Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T11:12:42.718Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Affective Priming with Pictures of Emotional Scenes: The Role of Perceptual Similarity and Category Relatedness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 April 2014

Pedro Avero*
Affiliation:
Universidad de La Laguna
Manuel G. Calvo
Affiliation:
Universidad de La Laguna
*
Address correspondence to: Pedro Avero, Departamento de Personalidad, Evaluación y Tratamientos Psicológicos, Universidad de La Laguna, 38205 Tenerife (Spain). Fax: + 34 922 317 461. E-mail: paverode@ull.es

Abstract

Prime pictures portraying pleasant or unpleasant scenes were briefly presented (150-ms display; SOAs of 300 or 800 ms), followed by probe pictures either congruent or incongruent in emotional valence. In an evaluative decision task, participants responded whether the probe was emotionally positive or negative. Affective priming was reflected in shorter response latencies for congruent than for incongruent prime-probe pairs. Although this effect was enhanced by perceptual similarity between the prime and the probe, it also occurred for probes that were physically different, and the effect generalized across semantic categories (animals vs. people). It is concluded that affective priming is a genuine phenomenon, in that it occurs as a function of stimulus emotional content, in the absence of both perceptual similarity and semantic category relatedness between the prime and the probe.

En un paradigma de activación o priming afectivo, se presentaron pares de fotografías: primero una como activadora (prime), brevemente (exposición de 150 ms; SOAs de 300 u 800 ms), y a continuación otra como prueba (probe). Las dos fotografías podían ser congruentes o incongruentes en valencia emocional (agradables vs. desagradables). Los participantes tenían que responder si la foto de prueba era emocionalmente positiva o negativa. Se observó un efecto de priming afectivo al producirse latencias de respuesta más cortas cuando la foto de prueba era congruente en valencia con la foto activadora que cuando eran incongruentes. Aunque este efecto se incrementó en función de la semejanza física entre los estímulos de cada par, ocurrió también cuando éstos eran físicamente diferentes. Además, dicho efecto se generalizó a categorías semánticas distintas (escenas de animales vs. de personas). Se concluye que el fenómeno de procesamiento afectivo temprano es genuino, que ocurre en función del contenido emocional de los estímulos, y en ausencia de semejanza perceptual y de relación semántica entre el estímulo activador y el de prueba.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Banse, R. (2001). Affective priming with liked and disliked persons: Prime visibility determines congruency and incongruency effects. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 501520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bargh, J.A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. In Wyer, R.S. & Srull, T.K. (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition: Vol. 1. Basic processes (pp. 140). Hillsdale, NJ: Erbaum.Google Scholar
Bargh, J.A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life. In Wyer, R.S. (Ed.), The automaticity of everyday life: Advances in social cognition, Vol. 10 (pp. 161). Mahwah, NJ: Erbaum.Google Scholar
Carroll, N.C., & Young, A.W. (2005). Priming of emotion recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 11731197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cave, K., & Batty, M. (in press). From searching for features to searching for threat: Drawing the boundary between preattentive and attentive vision. Visual Cognition.Google Scholar
Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention [CSEA-NIMH] (1999). The International Affective Picture System: Digitized photographs. Gainesville, FL: The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.Google Scholar
Fazio, R.H. (2001). On the activation of associated evaluations: An overview. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 115141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giner-Sorolla, R., García, M.T., & Bargh, J.A. (1999). The automatic evaluation of pictures. Social Cognition, 17, 7696.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hermans, D., Baeyens, F., Lamote, S., Spruyt, A., & Eelen, P. (2005). Affective priming as an indirect measure of food preferences acquired through odor conditioning. Experimental Psychology, 52, 180186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermans, D., De Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (1994). The affective priming effect: Automatic activation of evaluative information in memory. Cognition and Emotion, 8, 515533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hermans, D., De Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (2001). A time course analysis of the affective priming effect. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 143165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hermans, D., & Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (2003). Affective priming with subliminally presented pictures. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, 97114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermans, D., Spruyt, A., & Eelen, P. (2003). Automatic affective priming of recently acquired stimulus valence: Priming at SOA 300 but not at SOA 1000. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 8399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hermans, D., Vansteenwegen, D., Crombez, G., Baeyens, F., & Eelen, P. (2002). Expectancy learning and evaluative learning in human classical conditioning: Affective priming as an indirect and unobtrusive measure of conditioned stimulus valence. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 217234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hietanen, J.K., & Korpela, K.M. (2004). Do both negative and positive environmental scenes elicit rapid affective processing? Environment and Behavior, 36, 558577.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klauer, K.C., & Musch, J. (2003). Affective priming. Findings and theories. In Musch, J. & Klauer, K.C. (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 749). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Korpela, K.M., Klemettilä, T., & Hietanen, J.K. (2002). Evidence for rapid affective evaluation of environmental scenes. Environment and Behavior, 34, 634650.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moltó, J., Montañés, S., Poy, R., Segarra, P., Pastor, M. C., Tormo, M.P., Ramírez, I., Hernández, M.A., Sánchez, M., Fernández, M.C., & Vila, J. (1999). Un nuevo método para el estudio experimental de las emociones: El International Affective Picture System (IAPS). Adaptación española. Revista de Psicología General y Aplicada, 52, 5587.Google Scholar
Neely, J.H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. In Besner, D. & Humphreys, G.W. (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 264336). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 372422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime user's guide. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools, Inc.Google Scholar
Spruyt, A., Hermans, D., De Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (2002). On the nature of the affective priming effect: Affective priming of naming responses. Social Cognition, 20, 227256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spruyt, A., Hermans, D., De Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (2004). Automatic non-associative semantic priming: Episodic affective priming of naming responses. Acta Psychologica, 116, 3954.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Storbeck, J., & Robinson, M.D. (2004). Preferences and inferences in encoding of visual objects: A systematic comparison of semantic and affective priming. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 8193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Storbeck, J., Robinson, M.D., & McCourt, M.E. (in press). Semantic processing precedes affect retrieval: The neurological case for cognitive primacy in visual processing. Review of General Psychology.Google Scholar
Vila, J., Sánchez, M., Ramírez, I., Fernández, M.C., Cobos, P., Rodríguez, S., Muñoz, M.A., Tormo, M.P., Herrero, M., Segarra, P., Pastor, M.C., Montañés, S., Poy, R., & Moltó, J (2001). El Sistema Internacional de Imágenes Afectivas (IAPS): Adaptación española. Segunda parte. Revista de Psicología General y Aplicada, 54, 635657.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. (2000). Feeling and thinking: Closing the debate over the independence of affect. In Forgas, J.P. (Ed.), Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social cognition. Studies in emotion and social interaction, Vol. 2. (pp. 3158). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar