role in their language learning enjoyment, as well as their achievement in traditional language learning contexts. Furthermore, the relationship between learners’ ideal L2 self and both writing achievements (Zhang, Reference Zhang2023) and WTC (Yu & Ma, Reference Yu and Ma2024) was found to be mediated by L2 grit. However, more research is needed to examine the mediation effects of L2 grit in other language learning contexts and in conjunction with other psychological factors, as the complex dynamic systems highlighted the interrelationship between language learners’ cognitive, effective, and behavioral factors in SLA (Oxford, Reference Oxford and Gałajda, D.2016) and every language learning setting, VE not being an exception. As a result, in this study, we are attempting to fill several gaps in the literature regarding psycholinguistics, applied linguistics, and VE. First, not only have learners’ motivational self-system, L2 grit, and OSEL been under-researched, but also no study has confirmed the factorial structure of these constructs in VE. Second, there have been no prior studies in VE that look at how online L2 self-identities affect the psychological factors that language learners use to have successful language exchanges, even though L2MSS acts in different ways in different language learning situations (Henry & Cliffordson, Reference Henry and Cliffordson2017; You & Dörnyei, Reference You and Dörnyei2016). Third, while applied linguistics has previously discussed the role of motivational self-system factors, such as the ideal self and ought-to self, in shaping language learners’ success, the two most recent factors incorporated into L2MSS, the current L2 self and digital self-authenticity, have not been considered. In this regard, recent studies recommend exploring the predictive differences between the current L2 self and the ideal L2 self in predicting learners’ behaviors (Rahimi & Mosalli, Reference Rahimi and Mosalli2024; Rahimi et al., Reference Rahimi, Sheyhkholeslami and Mahmoudi Pour2025; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Foster, Baffoe-Djan, Li and Yu2020). Moreover, even though digital self-authenticity in LMOOCs (Rahimi, Reference Rahimi2023), ChatGPT-assisted language learning (Rahimi et al., Reference Rahimi, Sheyhkholeslami and Mahmoudi Pour2025), and OLL (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Foster, Baffoe-Djan, Li and Yu2020) has been studied in applied linguistics, it still needs to be addressed in VE. Additionally, while recent studies in VE have extensively explored cultural competence and applied qualitative designs (O’Dowd, Reference O’Dowd2021b), we intend to take a step forward by using a multi-level analysis to validate the factorial structure of OL2MSS, L2 grit, and OSEL in VE, as well as to develop a conceptual model to show how these factors are serially related to the development of learners’ cognitive, metacognitive, and management strategies while exchanging language in VE.

4. Methodology

4.1 Study participants

The study involved 92 students majoring in English studies at the Universitat de València (UV) in Spain. During the fall semester of 2023, they exchanged cultural and historical information with Cypriot and Irish students from Cyprus University of Technology (CUT) and Limerick University (UL) in Ireland, respectively. Most respondents (83%) were female, followed by a smaller number of males (9.8%). There were 79 participants aged 18–22, seven aged 23–26, and six over 26. Table 1 summarizes the demographic information.

Table 1. Participants’ demographic information

4.2 Study procedure

The SOCIEMOVE project participants came from Cyprus, Ireland, and Spain. Throughout the fall semester of 2023, they collaborated in two hybrid tandem partnerships from which data were collected. The first partnership involved students from Cyprus and Spain, interacting in English as a lingua franca, while the second one involved Irish and Spanish learners participating in tandem (Spanish–English) exchanges. Participants were grouped to communicate synchronously and collaborate on the final project output. Moreover, all of them interacted asynchronously on Google Classroom in either English or Spanish through a common discussion forum. Together with lecturers from the other two participant institutions, the second author of this paper facilitated both partnerships and designed the tasks completed by participants.

Guth and Helm’s (Reference Guth and Helm2011) model was followed. Thus, three main VE stages were present, involving communication via a wide range of synchronous and asynchronous tools: learning management systems (Moodle), virtual learning environments (Google Classroom), 3D (Spatial) and 2D (Zoom) videoconferencing tools, instant messaging tools (WhatsApp), collaborative writing tools (Google Docs), and video editing tools (Movie Maker), among others. Several tasks were common to both partnerships, while the tasks connected to the final collaborative artifact differed, as they were adapted to each curriculum and needs: Business Management and Spanish Language and Culture for UL participants, History and Culture of English-Speaking Countries for UV participants, and Public Relations and English Language Communication for CUT participants.

As part of the “Breaking the Ice” stage between three universities, participants exchanged short self-introduction videos as well as ice-breaking questions and answers in the Google Classroom community in written, audio, or video forms. Different games (e.g. Kahoot!) and language activities were then played. Next, participants interviewed a female historical figure from the target country in the AI app Hello History and shared the results in a Google Docs summary. After locating additional information about their chosen historical figure, they compared the information gathered from multiple online and written sources and updated their summaries. In the second stage, learners completed comparison tasks regarding cultural, linguistic, and historical elements from their target country(ies) and culture(s). Then they moved on to the main collaborative task, which differed for each partnership, resulting in different final artifacts.

In the third and last stage, participants in the UV–CUT partnership created their ideal country and collaboratively wrote two scripts, with the first describing the features of the new country as objectively as possible, and the second communicating the advantages of the new country more persuasively. These scripts subsequently became the voice-over narration of (a) a digital story in the form of a fictitious history documentary that objectively described the main characteristics of the new, ideal country; and (b) a digital story in the form of an advertisement aimed at persuading the earth population to move into the new country. Students met synchronously in Spatial for the first and second time to organize their work, and then exchanged feedback and suggestions for improvement on the scripts, before submitting their final project artifacts.

As for the UV–UL partnership, the third and final stage involved creating digital stories using the information from their interviews with historical figures. Thus, three synchronous meetings were held on Spatial. A report followed, as well as different drafts of scripts about historical figures from English-speaking countries (UV) or Hispanic countries (UL), with opportunities to exchange feedback and suggestions for improvement. The final script became the voice-over narration, which was combined with images, text (e.g. subtitles and captions), videos, and music to create digital stories as their final artifacts.

Having created and shared these artifacts on Google Classroom, the final common task was to assess the work produced by the different groups by exchanging comments asynchronously on Google Classroom, as if reviewing a movie. Afterwards, each digital story was assessed individually with a rubric. A third and last synchronous meeting in Spatial followed to assess the project from their own perspectives as well as their partners’. The final step involved submitting their third report and answering the post-questionnaire (see supplementary material), which was rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree. The data were normally distributed between −2 and 2.

4.3 Data analysis

The partial least squares structural equation modeling approach (PLS-SEM) was applied for several reasons. First, several recent studies have widely applied qualitative designs, but VE literature requires more quantitative statistics (Rienties & Rets, Reference Rienties and Rets2022) with a higher level of accuracy and exploratory nature. Having grafted ordinary least squares with principal components analysis, the PLS-SEM minimizes the error terms and explains latent variables’ variance through the coefficient of determination (R 2), specializing this approach for theory development (Hair, Sarstedt, Ringle & Gudergan, Reference Hair, Sarstedt, Ringle and Gudergan2023), which is the main objective of this study. It also implements higher-order constructs, which simultaneously specify a construct on multiple levels of abstraction, making it a superior approach for mediation analysis (Sarstedt, Hair, Nitzl, Ringle & Howard, Reference Sarstedt, Hair, Nitzl, Ringle and Howard2020). As an additional advantage, it can simultaneously process reflective and formative evaluation (Hair et al., Reference Hair, Sarstedt, Ringle and Gudergan2023).

5. Results

5.1 Measurement model

An assessment of the variables’ indicator reliability, internal consistency reliability, convergent validity, and discriminant validity forms the basis of the measurement phase of PLS-SEM. In general, indicator loadings greater than 0.708 are acceptable, as this construct will explain over 50% of the indicator variance (Hair et al., Reference Hair, Sarstedt, Ringle and Gudergan2023). Cronbach’s alpha and composite reliability coefficients (rho_A) were applied in this case, and all three measures were significant. Average variance extracted (AVE) was applied to assess the convergent validity of the indicators. To accurately predict at least 50% of the variance associated with indicators, Hair et al. (Reference Hair, Sarstedt, Ringle and Gudergan2023) concluded that the AVE needs to be above 0.50. As illustrated in Table 2, the study items demonstrated greater convergent validity.

Table 2. The reliability and validity of the study variables

The measurement model should also assess discriminant validity. A heterotrait–monotrait ratio of correlations was used to evaluate discriminant validity, and the validity of the study variables was found to be below the criteria of 0.90 (Hair et al., Reference Hair, Sarstedt, Ringle and Gudergan2023), as shown in Table 3.

Table 3. Heterotrait–monotrait ratio – Matrix

5.2 Structural model

The structural model was evaluated through collinearity, path coefficients, significance levels, and predictive power, accuracy, and overall fit, and 5,000 subsamples were used to determine the robustness of the indicator loadings and the significance of the relationship between variables. To begin with, the variance inflation factor was used to measure collinearity, and all of them were below the cut-off threshold of 0.05 (Hair et al., Reference Hair, Sarstedt, Ringle and Gudergan2023). An analysis of path coefficient, which is shown in Table 4, presented that the paths DSA->CI ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.391$ ), DSA->PE ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.326$ ), IDL->CI ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.379$ ), IDL->PE ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.319$ ), IDL->CI ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.379$ ), CU->PE ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.192$ ), CI->HS ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.548$ ), CI->SE ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.490$ ), PE->GS ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.633$ ), and PE->SE ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.212$ ) were significant. As for the rest, their t value and p value were below 1.96 and higher than 0.05, respectively, indicating no significant correlation between the variables.

Table 4. Result of the bootstrap analysis

The direct bootstrap analysis with 5,000 subsamples was run to explore the direct effect size of OL2MSS on learners’ OSEL. As shown in Table 5, digital self-authenticity significantly predicted GS ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.228;{\rm{\;}}$ confidence interval (CI) [0.104, 0.362]), HS ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.254;{\rm{\;}}$ CI [0.148, 0.353]), SE ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.261;{\rm{\;}}$ CI [0.159, 0.374]). Moreover, ideal L2 self positively predicted learners’ goal-setting ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.223;{\rm{\;}}$ CI [0.086, 0.355]), help-seeking ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.247;{\rm{\;}}$ CI [0.112, 0.393]), and self-evaluation ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.254;{\rm{\;}}$ CI [0.125, 0.379]), while the remainder failed to directly predict learners’ OSEL factors.

Table 5. Result of the direct analysis

To answer the third research question, we ran an indirect bootstrap analysis with 5,000 subsamples to investigate the mediation role of L2 grit. Accordingly, the results showed that learners’ consistency of interest significantly mediated the correlation between IDL-SE ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.186;{\rm{\;CI\;}}$ [0.074, 0.300]), IDL-HS ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.208;{\rm{\;CI\;}}$ [0.077, 0.357]), DSA-SE ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.192;{\rm{\;CI\;}}$ [0.081, 0.312]), and DSA-HS ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.214;{\rm{\;t\;}}\,=\,3.23;{\rm{\;CI\;}}$ [0.107, 0.322]). Furthermore, the consistency of interest significantly mediated the correlation between CU-GS ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.122;{\rm{\;}}$ CI [0.004, 0.247]), IDL-HS ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.208;{\rm{\;CI\;}}$ [0.077, 0.357]), DSA-GS ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.206;{\rm{\;CI\;}}$ [0.091, 0.328]), IDL-HS ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.208;{\rm{\;CI\;}}$ [0.077, 0.357]), IDL-GS ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.206;{\rm{\;CI\;}}$ [0.091, 0.328]), IDL-HS ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.208;{\rm{\;CI\;}}$ [0.077, 0.357]), DSA-GS, ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.208;$ CI [0.077, 0.357]), DSA-SE ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.206;{\rm{\;CI\;}}$ [0.091, 0.328]), and IDL-HS ( ${\rm{\beta }}\,=\,0.202;{\rm{CI}}$ [0.081, 0.337]), as shown in Table 6.

Table 6. Indirect bootstrap analysis

The coefficient of determination (R 2) was then calculated to assess the robustness of exogenous variable prediction when shaping endogenous latent variables (Hair et al., Reference Hair, Sarstedt, Ringle and Gudergan2023). The R 2 should be evaluated as low, medium, and substantial based on the values of 0.19, 0.37, and .067 (Hair et al., Reference Hair, Sarstedt, Ringle and Gudergan2023). Additionally, Stone–Geisser (Q2) was used to assess the predictive relevance of each endogenous variable, which is beyond zero. Lastly, the standardized root-mean-square residual was below 0.08, indicating a good fit (Hair et al., Reference Hair, Sarstedt, Ringle and Gudergan2023). Table 7 summarizes the results of the structural model, while Figure 1 displays the study model with relative values.

Table 7. Result of the predictive power, accuracy, and model fit

Note. SRMR = standardized root-mean-square residual.

Figure 1. Structural model with relative values.

6. Discussion

6.1 Factorial structure of OL2MSS, online L2 grit, and OSEL in VE

In light of the measurement model results, OL2MSS, online L2 grit, and VESRL were validated in the context of VE. Accordingly, along with previous studies that validated L2MSS in traditional language learning contexts in China (You & Dörnyei, Reference You and Dörnyei2016), LMOOCs (Rahimi, Reference Rahimi2023; Rahimi & Cheraghi, Reference Rahimi and Cheraghi2022), ChatGPT-assisted language learning (Rahimi et al., Reference Rahimi, Sheyhkholeslami and Mahmoudi Pour2025), and OLL (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Foster, Baffoe-Djan, Li and Yu2020), we now validated it in VE, and in Spain. As far as the researchers are aware, L2 grit had not been validated in the VE context until now, although it had been validated in both online (Paradowski & Jelińska, Reference Paradowski and Jelińska2024) and traditional (Teimouri et al., Reference Teimouri, Plonsky and Tabandeh2022) language learning contexts. Likewise, the OSEL factors had only been previously validated in the context of LMOOCs (Rahimi & Cheraghi, Reference Rahimi and Cheraghi2022), ChatGPT-assisted language learning (Rahimi et al., Reference Rahimi, Sheyhkholeslami and Mahmoudi Pour2025), and OLL (Zheng et al., Reference Zheng, Liang, Li and Tsai2018). Thus, we extended their validity to another CALL context, and VE.

These findings support previously published research recommending the validation of OL2MSS, especially the two newly identified factors, digital self-authenticity (Rahimi, Reference Rahimi2023; Rahimi & Mosalli, Reference Rahimi and Mosalli2024; Rahimi et al., Reference Rahimi, Sheyhkholeslami and Mahmoudi Pour2025) and current L2 self (Rahimi & Mosalli, Reference Rahimi and Mosalli2024; Rahimi et al., Reference Rahimi, Sheyhkholeslami and Mahmoudi Pour2025), in another digital learning context. These discriminations between the factors highlighted that VE can cover language learners’ future self-image, current L2 needs, and the obligations they must fulfill to achieve their future self-image. Furthermore, the digital context validated was found to be much more authentic than traditional language learning contexts, as well as closer to both their academic and private lives. In addition, by validating three new trends in this context, we respond to the pressing need previously pointed out by leading CALL figures that research in VE should focus on new trends (Colpaert, Reference Colpaert2020; O’Dowd, Reference O’Dowd2021b).

6.2 Relationship between OL2MSS, online L2 grit, and OSEL

The result of the structural model showed that, among the OL2MSS factors, learners’ ideal L2 self significantly predicted their perseverance of effort (β = 379) and consistency of interest (β = 319). Their ought-to L2 self, however, did not contribute to learners’ L2 grit in the VE context. This finding might lie behind the fact that the ideal L2 self covers learners’ personal and future desirable self-image to exchange language in VE, and L2 grit is related to long-term goals rather than short-term ones (Duckworth et al., Reference Duckworth, Peterson, Matthews and Kelly2007), since in this study the ought-to L2 self represents learners’ short-term goals (e.g. to succeed in their university course).

This also reflects the dynamic features of OL2MSS, in line with You and Dörnyei’s (Reference You and Dörnyei2016) observation that Eastern language learners prefer to use their ought-to L2 self more than their ideal L2 self when the target language is directly related to their careers. In our VE, however, this was not the case, as Spanish language learners appear to have a higher likelihood of acquiring cultural competence and a social entrepreneurial mindset to achieve their ideal future (Sevilla-Pavón & Nicolaou, Reference Sevilla-Pavón and Nicolaou2022), with VE providing an appropriate venue. As previously mentioned, VE provides students with the opportunity to develop global language competence, and learners invest their time and effort in the learning context they are expected to be in (Csizér & Tankó, Reference Csizér and Tankó2017).

The ideal L2-self also predicted learners’ online self-regulation, including their goal setting (β = 223), help-seeking (β = 247), and self-evaluation (β = 254), meaning that a future-oriented learner was more likely to plan their own learning with specific objectives, ask for assistance, and conduct frequent self-assessments in VE. As per previous findings, learners with promotional motivation could regulate their learning (You & Dörnyei, Reference You and Dörnyei2016; Zheng et al., Reference Zheng, Liang, Li and Tsai2018). Elements such as the ideal future self of university learners to exchange or learn another language as a means for immigrants to enter the target country (Castañeda, Reference Castañeda2021), achieve better employment prospects (O’Dowd & Dooly, Reference O’Dowd and Dooly2022), acquire more opportunities for advancement at university (Sevilla-Pavón & Nicolaou, Reference Sevilla-Pavón and Nicolaou2022), or achieve a higher social status (O’Dowd & Dooly, Reference O’Dowd and Dooly2022) might relate to these findings.

Moreover, the current L2 self only predicted learners’ perseverance of effort (β = 192), while failing to predict consistency of interest and learners’ VESRL. Accordingly, the higher the learners’ assessment of their learning progress in VE, the more they preserve their effort to reach their objects in VE. Hence, we found discrepancies between learners’ ideal L2 self and current L2 self in shaping learners’ efforts in VE. However, Smith et al. (Reference Smith, Foster, Baffoe-Djan, Li and Yu2020) and Henry and Cliffordson (Reference Henry and Cliffordson2017) found no discrepancy between these two constructs in online and digital language learning in Chinese and Swedish contexts to shape learners’ intended efforts. The concept of “keep on chasing” (Thorsen et al., Reference Thorsen, Henry and Cliffordson2020) may explain such outcomes, as learners strive to achieve better results by evaluating their current language skills and competence. The communicative nature of VE offers continuous opportunities for self-evaluation, which in turn leads to dedicating more effort than mere interest. The notion of “upward revised possible selves” (Carroll et al., Reference Carroll, Agler and Newhart2015) also supports our findings, as learners who understand their core competences and progress are more likely to utilize upward revised current self systematically, which ultimately results in greater effort.

Notably, digital self-authenticity significantly predicted learners’ CI (β = 0.391) and PE (0.326), showing that learners had a high level of motivation to exchange language and information in VE in comparison to their previous language learning contexts, which led them to achieve their long-term goals by dedicating more effort and interest in this context. It also directly (see Table 6) predicted learners goal setting (β = 0.228), help-seeking (β = 0.254), and self-evaluation (β = 0.261) in VE. Hence, a higher level of motivation to use the language in VE, compared to previous learning contexts, would encourage learners to evaluate their progress, seek help from others, and set goals. Accordingly, we observed the sign of authenticity gap in the VE and Spanish EFL contexts. This way, the sign of authenticity gap, after being recently observed in OLL (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Foster, Baffoe-Djan, Li and Yu2020), ChatGPT-assisted language learning (Rahimi et al., Reference Rahimi, Sheyhkholeslami and Mahmoudi Pour2025), and LMOOCs (Rahimi, Reference Rahimi2023), is now reported in another CALL area. While previous studies found that digital self-authenticity impacted learners’ attitudes (Rahimi, Reference Rahimi2023) and OLL efforts (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Foster, Baffoe-Djan, Li and Yu2020), we now add to the CALL field evidence of the high level of shared variance of digital self-authenticity to shape language learners’ L2 grit and VESRL. The findings may be explained by various contextual factors related to VE. First, the multimodality and adaptability of VE (O’Dowd, Reference O’Dowd2021a), where learners access various VR, AR, and AI modules and adapt to these environments for learning.

The notion of the “life stage” (Rahimi, Reference Rahimi2023) also supports this finding, since learners can take advantage of older students’ experiences to reach their ideal future image, which is observed in the correlation between IDL and OSEL. This is also observable in VE, as professional peers with diverse cultures, experiences, and backgrounds relevant to the target context share their experiences with others – something that would be impossible in traditional language instruction. Furthermore, the VE context specializes in exchanging cultural competence in addition to performing tasks related to such exchange (O’Dowd & Dooly, Reference O’Dowd and Dooly2022; O’Dowd & Ritter, Reference O’Dowd and Ritter2006), which sets it apart from traditional learning contexts since, in those contexts, students follow teachers’ “mirror in-class” tasks, and try to emulate their pronunciation or adhere to a set of grammatical rules (Rahimi, Reference Rahimi2023). This finding and reports may be a valid response to Kurek (Reference Kurek2015) and Grau and Turula (Reference Grau and Turula2019), who found that the complex VE learning environments resulted in instability and unpredictability due to a variety of factors such as tasks, participants, and materials, all of which were signs of digital self-authenticity, as reported by Rahimi (Reference Rahimi2023). The variety of digital tasks, modules (AI, VR), and participants’ skills and experiences (life stage) can raise learners’ attitudes and motivation in digital language learning while decreasing their motivation in traditional language learning (Rahimi, Reference Rahimi2023; Rahimi & Mosalli, Reference Rahimi and Mosalli2024), which are present in VE, as mentioned above. Additionally, it has been reported that students from Western Europe (Henry & Cliffordson, Reference Henry and Cliffordson2017), Iran (Rahimi, Reference Rahimi2023; Rahimi & Mosalli, Reference Rahimi and Mosalli2024), and China (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Foster, Baffoe-Djan, Li and Yu2020) are more likely to dedicate more time and effort to unstructured learning environments. This was also evident in our study, where learners exhibited higher levels of online L2 grit and VESRL in VE.

6.3 The mediation role of L2 grit

A final analysis aimed to explore the mediation role of online L2 grit between learners’ OL2MSS and their VESRL. Based on the indirect bootstrap analysis, learners’ consistency of interest in exchanging information in VE mediated the relationship between their future L2 self and digital self-authenticity with their metacognitive (self-evaluation) and resource management (help-seeking) strategies in VE. Similarly, their perseverance of effort to exchange information in VE mediated the correlation between learners’ ideal L2 self and digital self-authenticity with their metacognitive strategies, particularly selecting their objects in VE. All these mediations were complementary, as direct and indirect bootstraps were along the same lines.

On the other hand, L2 grit acted as moderator variables in IDL->PE->SE; IDL->PE->HS; DSA->PE->HS; DSA->CI->GS; and DSA->PE->SE, as it negatively affected the direct correlation between learners’ ideal L2 self and self-evolution, help-seeking, as well as digital self-authenticity, with help-seeking, goal setting, and self-evaluation. More importantly, perseverance of effort acted as a moderator variable between current L2 self and learners’ goal setting since CU did not correlate with OSEL. Accordingly, the flipping effect occurred by PE in this correlation. Moreover, the suppression pheromone observed in this serial correlation as PE increased the productive power of CU (β = 0.118) to (β = 0.122).

As Duckworth et al. (Reference Duckworth, Peterson, Matthews and Kelly2007) points out, being gritty requires long-term dedication. It is likely that learners’ current language needs do not correspond to their future goals at the beginning of VE. However, with time and perseverance of effort, learners will achieve a high level of self-regulation, especially when it comes to setting goals in VE. This reminds us that internet-mediated intercultural exchange is both an extremely complex social activity and a highly challenging educational endeavor (Müller-Hartmann, Reference Müller-Hartmann2005). It should also be borne in mind that it can be unpredictable due to a variety of factors, such as participants’ psychological factors (Grau & Turula, Reference Grau and Turula2019; Kurek, Reference Kurek2015). This is demonstrated in our direct and indirect results, where the perseverance of effort and consistency of interest mediated learners’ motivation to self-regulate themselves in the VE. This serial mediation responds to the call to understand “the relations that connect the components of a complex system” (Hiver & Larsen-Freeman, Reference Hiver, Larsen-Freeman, Al-Hoorie and MacIntyre2020: 287, as cited in Paradowski & Jelińska, Reference Paradowski and Jelińska2024: 2344) in various language learning contexts, and we provided such understanding in a VE context.

7. Implications for theory and practice

Several aspects of this study contribute to the humanities literature in general, as well as language teaching and learning, CALL, VE, and psycholinguistics specifically. First, a psycholinguistic perspective was adopted instead of a sociolinguistic approach in the exam of VE success in CALL. We addressed recent VE figureheads’ requests to integrate new aspects and explore beyond ICC (Colpaert, Reference Colpaert2020; O’Dowd, Reference O’Dowd2021b) by incorporating new trends, such as OL2MSS, online grit, and VESRL. Our study also validated the factorial structures of OL2MSS, OSEL, and online L2 grit in the context of VE and added them to both the CALL and psycholinguistics literature. A sign of the authenticity gap was also discovered in VE, which had already been reported in previous CALL studies in OLL and LMOOCs. Furthermore, this study is among the first to apply an exploratory quantitative design and develop a conceptual framework specific to VE, as recent studies have relied heavily on qualitative approaches and reported the role of VE in shaping learners’ ICC.

Three levels of practical implications may be apparent from our study. First, educators should be aware of the role that both the ideal L2 self and the current L2 self play in shaping learners’ VESRL and L2 grit in VE. Therefore, language educators should place greater emphasis on developing learners’ ideal future selves rather than focusing on their past learning achievements. This can be done by encouraging students to evaluate their progress in VE while engaging in information exchange. Additionally, students should recognize the importance of their future goals and how these contribute to their OSEL and L2 grit, as opposed to focusing on fulfilling their obligations, since the ought-to L2 self failed to predict both OSEL and L2 grit in VE. Moreover, to help students become successful and gritty L2 learners, teachers should promote self-regulation and reflection on learners’ motivation.

Practitioners should also become aware of the signs of authenticity gap in VE in Spain and allocate more financial resources to its integration into their educational contexts, since learners are more motivated to learn in VE settings than in previous learning contexts, resulting in a higher level of both perseverance of effort and consistency of interest, which impacted their VESRL as well. Training opportunities should also be provided, focusing on the integration of VE in language learning settings, going beyond the class syllabus and the development of learners’ ICC. The focus should also be switched from merely passing the course to addressing learners’ current needs as well as their future personal and academic images when exchanging information.

At the final level, this study introduces a new conceptual framework to CALL scholars and provides a new avenue for validating, extending, and replicating the current model in other VE contexts. While researchers in other CALL areas, such as those focused on LMOOCs (Rahimi, Reference Rahimi2023), have already developed their own conceptual frameworks, our framework may serve as a starting point for VE researchers to develop a specific framework tailored to VE. In this way, VE researchers will be better equipped to conduct more in-depth analyses of variables and uncover correlations, mediations, or moderations that may not be identifiable through qualitative or basic quantitative research designs.

Future studies could address several limitations of this research. Given the exploratory nature of the current study, conducting multi-group analyses and applying more rigorous tests are recommended to validate the findings. Additionally, since data in this study were collected using self-reported Likert scales, future research could incorporate qualitative data from in-depth interviews or focus groups to provide a deeper understanding of the complex relationships between OL2MSS, L2 grit, and OSEL among students from diverse sociocultural backgrounds. Such research could also explore VE success from a psycholinguistic perspective.

We also recommend a more quantitative design for future VE research, specifically the by-symmetric research design recently introduced by Rahimi and Sevilla-Pavón (Reference Rahimi and Sevilla-Pavón2024a) and integrated into CALL. This approach allows researchers to explore direct correlations, mediation, and moderation symmetrically, through the average impact of each variable on one another (as in the current study), as well as asymmetrically, through necessary conditional analysis to examine combinations of independent variables that drive target outcomes.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material referred to in this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S095834402400034X

Data availability statement

Data available on request from the authors.

Acknowledgements

Acknowledgements are due to the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation for funding the research project SOCIEMOVE: Developing Socioemotional Skills Through Virtual Exchange (SOCIEMOVE: Desarrollo de habilidades socioemocionales en intercambios virtuales), Ref. PID2023-151087NB-I00, “Knowledge Generation Projects” call from the State Programme to Promote Scientific-Technical Research and its Transfer, within the framework of the State Plan for Scientific, Technical Research and Innovation 2021-2023, co-directed by Dr Ana Sevilla-Pavón and Dr Margarita Vinagre-Laranjeira from 01/09/2024 to 01/09/2027.

Authorship contribution statement

Amir Reza Rahimi: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Visualization, Validation, Software, Resources, Methodology, Investigation, Formal analysis, Data curation, Conceptualization; Ana Sevilla-Pavón: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Visualization, Data curation, Software, Resources, Investigation, Funding acquisition, Supervision.

Funding disclosure statement

This study is part of the research project financially supported by the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation: SOCIEMOVE: Developing Socioemotional Skills Through Virtual Exchange (SOCIEMOVE: Desarrollo de habilidades socioemocionales en intercambios virtuales), Ref. PID2023-151087NB-I00, “Knowledge Generation Projects” call from the State Programme to Promote Scientific-Technical Research and its Transfer, within the framework of the State Plan for Scientific, Technical Research and Innovation 2021-2023, co-directed by Dr Ana Sevilla-Pavón and Dr Margarita Vinagre-Laranjeira from 01/09/2024 to 01/09/2027.

Competing interests statement

The authors declare no competing interests.

Ethical statement

The procedures used in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committees of the Universitat de València, Spain, and the target study context.

GenAI use disclosure statement

The authors declare no use of generative AI.

About the authors

Amir Reza Rahimi is a PhD candidate at the Universitat de València, Spain (Programme of Language, Literature, and Culture). He has taught English at various institutions in Iran and participated in the FORTHEM and SOCIEMOVE research projects. His research interests include psycholinguistics, CALL, virtual exchange, online language learner motivation, and theory development in education.

Ana Sevilla-Pavón is an associate professor in the Department of English and German Philology at the Universitat de València, Spain. Dr Sevilla-Pavón has a PhD in English Philology (Universitat Politècnica de València) and is the leader of the SOCIEMOVE Spanish Ministry project. Her research interests include immersive/AI-assisted virtual exchange, 21st-century competences, intercultural communication, and English for specific purposes.

References

Barnard, L., Lan, W. Y., To, Y. M., Paton, V. O., & Lai, S.-L. (2009). Measuring self-regulation in online and blended learning environments. The Internet and Higher Education, 12(1): 16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.iheduc.2008.10.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baroni, A., Dooly, M., Garcés García, P., Guth, S., Hauck, M., Helm, F., Lewis, T., Mueller-Hartmann, A., O’Dowd, R., Rienties, B., & Rogaten, J. (2019). Evaluating the Iimpact of Virtual Exchange on Initial Teacher Education: A European Policy Experiment. https://Research-Publishing.net. https://doi.org/10.14705/rpnet.2019.29.9782490057337 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, P. J., Agler, R. A. & Newhart, D. W. (2015) Beyond cause to consequence: The road from possible to core self-revision. Self and Identity, 14(4): 482498. https://doi.org/10.1080/15298868.2015.1026385 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castañeda, D. A. (2021) Improving conversational interactions with task-based activities in a Spanish as a second language class. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 34(8): 11541181. https://doi.org/10.1080/09588221.2019.1666149 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colpaert, J. (2020) Editorial position paper: How virtual is your research? Computer Assisted Language Learning, 33(7): 653664. https://doi.org/10.1080/09588221.2020.1824059 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Credé, M., Tynan, M. C., & Harms, P. D. (2017). Much ado about grit: A meta-analytic synthesis of the grit literature. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113(3): 492511. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000102 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Csizér, K., & Tankó, G. (2017). English Majors’ Self-Regulatory Control Strategy Use in Academic Writing and its Relation to L2 Motivation. Applied Linguistics, amv033. https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/amv033 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dörnyei, Z. (2009) The psychology of second language acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z. & Ryan, S. (2015) The psychology of the language learner revisited. New York: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315779553 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duckworth, A. L., Peterson, C., Matthews, M. D. & Kelly, D. R. (2007) Grit: Perseverance and passion for long-term goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(6): 10871101. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.1087 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fathi, J. & Hejazi, S. Y. (2024) Ideal L2 self and foreign language achievement: The mediating roles of L2 grit and foreign language enjoyment. Current Psychology, 43(12): 1060610620. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-023-05187-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeborn, L., Andringa, S., Lunansky, G. & Rispens, J. (2023) Network analysis for modeling complex systems in SLA research. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 45(2): 526557. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263122000407 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuchs, K., Pösse, L., Bedenlier, S., Gläser-Zikuda, M., Kammerl, R., Kopp, B., Ziegler, A., & Händel, M. (2022). Preservice teachers’ online self-regulated learning: Does digital readiness matter? Education Sciences, 12(4): 272. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci12040272 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, R. C. (1985). Social Psychology and Second Language Learning: The Role of Attitudes and Motivation. London: Edward Arnold. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0083787 Google Scholar
Golonka, E. M., Bowles, A. R., Frank, V. M., Richardson, D. L. & Freynik, S. (2014) Technologies for foreign language learning: A review of technology types and their effectiveness. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 27(1): 70105. https://doi.org/10.1080/09588221.2012.700315 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grau, M. K. & Turula, A. (2019) Experiential learning of telecollaborative competences in pre-service teacher education. Language Learning & Technology, 23(3): 98115. http://hdl.handle.net/10125/44698 Google Scholar
Gu, M. (Michelle), & Cheung, D. S. (2016). Ideal L2 self, acculturation, and Chinese language learning among South Asian students in Hong Kong: A structural equation modelling analysis. System, 57: 1424. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2016.01.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guth, S., & Helm, F. (2011). Developing multiliteracies in ELT through telecollaboration. ELT Journal, 66(1): 4251. https://doi.org/10.1093/elt/ccr027 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hair, J. F. Jr, Sarstedt, M., Ringle, C. M. & Gudergan, S. P. (2023) Advanced issues in partial least squares structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks: SAGE Publications.Google Scholar
Henry, A. (2013). Digital games and ELT: Bridging the authenticity gap. In Ushioda, E., (ed.), International perspectives on motivation: Language learning and professional challenges. London: Palgrave Macmillan, 133155. https://doi.org/10.1057/9781137000873 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henry, A. & Cliffordson, C. (2017) The impact of out-of-school factors on motivation to learn English: Self-discrepancies, beliefs, and experiences of self-authenticity. Applied Linguistics, 38(5): 713736. https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/amv060 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hiver, P., & Larsen-Freeman, D. (2020). Motivation: It is a relational system. In Al-Hoorie, A. H. & MacIntyre, P. D. (eds.), Contemporary language motivation theory: 60 years since Gardner and Lambert (p. 1959). Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters, 285303.Google Scholar
Kormos, J., & Csizér, K. (2013). The interaction of motivation, self-regulatory strategies, and autonomous learning behavior in different learner groups. TESOL Quarterly, 48(2): 275299. https://doi.org/10.1002/tesq.129 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurek, M. (2015). Designing tasks for complex virtual learning environments. Bellaterra Journal of Teaching & Learning Language & Literature, 8(2): 1332. https://doi.org/10.5565/rev/jtl3.633 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamb, M. & Arisandy, F. E. (2020) The impact of online use of English on motivation to learn. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 33(1–2): 85108. https://doi.org/10.1080/09588221.2018.1545670 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, G. L., Zhang, Y. & Zhang, R. (2024) Examining the relationships among motivation, informal digital learning of English, and foreign language enjoyment: An explanatory mixed-method study. ReCALL, 36(1): 7288. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0958344023000204 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, M. (2024) Mapping the landscape of research on the L2 motivational self system theory (2005–2021): A bibliometric and text network analysis. System, 120: Article 103180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2023.103180 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luo, H. & Gui, M. (2021) Developing an effective Chinese-American telecollaborative learning program: An action research study. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 34(5–6): 609636. https://doi.org/10.1080/09588221.2019.1633355 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luo, H., & Yang, C. (2021). Pedagogical benefits of Chinese-American virtual exchange: A study of student perceptions. ReCALL, 34(1): 3750. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0958344021000203 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller-Hartmann, A. (2005) Learning how to teach intercultural communicative competence via telecollaboration: A model for language teacher education. In Belz, J. A. & Thorne, S. L. (eds.), Internet-mediated intercultural foreign language education. Boston: Heinle, 63–84. http://hdl.handle.net/10125/69620 Google Scholar
O’Dowd, R. (2021a) What do students learn in virtual exchange? A qualitative content analysis of learning outcomes across multiple exchanges. International Journal of Educational Research, 109, Article 101804. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2021.101804 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Dowd, R. (2021b) Virtual exchange: Moving forward into the next decade. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 34(3): 209224. https://doi.org/10.1080/09588221.2021.1902201 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Dowd, R. & Dooly, M. (2022) Exploring teachers’ professional development through participation in virtual exchange. ReCALL, 34(1): 2136. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0958344021000215 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Dowd, R. & Ritter, M. (2006) Understanding and working with ‘failed communication’ in telecollaborative exchanges. CALICO Journal, 23(3): 623642. http://www.jstor.org/stable/24156364 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oxford, R. L. (2016). Powerfully positive: Searching for a model of language learner well-being. In Gabryś-Barker, D. & Gałajda, D., (eds.), Positive Psychology Perspectives on Foreign Language Learning and Teaching. New York: Springer, 2137. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-32954-3_2 Google Scholar
Paradowski, M. B. & Jelińska, M. (2024) The predictors of L2 grit and their complex interactions in online foreign language learning: Motivation, self-directed learning, autonomy, curiosity, and language mindsets. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 37(8): 23202358. https://doi.org/10.1080/09588221.2023.2192762 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rahimi, A. R. (2023) The role of EFL learners’ L2 self-identities, and authenticity gap on their intention to continue LMOOCs: Insights from an exploratory partial least approach. Computer Assisted Language Learning. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1080/09588221.2023.2202215 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rahimi, A. R. & Cheraghi, Z. (2022) Unifying EFL learners’ online self-regulation and online motivational self-system in MOOCs: A structural equation modeling approach. Journal of Computers in Education, 11(1): 127. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40692-022-00245-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rahimi, A. R. & Mosalli, Z. (2024) Exploring the direct and indirect effects of EFL learners’ online motivational self-system on their online language learning acceptance: The new roles of current L2 self and digital self-authenticity. Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education, 9: Article 49. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40862-024-00266-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rahimi, A. R. & Sevilla-Pavón, A. (2024a) The role of ChatGPT readiness in shaping language teachers’ language teaching innovation and meeting accountability: A bisymmetric approach. Computers and Education: Artificial Intelligence, 7: Article 100258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.caeai.2024.100258 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rahimi, A. R. & Sevilla-Pavón, A. (2024b) A pathway from surface to deep online language learning approach: The crucial role of online self-regulation. Acta Psychologica, 251: Article 104644. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2024.104644 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rahimi, A. R., Sheyhkholeslami, M. & Mahmoudi Pour, A. (2025) Uncovering personalized L2 motivation and self-regulation in ChatGPT-assisted language learning: A hybrid PLS-SEM-ANN approach. Computers in Human Behavior Reports, 17: Article 100539. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chbr.2024.100539 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rienties, B., & Rets, I. (2022). The impact of virtual exchange on East–West social relations: Lessons from a China-Portugal foreign language exchange. Journal of China Computer-Assisted Language Learning, 2(1): 119. https://doi.org/10.1515/jccall-2021-0003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sarstedt, M., Hair, J. F. Jr, Nitzl, C., Ringle, C. M. & Howard, M. C. (2020) Beyond a tandem analysis of SEM and PROCESS: Use of PLS-SEM for mediation analyses! International Journal of Market Research, 62(3): 288299. https://doi.org/10.1177/1470785320915686 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sevilla-Pavón, A. & Nicolaou, A. (2022) Artefact co-construction in virtual exchange: ‘Youth Entrepreneurship for Society.’ Computer Assisted Language Learning, 35(7): 16421667. https://doi.org/10.1080/09588221.2020.1825096 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, S. A., Foster, M. E., Baffoe-Djan, J. B., Li, Z. & Yu, S. (2020) Unifying the current self, ideal self, attributions, self-authenticity, and intended effort: A partial replication study among Chinese university English learners. System, 95: Article 102377. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2020.102377 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teimouri, Y., Plonsky, L. & Tabandeh, F. (2022) L2 grit: Passion and perseverance for second-language learning. Language Teaching Research, 26(5): 893918. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168820921895 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, A. S., & Liu, Y. (2018). Multilingualism and emergent selves: Context, languages, and the anti-ought-to self. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 24(2): 173190. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2018.1452892 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorsen, C., Henry, A. & Cliffordson, C. (2020) The case of a missing person? The current L2 self and the L2 motivational self system. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 23(5): 584600. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2017.1388356 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
You, C. (J.) & Dörnyei, Z. (2016) Language learning motivation in China: Results of a large-scale stratified survey. Applied Linguistics, 37(4): 495519. https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/amu046 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yousefi, M., & Mahmoodi, M. H. (2022). The L2 motivational self‐system: A meta‐analysis approach. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 32(2): 274294. https://doi.org/10.1111/ijal.12416 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yu, X. & Ma, J. (2024) Modelling the predictive effect of enjoyment on willingness to communicate in a foreign language: The chain mediating role of growth mindset and grit. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1080/01434632.2023.2300351 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zarrinabadi, N., Rezazadeh, M. & Mohammadzadeh Mohammadabadi, A. (2024) L2 grit and language mindsets as predictors of EFL learners’ attitudes toward effectiveness and value of CALL. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 37(7): 17261743. https://doi.org/10.1080/09588221.2022.2108061 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, J. (2023) Modelling the interplay of writing achievement goals and grit in predicting L2 writing achievements. System, 117: Article 103118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2023.103118 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, Y. (2024) The influence of practice contexts on L2 learners’ compliment responses: Telecollaborative video-based practice versus face-to-face practice. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 37(7): 21742201. https://doi.org/10.1080/09588221.2022.2152054 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zheng, C., Liang, J.-C., Chai, C. S., Chen, X. & Liu, H. (2023) Comparing high school students’ online self-regulation and engagement in English language learning. System, 115: Article 103037. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2023.103037 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zheng, C., Liang, J.-C., Li, M. & Tsai, C.-C. (2018) The relationship between English language learners’ motivation and online self-regulation: A structural equation modelling approach. System, 76: 144157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2018.05.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimmerman, B. J. (2000). Attaining self-regulation: A social cognitive perspective. In Boekaerts, M., Pintrich, P. R. & Zeidner, M. (eds.), Handbook of self-regulation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, 1339. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012109890-2/50031-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimmerman, B. J. (2001). Theories of self-regulated learning and academic achievement: An overview and analysis. In Zimmerman, B. J. & Schunk, D. H. (eds.), Self-Regulated learning and academic achievement: Theoretical perspectives (2nd ed.). New York: Springer-Verlag, 1–25.Google Scholar
Zimmerman, B. J., & Kitsantas, A. (2014). Comparing students’ self-discipline and self-regulation measures and their prediction of academic achievement. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 39(2): 145155. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2014.03.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Participants’ demographic information

Figure 1

Table 2. The reliability and validity of the study variables

Figure 2

Table 3. Heterotrait–monotrait ratio – Matrix

Figure 3

Table 4. Result of the bootstrap analysis

Figure 4

Table 5. Result of the direct analysis

Figure 5

Table 6. Indirect bootstrap analysis

Figure 6

Table 7. Result of the predictive power, accuracy, and model fit

Figure 7

Figure 1. Structural model with relative values.

Supplementary material: File

Rahimi and Sevilla-Pavón supplementary material

Rahimi and Sevilla-Pavón supplementary material
Download Rahimi and Sevilla-Pavón supplementary material(File)
File 22.7 KB