Hostname: page-component-6bf8c574d5-mggfc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-26T00:15:30.906Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Dark Side of the Self: When Family is Highly Related to Mental Health Deterioration

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 February 2025

Merjema Ertema
Affiliation:
Universidad de Valencia, Spain
Juan Carlos Sanchez-Sosa
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Nuevo León, Mexico
Oscar F. Garcia*
Affiliation:
Universidad de Valencia, Spain
Maria Elena Villarreal-Gonzalez
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Nuevo León, Mexico
Fernando Garcia
Affiliation:
Universidad de Valencia, Spain
*
Corresponding author: Oscar F. Garcia; Email: oscar.f.garcia@uv.es
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This study aims to empirically test whether family has a unique significance for the self that cannot be captured by the social self alone. Specifically, it examines whether family self-concept, compared to social self-concept, is more closely related to family-specific indicators (i.e., parent–child communication and family functioning) as well as to indicators of emotional maladjustment like mental health deterioration (i.e., psychological distress and depressive symptoms). The sample comprised 4,953 Mexican adolescents, including 2,551 men (51.5%) and 2,402 women, aged 14–17 years (M = 15.60, SD = 0.92). Confirmatory factor analysis was applied to evaluate the proposed big five-dimensional self-concept model. Cohen’s d confidence intervals, derived from the shared variance of Pearson’s r correlations, were analyzed to relate self-concept dimensions to parent–child communication, family functioning, and mental health deterioration. Results from factorial confirmatory analysis showed that the five-dimensional oblique model (i.e., academic, social, emotional, physical, and family, as different from social) provided a better fit than competing unidimensional and orthogonal models. Correlation analyses showed that family self-concept was significantly associated with both parent–child communication and family functioning, as well as with psychological distress (d = −1.10, confidence interval [CI] −1.21 to −1.02) and depressive symptoms (d = −1.24, CI −1.31 to −1.22). These findings add evidence that family is not accurately represented within the social self-concept. Furthermore, perceiving oneself as unloved and undervalued at home (i.e., low family self-concept) is strongly associated not only with dysfunctional family processes but also with mental health deterioration.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de la Psicología de Madrid

Introduction

Self-concept multidimensional model

Although during decades the individual (i.e., the self) has been studied from different psychological perspectives and terms (i.e., self-awareness, self-construal, self-appraisal, self-esteem, self-concept), including evaluative and descriptive components (Baumeister, Reference Baumeister, Baumeister and Finkel2010; Marsh & Martin, Reference Marsh and Martin2011), it is possible that the greatest advance for psychology as a science in this convoluted research field was to conceptualize the self with an empirically contrastable model. In the 1970s, a theoretical model of self-concept that could empirically test deep theoretical problems was defined (Shavelson et al., Reference Shavelson, Hubner and Stanton1976).

The model proposes that a general (i.e., unidimensional) assessment of the self is situated in the apex and is divided into four domains: the academic, social, emotional, and physical self-concepts (i.e., the classical four-dimensional model). These domains are further split into sub-areas that become more correlated to actual behavior as one moves further down the hierarchy. For example, the physical self-concept domain is split in physical appearance and physical ability sub-areas, assuming that one and the other represent common parts of the physical self and that they are closely related. Although the model had a theoretical dilemma questioning whether it was orthogonal (i.e., independent dimensions) or oblique (i.e., related dimensions), the oblique model received more support (Byrne & Shavelson, Reference Byrne and Shavelson1996; Shavelson et al., Reference Shavelson, Hubner and Stanton1976). On the one hand, the specificity principle states that each domain of the self-concept will be related to the specific behaviors it represents to a greater degree than the other domains (Martín-Albo et al., Reference Martín-Albo, Núñez, Navarro and Grijalvo2007; Prentice & Miller, Reference Prentice and Miller1992). For example, academic self-concept is more related to academic achievement than the other dimensions (e.g., emotional) or the general self-concept (Byrne, Reference Byrne1984; Marsh et al., Reference Marsh, Byrne and Shavelson1988; Marsh & Craven, Reference Marsh and Craven2006). On the other hand, because of the obliquity of the model, the other dimensions will also be related to those specific behaviors, although to a lesser extent (Brunner et al., Reference Brunner, Keller, Dierendonck, Reichert, Ugen, Fischbach and Martin2010; Marsh & Craven, Reference Marsh and Craven2006).

Even though the classical model (Shavelson et al., Reference Shavelson, Hubner and Stanton1976) is the blueprint for upcoming self-concept studies, there is still debate about the number of dimensions of self-concept (Brown & Alexander, Reference Brown and Alexander1991; Coopersmith, Reference Coopersmith1967; Fitts, Reference Fitts1965; Harter, Reference Harter, Yawkey and Johnson1988; Piers & Harris, Reference Piers and Harris1964). This study focuses on an important limitation of the classical big four-dimensional model is that it has not considered the family dimension as the big fifth dimension, differentiable from the social part. In the classical model, the family (i.e., assuming they are significant others) has been considered together with peers as sub-areas within the social dimension (Byrne & Shavelson, Reference Byrne and Shavelson1996; Shavelson et al., Reference Shavelson, Hubner and Stanton1976). However, family could be more than a subarea of the social dimension (Garcia & Musitu, Reference Garcia and Musitu1999; Marsh & O’Neill, Reference Marsh and O’Neill1984); it may be that the family plays a specific significance in the self, differentiable from the social part.

Family and the self

Society is the reflection of the family, a basic and primary psychosocial agent. The family (parents or primary caretaker) is the main agent in charge of children’s development by teaching attitudes, values, and motives so that they can be functional members of the social community (Climent-Galarza et al., Reference Climent-Galarza, Alcaide, Garcia, Chen and Garcia2022; Gracia et al., Reference Gracia, Garcia and Musitu1995; Grusec, Reference Grusec2002). Therefore, the family is responsible for the child to acquire a sense of self and learn to function autonomously in a social community that has its constraints and rules (Maccoby & Martin, Reference Maccoby, Martin and Mussen1983; Martinez-Escudero et al., Reference Martinez-Escudero, Villarejo, Garcia and Garcia2020; Steinberg, Reference Steinberg2001; Villarejo et al., Reference Villarejo, Martinez-Escudero and Garcia2020). Experiences in the family context could affect how adolescents perceive and evaluate themselves (i.e., self-concept). Self-concept is constructed through the interpretation of experiences in significant environments, such as the family (Cooley, Reference Cooley1902; Krauss et al., Reference Krauss, Orth and Robins2020). Adolescence is a decisive period for the development of the self in which independence is sought, and important social, academic, emotional, and physical challenges are faced (Fuentes et al., Reference Fuentes, Alarcón, Gracia and Garcia2015; Reyes et al., Reference Reyes, Garcia, Perez-Gramaje, Serra, Melendez, Alcaide and Garcia2023; Steinberg & Morris, Reference Steinberg and Morris2001). Parents tend to have a lesser influence than peers (Martin-Blesa et al., Reference Martin-Blesa, Garcia, Alcaide and Garcia2024; Veiga et al., Reference Veiga, Festas, Garcia, Oliveira, Veiga, Martins, Covas and Carvalho2023; Villarejo et al., Reference Villarejo, Garcia, Alcaide, Gonzalez and Garcia2024), while demands and obligations are greater (e.g., in high school), especially in middle and late adolescence, and sometimes adolescents might report certain self-concept and mental health problems (Fall & Roberts, Reference Fall and Roberts2012; Young et al., Reference Young, Sandman and Craske2019; Zapf et al., Reference Zapf, Boettcher, Haukeland, Orm, Coslar and Fjermestad2024). Family processes, particularly parent–child communication and family functioning can either facilitate or undermine the child’s development (Cummings et al., Reference Cummings, Davies and Campbell2000; Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Serra, Garcia, Martinez and Cruise2019; Olson et al., Reference Olson, Waldvogel and Schlieff2019).

Parent–child communication is probably one of the most important processes that take place in the family for children to develop their individuality in the social community (Darling & Steinberg, Reference Darling and Steinberg1993; Martinez et al., Reference Martinez, Cruise, Garcia and Murgui2017; Martinez-Escudero et al., Reference Martinez-Escudero, Garcia, Alcaide, Bochons and Garcia2023). Responsive parents have open communication with children in an emotional climate based on care, support, love, and acceptance to their children, regardless of their child’s behavior (Gimenez-Serrano et al., Reference Gimenez-Serrano, Garcia and Garcia2022; Grusec & Lytton, Reference Grusec, Lytton, Grusec and Lytton1988; Martinez et al., Reference Martinez, Garcia, Musitu and Yubero2012). These parents communicate confidence and security to their children by fostering the belief that the child will succeed and is a valuable individual with positive qualities (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Garcia, Alcaide, Garcia-Ros and Garcia2024; Darling & Steinberg, Reference Darling and Steinberg1993). But equally important is for children to openly communicate with their parents, tell their problems and worries, in a spontaneous way, without fear that they will get angry with them or that their parents will reject them (Keijsers & Poulin, Reference Keijsers and Poulin2013; Kerr et al., Reference Kerr, Stattin and Trost1999). Overall, previous research has noted that adolescents tend to disclose more to their mothers than to their fathers (Keijsers et al., Reference Keijsers, Branje, VanderValk and Meeus2010; Smetana et al., Reference Smetana, Metzger, Gettman and Campione-Barr2006; Waizenhofer et al., Reference Waizenhofer, Buchanan and Jackson-Newsom2004)

However, family functioning does not only depend on the parent–child relationship. Good family functioning is based on cohesion, characterized by connectedness, care, and support, which enhances healthy interactions among all family members (Kapetanovic & Skoog, Reference Kapetanovic and Skoog2021; Olson et al., Reference Olson, Sprenkle and Russell1979; Segrin & Flora, Reference Segrin and Flora2018). For example, marital relations might positively or negatively affect family functioning (Kapetanovic & Skoog, Reference Kapetanovic and Skoog2021; Koerner & Fitzpatrick, Reference Koerner and Fitzpatrick2002). A good relationship between the spouses is associated with child involvement, which, in turn, positively contributes to the family functioning (Grusec & Lytton, Reference Grusec, Lytton, Grusec and Lytton1988; Knopp et al., Reference Knopp, Rhoades, Allen, Parsons, Ritchie, Markman and Stanley2017; Koerner & Fitzpatrick, Reference Koerner and Fitzpatrick2002).

The dark side of family

Unfortunately, not all family processes have a positive influence on the child’s development. Some family processes are dysfunctional, that is, they are characterized by rejection, lack of love, and worthlessness (Alcaide et al., Reference Alcaide, Garcia, Queiroz and Garcia2023; Lila et al., Reference Lila, Garcia and Gracia2007; Palacios et al., Reference Palacios, Garcia, Alcaide and Garcia2022; Steinberg et al., Reference Steinberg, Lamborn, Darling, Mounts and Dornbusch1994). Dysfunctional family processes have been related to serious disturbances in the self (Lamborn et al., Reference Lamborn, Mounts, Steinberg and Dornbusch1991; Van Dijk et al., Reference Van Dijk, Branje, Keijsers, Hawk, Hale and Meeus2014), probably due to difficulties in the emotion regulation (Baumrind, Reference Baumrind1968; Darling & Steinberg, Reference Darling and Steinberg1993; Kganyago, Reference Kganyago and Silva2023; Sroufe, Reference Sroufe1996). Compared to young adulthood associated with more maturity and less risk taking, adolescents face new demands and requirements (e.g., higher school expectations) that will define their self-concept so that conflicts in the family (e.g., communication problems) sometimes represent a source of discomfort and external conflict that could become internalized and seriously deter their adaptation (Fall & Roberts, Reference Fall and Roberts2012; Mañez et al., Reference Mañez, Skrobiszewska, Descals, Cantero, Cerdan, Garcia and Garcia-Ros2024; Steinberg, Reference Steinberg2007; Zapf et al., Reference Zapf, Boettcher, Haukeland, Orm, Coslar and Fjermestad2024). A key component of children’s successful development is to learn to regulate their emotions and related behaviors in socially appropriate ways (i.e., emotional self-concept) (Darling & Steinberg, Reference Darling and Steinberg1993; Grusec, Reference Grusec2002). A deterioration in emotion regulation has been linked to depression, anxiety, psychosis, and suicidal ideation, even in community samples (Gross & Muñoz, Reference Gross and Muñoz1995; Young et al., Reference Young, Sandman and Craske2019; Zapf et al., Reference Zapf, Boettcher, Haukeland, Orm, Coslar and Fjermestad2024).

Although previous research has identified the relationship between family processes and variations in mental health problems (Kapetanovic & Skoog, Reference Kapetanovic and Skoog2021; Zapf et al., Reference Zapf, Boettcher, Haukeland, Orm, Coslar and Fjermestad2024), the relationship between self-concept with family processes and mental health problems remains less established. Specifically, the family self-concept dimension may be particularly connected not only to family processes (Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Alcaide, Musitu-Ferrer, Pons-Benavent and Garcia2024; Lamborn et al., Reference Lamborn, Mounts, Steinberg and Dornbusch1991) but also to mental health deterioration (Cornella-Font et al., Reference Cornella-Font, Vinas-Poch, Juarez-Lopez and Malo-Cerrato2020; Fuentes et al., Reference Fuentes, Garcia and Garcia2020; Garaigordobil et al., Reference Garaigordobil, Ignacio Perez and Mozaz2008). However, in the classical theoretical model of self-concept (i.e., the big four dimensions), the family component is subsumed under the social dimension (Byrne & Shavelson, Reference Byrne and Shavelson1996; Shavelson et al., Reference Shavelson, Hubner and Stanton1976). This consolidation may obscure key aspects that are unique to the family dimension and not fully captured by the social dimension. In line with the idea that social self-concept might adequately represent family self-concept, previous research has posited that family interactions help to shape the child’s ability to form relationships outside the home (Fraley & Roisman, Reference Fraley and Roisman2019; Krauss et al., Reference Krauss, Orth and Robins2020). This could partly explain why the capability to relate to others and form friendships (i.e., social self-concept) has frequently been used as a key dimension representing family processes (Archuleta et al., Reference Archuleta, Kaminski and Ross2024; Chansky & Kendall, Reference Chansky and Kendall1997; Connolly, Reference Connolly1989; Jhang, Reference Jhang2017; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Wu, Gu, Cui and Wang2024).

However, few efforts have been made to empirically test whether the social component of the self-concept is equally related to family processes—implying that the family might be just a part of the social self-concept—or if, conversely, the family dimension is more closely linked to family processes than the social dimension. In addition, there has been limited exploration of the relationship between mental health impairments such as psychological distress and depressive symptoms and different parts of the self-concept. Previous research has suggested that emotional regulation might be particularly affected in mental health issues (Gross & Muñoz, Reference Gross and Muñoz1995; Young et al., Reference Young, Sandman and Craske2019; Zapf et al., Reference Zapf, Boettcher, Haukeland, Orm, Coslar and Fjermestad2024), indicating that the emotional self-concept could be significantly impaired. However, all aspects of the self may be affected by mental health impairments, albeit to varying degrees that need to be quantified. As suggested by earlier studies, the family self-concept might also relate to mental health impairment, though its connection to mental health problems should be compared with the emotional dimension—which may be particularly affected—or the social dimension, which might not be adequately represented.

The present study

The present study aims to determine whether the family component of self-concept is inadequately represented by the social dimension. To address this question, a two-step approach was adopted. The first step involved empirically testing the structure of self-concept by incorporating family self-concept into the classical four-dimensional model, which includes academic, social, emotional, and physical dimensions. This was accomplished using confirmatory factor analysis and tests of invariance. The study examined the fit of three competing theoretical models: a unidimensional model, a multidimensional oblique model that includes academic, social, emotional, family, and physical dimensions, and a multidimensional orthogonal model. It was hypothesized that the multidimensional oblique model, which assumes correlated dimensions, would provide a better fit to the data compared to the unidimensional model and the multidimensional orthogonal model. In addition, it was expected that the multidimensional oblique model would show invariance, ensuring that the five related dimensions of the self-concept structure would remain consistent across different groups defined by sex and age.

The second step involved analyzing the relationship between each dimension of self-concept and various indicators, both family-specific and non-family-specific. To distinguish between levels of relationships, effect sizes and their confidence intervals were used. It was tested whether family self-concept would be most strongly related to parent–child communication for both mothers and fathers—captured through open communication, communication problems, and avoidant communication—as well as to family functioning, since these criteria are specific to the family. Furthermore, the study examined whether the emotional self-concept is most strongly related to mental health deterioration, captured by psychological distress and depressive symptoms, justifying this analysis because these criteria are emotional-specific. It was also hypothesized that, given the family’s crucial role in psychosocial development, family self-concept would be highly related to mental health deterioration to a degree similar to that of emotional self-concept. This hypothesis would be supported by a large effect size with overlapping confidence intervals, despite the fact that mental health deterioration is not strictly a family-specific indicator.

Methods

Participants and Procedure

The participants in this study were 4,953 Mexican adolescents from 9th to 12th grade, aged between 14 and 17 years (M = 15.60, SD = 0.92). Of these, 2,551 (51.5%) were men and 2,402 were women. Although different criteria exist for defining adolescent age groups, this study focused on middle and late adolescence, as defined in previous research (Rubach et al., Reference Rubach, Dicke, Lazarides, Simpkins and Eccles2020; van der Wal et al., Reference van der Wal, Valkenburg and van Driel2024). Two age groups were established: middle adolescence, comprising 2,395 participants aged 14 and 15 years (i.e., 9th and 10th grades), and late adolescence, comprising 2,558 participants aged 16 and 17 years (i.e., 11th and 12th grades).

An a priori power analysis was conducted to determine the sample-size necessary to detect a quarter of a small effect size (r = .10, R 2 = .01) for Pearson correlation analysis (r = .0499, R 2 = .0025), with a statistical power of .95 (1 − β = .95) and conventional values of Type I and Type II error rates (α = .05; 1 − β = .95). The results indicated that a minimum sample of 5,200 participants was required (Faul et al., Reference Faul, Erdfelder, Lang and Buchner2007; Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Alcaide, Musitu-Ferrer, Pons-Benavent and Garcia2024; Pérez et al., Reference Pérez, Navarro and Llobell1999).

A sensitivity power analysis for the study sample (N = 4.953) with a statistical power of .95 and the same Type I and Type II error rates (α = .05; 1 − β = .95), indicated the sensitivity to detect an effect size close to the target (r = .0509, R 2 = .0026), for Pearson correlation analysis, considering 4,951 error degrees of freedom (Faul et al., Reference Faul, Erdfelder, Buchner and Lang2009; Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Pascual, Frias, Van Krunckelsven and Murgui2008). These calculations were performed using G*Power 3.1 software (Faul et al., Reference Faul, Erdfelder, Buchner and Lang2009).

To achieve the study sample of 4,953 participants, principals from 12 high schools in a large metropolitan area of Nuevo León, Mexico—a city with over one million inhabitants—were contacted. Schools were randomly selected from a comprehensive list of 106 schools in the city. Participants (96% response rate) met the following criteria: (1) Mexican nationality, (2) aged between 14 and 17 years, (3) enrolled in grades 9 through 12, (4) obtained parental or legal guardian consent, and (5) attended the designated classroom for data collection. Ethical approval for the study was granted by the ethics committee of Autonomous University of Nuevo Leon (Mexico), code No. 2023/90. The study adhered to the principles outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki.

Measures

Self-concept was measured with the Five-Factor Self-Concept Questionnaire (AF-5; Garcia & Musitu, Reference Garcia and Musitu1999). This questionnaire captures self-concept through 30 items distributed across five dimensions: academic, social, emotional, family, and physical self-concepts. The academic or work self-concept refers to how the subject perceives their performance as a student or worker (e.g., “My teachers think I am a good student”). The social self-concept, which consists of the perceptions the subjects have of their performance in social settings (e.g., “I am a friendly person”). Emotional self-concept, which measures the perception the individual has of their emotional state and their responses to specific circumstances (e.g., “A lot of things make me nervous,” reversed item). Family self-concept, which refers to the individual’s perceptions of participation, involvement, and integration in the family context (e.g., “I feel happy at home”). Physical self-concept, which captures the perception the subject has of their physical appearance and physical performance (e.g., “I like my physical appearance). Each subscale consists of 6 items, which are answered on a 99-point scale, ranging from 1 = “Complete Disagreement” to 99 = “Complete Agreement” (Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Musitu, Riquelme and Riquelme2011; Garcia & Musitu, Reference Garcia and Musitu1999). A higher score on the subscales indicates a higher self-concept. The AF-5 has shown good psychometric properties. The factor structure has been confirmed in different studies (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Garcia, Alcaide, Garcia-Ros and Garcia2024; Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Musitu and Veiga2006; Garcia & Musitu, Reference Garcia and Musitu1999; Tomás & Oliver, Reference Tomás and Oliver2004). The AF-5 is widely used to capture multidimensional self-concept in countries, such as Spain (Fuentes et al., Reference Fuentes, Garcia, Gracia and Lila2011, Reference Fuentes, Garcia, Alcaide, Garcia-Ros and Garcia2022; Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Musitu, Riquelme and Riquelme2011), Portugal (Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Musitu and Veiga2006), Brazil (Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Martinez, Balluerka, Cruise, Garcia and Serra2018), the United States (Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Gracia and Zeleznova2013), and China (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Garcia, Fuentes, Garcia-Ros and Garcia2020). The AF-5 has not shown method effects related to negatively worded items (Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Musitu, Riquelme and Riquelme2011; Tomás & Oliver, Reference Tomás and Oliver2004). Cronbach’s alpha was .87 for academic, .81 for social, .82 for emotional, .87 for family, and .79 for physical subscales.

Parent–child communication was measured using the Parent-Adolescent Communication Scale (PACS; Barnes & Olson, Reference Barnes and Olson1982). The PACS questionnaire captures the dyadic quality of parent to adolescent communication through 20 items spread across three subscales: open communication, communication problems, and avoidant communication (Feldman & Rosenthal, Reference Feldman and Rosenthal2000; Zapf et al., Reference Zapf, Boettcher, Haukeland, Orm, Coslar, Wiegand-Grefe and Fjermestad2023). The open communication subscale evaluates free exchange of information in an understanding environment (e.g., “When I ask questions, I get honest answers from my mother/father”). The communication problems subscale evaluates the resistance to share and negative interaction styles (e.g., “My mother/father insults me when s/he is angry with me”). The avoidant communication subscale assesses selectivity and caution in the content that is shared (e.g., “I am sometimes afraid to ask my mother/father for what I want”). The items are answered in a 5-Point Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 = “Never” to 5 = “Always”. For open communication, greater scores represent a good sense of parent–child communication, whereas for both communication problems and avoidant communication subscales, greater scores represent lower parent–child communication. Adolescents rated the items twice, once for mother and once for father (Feldman & Rosenthal, Reference Feldman and Rosenthal2000). The PACS is amongst the most popular questionnaires to measure parent–child communication (Barnes & Olson, Reference Barnes and Olson1982; Zapf et al., Reference Zapf, Boettcher, Haukeland, Orm, Coslar, Wiegand-Grefe and Fjermestad2023). The alpha values for mother–child communication were .93 for open communication, .79 for communication problems, and .75 for avoidant communication. The alpha values for father–child communication were .94 for open communication, .73 for communication problems, and .72 for avoidant communication.

Family functioning was assessed using the Family APGAR Questionnaire (Smilkstein, Reference Smilkstein1978). This instrument measures global family functioning through 5 items that assess adaptability, partnership, growth, affection, and resolve (e.g., “I find that my family accepts my wishes to take on new activities or make changes in my lifestyle”). The responses to the items are answered in a 3-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 0 = “Never” to 2 = “Sometimes”. Higher scores represent a higher family functioning. Studies have shown that the Family APGAR Questionnaire has good psychometric properties (Campo-Arias & Caballero-Domínguez, Reference Campo-Arias and Caballero-Domínguez2021; Good et al., Reference Good, Smilkstein, Good, Shaffer and Arons1979; Smilkstein et al., Reference Smilkstein, Ashworth and Montano1982). In the present study, Cronbach’s alpha was .81.

Psychological distress was measured though the Kessler Psychological Distress Scale (Furukawa et al., Reference Furukawa, Kessler, Slade and Andrews2003; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Andrews, Colpe, Hiripi, Mroczek, Normand, Walters and Zaslavsky2002). The K10 questionnaire measures distress in the past 30 days through 10 items that capture the frequency of psychological distress symptoms (e.g., “Did you feel so nervous that nothing could calm you down”). The items are answered in a 5-point Likert-scale, ranging from 1 = “None of the Time” to 5 = “All of the Time”. Higher scores represent more psychological distress (Furukawa et al., Reference Furukawa, Kessler, Slade and Andrews2003). The K10 has been reported to have adequate psychometric properties (Vargas-Terrez et al., Reference Vargas-Terrez, Villamil-Salcedo, Rodríguez-Estrada, Pérez-Romero and Cortés-Sotres2011). In the present study, Cronbach’s alpha was .90.

Depressive symptoms were measured with the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff, Reference Radloff1977). This scale evaluates the frequency and severity of depressive symptoms through 20 items (e.g., “I thought my life had been a failure”). The questions are answered in a five-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 = “None of the Time” to 5 = “All of the Time” (Bean et al., Reference Bean, Mueller, Abitante, Ciesla, Cho and Cole2024; Eaton et al., Reference Eaton, Muntaner, Smith, Tien, Ybarra and Maruish2004). Higher scores in the scale represent more depressive symptoms. The CES-D has shown adequate psychometric properties (Herrero & Gracia, Reference Herrero and Gracia2007; Radloff, Reference Radloff1991, Reference Radloff1977). In the present study, Cronbach’s alpha was .91.

Data Analysis

Confirmatory Factor Analysis

Structural equation models (SEMs) were used to test which theoretical model is the most accurate by analyzing its goodness-of-fit to the empirical data. The software EQS 6.1 was used to examine the SEMs (Byrne, Reference Byrne2006). A robust maximum likelihood method was applied as self-concept does not follow a multinormal distribution (Satorra & Bentler, Reference Satorra and Bentler2001). In addition, the analyses were run again after the 99-point scale was transformed into a discrete scale with two points, one above and one below the median value, to reduce the error variance and increase the adjustment of the models to the data (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Garcia, Fuentes, Garcia-Ros and Garcia2020; Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Musitu and Veiga2006; Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Gracia and Zeleznova2013).

In the first step, the fit of the one-factor model of self-concept to data was tested. This model considers self-concept as one dimension (Baumeister et al., Reference Baumeister, Campbell, Krueger and Vohs2003; Rosenberg, Reference Rosenberg1965). In the second step, the orthogonal five-factor model was tested, where self-concept is considered multidimensional and has five orthogonal (i.e., unrelated) dimensions (i.e., academic, social, emotional, family, and physical) (Burbach & Bridgemen, Reference Burbach and Bridgemen1976; Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Martinez, Balluerka, Cruise, Garcia and Serra2018; Shavelson et al., Reference Shavelson, Hubner and Stanton1976). In the third step, the oblique five-factor model of self-concept was tested, where self-concept has five dimensions, but they are correlated between each-other (Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Musitu and Veiga2006, Reference Garcia, Martinez, Balluerka, Cruise, Garcia and Serra2018; Shavelson et al., Reference Shavelson, Hubner and Stanton1976). Finally, in the fourth step, the five-dimensional oblique (i.e., correlated) model is tested, but error covariances are released for the most correlated pair of items within each factor (Byrne & Shavelson, Reference Byrne and Shavelson1996).

To assess the model fit to data, robust goodness-of-fit indices were used, the Satorra-Bentler chi-squared statistic, the root-mean-squared error of approximation (RMSEA) with 90% CI, comparative fit index (CFI), and Akaike information criterion (AIC). The RMSEA with a 90% confidence interval indicates a poor fit for values above .08, acceptable fit for values between .05 and .08, and good fit for values lower than .05 (Browne & Cudeck, Reference Browne and Cudeck1992). The CFI indicates an acceptable fit when values are above .90, and a good fit when values are above .95 (Marsh & Hau, Reference Marsh and Hau1996). AIC suggests a better fit of the model to the data the lower its value (Akaike, Reference Akaike1987).

Invariance across sex and age groups

To test the equivalence (i.e., invariance) of model parameters across sex (men vs. women) and age groups (14–15-year-olds vs. 16–17-year-olds), four nested models that progressively increased the number of restrictions by constraining free parameters was proved: (a) unconstrained, without any restrictions across parameters, (b) restricting factor pattern coefficients across the samples, (c) restricting factor variances and covariances across the samples, and (d) restricting equality of the error variances across the samples. The |∆CFI| is a robust index of invariance for nested models (Cheung & Rensvold, Reference Cheung and Rensvold2002). Cheung and Rensvold (Reference Cheung and Rensvold2002) suggested that |∆CFI| values smaller than .010 (i.e., |∆CFI| < .010) indicate invariance.

Analysis of the relationship between self-concept with indicators

Pearson’s r correlation coefficients were used to calculate the relationship between the dimensions of self-concept (i.e., academic, social, emotional, family, and physical), family-specific indicators based on family processes (i.e., parent–child communication and family functioning), and family-non-specific indicators associated with mental health deterioration (i.e., psychological distress and depressive symptoms). To calculate the Pearson’s r correlation and its confidence intervals, IBM SPSS Statistics version 28 was used (previously, the correlation procedure in SPSS did not provide confidence intervals in the output) (Beaulieu-Prevost, Reference Beaulieu-Prevost2006; Fisher, Reference Fisher1925; Weaver & Koopman, Reference Weaver and Koopman2014). Cohen’s d effect size was calculated using the shared variance (R 2) from Pearson’s r correlation coefficient (Ferguson, Reference Ferguson2016; Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Pascual, Frias, Van Krunckelsven and Murgui2008; Vacha-Haase & Thompson, Reference Vacha-Haase and Thompson2004).

However, since the R 2 (shared variance effect size) does not retain the sign of the correlation, Cohen’s d effect size and its confidence intervals were calculated from R 2 Pearson’s correlation. Cohen’s d has a known normal distribution and retains the sign of the relationship. Cohen’s d effect size was used to represent the effect size of the relationship between each self-concept dimension and the family-specific and family-non-specific indicators. For Cohen’s d effect sizes, values equal to or below 0.19 are considered irrelevant, values between 0.20 and 0.49 are small, values between 0.50 and 0.79 are medium, and values equal to or above 0.80 are large (Cohen, Reference Cohen1988).

In addition, to test for significant differences between the effect sizes of the relationships, confidence interval analysis was applied. There were no statistically significant differences when confidence intervals overlapped, while statistically significant differences were indicated when confidence intervals did not overlap (Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Pascual, Frias, Van Krunckelsven and Murgui2008; Stevens, Reference Stevens1992; Weaver & Koopman, Reference Weaver and Koopman2014). The advantage of this method is that the null hypothesis test only indicates whether the relationships between two variables differ from zero, whereas Cohen’s d effect size confidence intervals assess whether the effect sizes are equivalent. With this common scale, it is possible to determine the size of the relationship and compare it with other variables, even across studies, as seen in meta-analyses (Poston & Hanson, Reference Poston and Hanson2010; Powers & Emmelkamp, Reference Powers and Emmelkamp2008; Rock et al., Reference Rock, Roiser, Riedel and Blackwell2014). In this regard, scholars have emphasized that effect sizes should be reported alongside statistical significance (Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Pascual, Frias, Van Krunckelsven and Murgui2008; Stevens, Reference Stevens1992; Weaver & Koopman, Reference Weaver and Koopman2014).

Results

Confirmatory Factor Analysis

The comparisons between four empirical competitive models to the data (Table 1) showed that the model that worst fit the data was the unidimensional model of self-concept, CFI = .540, RMSEA = .122 (90% CI = .121 – .123), AIC = 29471.8. The orthogonal five-factor model showed an improvement in the fit to the data, CFI = .848, RMSEA = .070 (90% CI = .069 – .071), AIC = 9444.6, and there was no overlap with the RMSEA confidence intervals with the previous model. By releasing the orthogonality constraint, the oblique five-factor model improved the fit to the data, CFI = .884, RMSEA = .062 (90% CI = .061 – .063), AIC = 7125.2, and there was also no overlap with the RMSEA confidence intervals with the previous model. Similarly, when testing the fit of the data to the oblique five-factor model while freeing the error covariance for the more correlated pairs of items, the best-fit indices were achieved, CFI = .921, RMSEA = .052 (90% CI = .050 – .053), AIC = 4745.1, and there was also no overlap with the RMSEA confidence intervals with the previous model.

Table 1. Confirmatory factor analysis and analysis for the invariance between sex and age groups

Note: Satorra-Bentler Chi-square (SB-χ2), degrees of freedom (df), Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), Comparative Fix Index (CFI), value of the change in CFI |ΔCFI|, Akaike Information Criterion (AIC). Tr model is the same as T but with the restriction of independence for error freed in the pairs: 17-2, 13-3, 14-4, 16-6, 25-10.

In addition, all the fit indices consistently improved when the models were examined based on the discrete scale. The unidimensional model had the worst fit to the data, CFI = .585, RMSEA = .092 (90% CI = .091 – .094), AIC = 16720.1. The orthogonal five-factor model showed an improvement of fit to the data, CFI = .891, RMSEA = .047 (90% CI = .046 – .048), AIC = 4078.0. The oblique five-factor model also had an improvement of fit to the data, CFI = .913, RMSEA = .043 (90% CI = .041 – .044), AIC = 3172.1. The best fit of the data was achieved with the oblique five-factor model while freeing the error covariance for the more correlated pairs of items, CFI = .962, RMSEA = .025 (90% CI = .023 – .027), AIC = 214.7. Also, the RMSEA confidence intervals did not overlap with each step, indicating an improvement in model fit to the data.

Invariance across Sex and Age Groups

The fit indices for the four increasingly restrictive models of invariance between sexes (males vs. females) and ages (14–15 vs. 16–17) obtained satisfactory results (Table 1). The equal loadings model, which constrains the pattern loadings across sex (ΔCFI < .01) and age (ΔCFI < .01) suggested that factor loadings were invariant across all subsamples. In addition, since the RMSEA confidence intervals overlapped, the fit of the more restrictive model was equivalent. It can be observed that all items loaded in the assigned factor with loadings always larger than .42 (Tables 2 and 3). These results have been maintained even though a single loading value has been forced to be maintained for all subsamples. For example, item 3, which corresponded to the emotional factor, had an estimated loading of .55 in both sexes (Table 2), and .57 for both age groups (Table 3).

Table 2. Confirmatory factor analysis loadings, variances, covariances, and errors in the most constrained model across sex

Note: AC = Academic; SO = Social; EM = Emotional; FA = Family; PH = Physical. All estimated parameters were statistically significant (p < .05), except the covariance between EM and AC. Negatively worded items (3, 4, 8, 12, 13, 14, 18, 22, 23, and 28) were inverted.

Table 3. Confirmatory factor analysis loadings, variances, covariances, and errors in the most constrained model across age

Note: AC = Academic; SO = Social; EM = Emotional; FA = Family; PH = Physical. All estimated parameters were statistically significant (p < .05), except the covariance between EM and AC. Negatively worded items (3, 4, 8, 12, 13, 14, 18, 22, 23, and 28) were inverted.

The equal variances and covariances model, which constrains the pattern of variances and covariances across sex (ΔCFI < .01) and age (ΔCFI < .01) suggested that factor patterns of variances and covariances were invariant across all subsamples. In addition, since the RMSEA confidence intervals overlapped, the fit of the more restrictive model was equivalent. All the factors had the same patterns of variances and structural covariances and, despite maintaining the same values, the model did not lose fit. It can be observed that the emotional variance was 19.08 between sexes (Table 2) and 20.16 between age groups (Table 3). The covariance of the family factor with the social factor was 19.78 in both sexes (Table 2) and 19.84 in both age groups (Table 3). The correlation of family with the social factor was .37 in both sexes (Table 2) and .37 in both age groups (Table 3).

The model of equal estimation errors for each item, which imposes equal estimation error between sex (ΔCFI < .010) and age (ΔCFI < .010) suggested that estimation errors were invariant for all subsamples. In addition, since the RMSEA confidence intervals overlapped, the fit of the more restrictive model was equivalent. All factors had the same estimation errors, and despite maintaining the same values, the model did not lose fit. It can be observed that item 10, which belongs to the physical factor, had an estimated error of .86 for both sexes (Table 2) and .85 (Table 3) for both age groups.

Relationships between self-concept with indicators

The study examined the relationships between different dimensions of self-concept (academic, social, emotional, family, and physical) and family-specific indicators (parent–child communication and family functioning), finding both positive and negative relationships, as shown by Cohen’s d effect sizes. Positive relationships showed effect sizes distributed to the right, while negative relationships were distributed to the left (see Tables 4 and 5, Figures 1– 3).

Table 4. Correlations (r) between self-concept and parent–child communication, family functioning, psychological distress, and depressive symptoms

Note: AC = Academic; SO = Social; EM = Emotional; FA = Family; PH = Physical. All Pearson correlations were statistically significant (p < .05), except avoidant communication in Father Family Communication and academic self-concept.

Table 5. Cohen’s (d) effect size values between self-concept, and parent–child communication, family functioning, psychological distress, and depressive symptoms

Note: AC = Academic; SO = Social; EM = Emotional; FA = Family; PH = Physical. Effect size of d: 0irrelevant (≤0.19), 1small (0.20 – 0.49), 2medium (0.50 – 0.79), and 3large (≥0.80).

Figure 1. Cohen’s (d) effect size and 95% CI values between mother–child and father–child open communication, communication problems, and avoidant communication with five self-concept dimensions.

Figure 2. Cohen’s (d) effect size and 95% CI values between family functioning, with five self-concept dimensions.

Figure 3. Cohen’s (d) effect size and 95% CI values between psychological distress, and depressive symptoms with five self-concept dimensions.

The relationship between self-concept dimensions and open communication was positive, with significant results (α = .05), as no confidence interval included 0. Confidence interval overlaps (1 − α = .95) revealed three statistically different levels of relationship. At the highest level, family self-concept was most strongly associated with open communication, with mother–child communication (d = 1.83, 95% CI [1.81, 1.92]) showing a significantly stronger relationship than father–child communication (d = 1.33, 95% CI [1.31, 1.42]), as their confidence intervals did not overlap. The relationship had a large effect size (d > .8). At the next level, academic, social, and physical self-concepts showed medium effect sizes (d > .5) with overlapping confidence intervals. Emotional self-concept, at the third level, showed a small effect size (d > .2). In these levels, there were no differences between mother–child and father–child communication, as confidence intervals overlapped.

For parent–child communication problems, negative relationships with self-concept dimensions were significant (α = .05), as no confidence interval included 0. There were two distinct levels, with family self-concept showing the strongest relationship, and mother–child problems (d = −1.19, 95% CI [−1.31, −1.12]) being more strongly related than father–child problems (d = −0.93, 95% CI [−1.01, −0.92]). This relationship also had a large effect size (d > |.8|). At the next level, academic, social, emotional, and physical self-concepts had small effect sizes (d > |.2|) with overlapping intervals, with no differences between mother–child and father–child communication problems.

Negative relationships also emerged between self-concept dimensions and avoidant communication. In the first level, family and emotional self-concepts were moderately related to mother–child avoidant communication (d = −0.54, 95% CI [−0.61, −0.52] and d = −0.52, 95% CI [−0.61, −0.52], respectively). Father–child avoidant communication was weakly associated with emotional self-concept (d = −0.46, 95% CI [−0.51, −0.42]). At the third level, mother–child avoidant communication was related to social (d = −0.30, 95% CI [−0.41, −0.22]) and physical self-concepts (d = −0.23, 95% CI [−0.31, −0.22]), and father–child avoidant communication with family self-concept (d = −0.38, 95% CI [−0.41, −0.32]) and social self-concept (d = −0.20, 95% CI [−0.31, −0.12]), all showing small effect sizes. Some relationships, like between avoidant communication and academic or physical self-concept, were insignificant (d < |.2|), with one confidence intervals including 0.

Family functioning had significant positive relationships with self-concept dimensions, with confidence interval overlaps revealing three levels. At the top level, family self-concept had the strongest association (d = 1.84, 90% CI [1.81, 1.92]). Academic, physical, and social self-concepts at the second level had medium to small effect sizes, while emotional self-concept at the third level showed a small effect size.

Negative relationships were also found between self-concept dimensions and mental health indicators, specifically psychological distress and depressive symptoms. For psychological distress, emotional and family self-concepts had strong associations with large effect sizes (d > .8). Social and physical self-concepts at the second level had medium effect sizes (d > |.5|), while academic self-concept, at the third level, showed a small effect size. For depressive symptoms, family and emotional self-concepts showed large associations, with physical and social self-concepts showing moderate to small associations. At the fourth level, academic self-concept showed a medium effect size.

Family self-concept consistently showed higher relationships with indicators than social self-concept, with confidence intervals indicating no overlap. Family self-concept had the strongest associations with family functioning and mother–child open communication. It was also more strongly associated with other indicators than social self-concept, including father–child open communication, mother–child communication problems, psychological distress, depressive symptoms, and father–child communication problems.

Discussion

For decades, psychological science has been examining what makes a person unique—the self—through multidimensional models. However, these theoretical models require empirical testing in psychological science. The present study tests a model based on the Big Five dimensions of self-concept, which includes the often-overlooked dimension: family self-concept. Family self-concept can be viewed as a part of the self, distinct from one’s self-perceptions of good social skills (i.e., social self-concept). Nevertheless, classical models have regarded family self-concept as a sub-area of social self-concept (Byrne & Shavelson, Reference Byrne and Shavelson1996; Shavelson et al., Reference Shavelson, Hubner and Stanton1976). Furthermore, classical dimensions (academic, social, emotional, and physical), when compared with the neglected dimension of family self-concept, may not exhibit the same relationships, not only with family processes but also with the deterioration of mental health. This study has examined whether the family aspect should be distinguished from the broader social dimension.

First of all, the study tested which theoretical structure of self-concept best fit the data. This question is crucial for ensuring that theoretical models are grounded in empirical evidence. The CFA analyses showed that the big five model of self-concept, based on different but related dimensions (i.e., an oblique model), provided a better fit to the data than the competing five-dimensional orthogonal and unidimensional models. The indicators showed that the items for each dimension loaded on their theoretically corresponding factor, suggesting that the Big Five factor model has empirical support. In addition, the Big Five structure of self-concept was invariant across sex and age. The invariance analyses revealed that both females and males, as well as early and late adolescents, did not differ in their response patterns. These findings represent a significant advance in psychology, as they facilitate the identification of the multidimensional structure of self-concept based on the big five factors. Particularly in the study of self-concept during adolescence, it is important for models to be invariant with respect to sex and age. These findings extend previous studies providing support for a five-factor structure of self-concept that is invariant across sex, age, and cultural contexts (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Garcia, Fuentes, Garcia-Ros and Garcia2020; Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Musitu and Veiga2006, Reference Garcia, Martinez, Balluerka, Cruise, Garcia and Serra2018; Murgui et al., Reference Murgui, Garcia, Garcia and Garcia2012). Compliance with the invariance requirement ensures comparability between groups (e.g., males and females). This invariance testing is a prerequisite often overlooked in research. Establishing invariant models is essential for enhancing the scientific understanding of self-concept across different contexts and demographic characteristics (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Garcia, Fuentes, Garcia-Ros and Garcia2020; Marsh et al., Reference Marsh, Byrne and Shavelson1988; Putnick & Bornstein, Reference Putnick and Bornstein2016).

This study analyzed the relationships between self-concept and parent–child communication (in terms of open communication, communication problems, and avoidant communication) as well as family functioning, both family-specific. Family self-concept had the strongest relation to all family process indicators. In no case was the social self-concept related to family indicators to an equal or greater extent than family self-concept. Instead, social self-concept was similarly related to family processes as the other self-concept dimensions (academic, physical, and emotional) in almost all cases; except for parent–child avoidant communication, where social self-concept had an even weaker association than emotional self-concept. Parent–child communication (i.e., open communication, communication problems, avoidant communication) had a high relationship with family self-concept, particularly for mothers. Mothers show a stronger link between communication patterns and family self-concept compared to fathers, probably because adolescents disclose more to mothers than to fathers (Keijsers et al., Reference Keijsers, Branje, VanderValk and Meeus2010; Smetana et al., Reference Smetana, Metzger, Gettman and Campione-Barr2006; Waizenhofer et al., Reference Waizenhofer, Buchanan and Jackson-Newsom2004).

According to the theoretical assumption of the specificity principle in the four-factor model, since social self-concept represents family self-concept, the social dimension would have at least equivalent relation with family process indicators as the family dimension (Byrne & Shavelson, Reference Byrne and Shavelson1996; Shavelson et al., Reference Shavelson, Hubner and Stanton1976). Previous research has assumed that family processes were especially related to good interpersonal relationships with peers and significant others (i.e., social self-concept) (Archuleta et al., Reference Archuleta, Kaminski and Ross2024; Fraley & Roisman, Reference Fraley and Roisman2019). However, evidence from the present research questioned this assumption. According to the present findings, social self-concept does not seem to represent family self-concept. Although, in general, the family indicators were linked to all dimensions of self-concept (i.e., oblique model), the strongest relationship was found with the family dimension (i.e., family self-concept). Family processes are not solely about helping a child develop good social skills (Alcaide et al., Reference Alcaide, Garcia, Queiroz and Garcia2023; Lamborn et al., Reference Lamborn, Mounts, Steinberg and Dornbusch1991). Family processes are related to social self-concept to a similar degree as the other self-concept dimensions, but always to a lesser degree than to family self-concept. It seems that family processes have a uniquely relevant relationship in helping the child to feel like a loved and valued member of the family (i.e., family self-concept). Thus, the findings from this study question the assumption that social self-concept represents family processes. The family dimension of self-concept appears to be more accurate (i.e., specific) for studying family processes than the social dimension.

Although family self-concept was always the dimension of self-concept that showed the greatest relationship with family processes, not all family processes were related to family self-concept to the same degree. The findings revealed that parent–child communication based on open communication and family functioning were highly related to a great family self-concept and in the same degree of relationship. As previous research indicated, open communication can help children feel valued and loved as members of their family. Parents can support their children by reasoning and explaining rules to provide clear guidance, and importantly, by being available for discussions about concerns and worries, serving as guides and references (Fraley & Roisman, Reference Fraley and Roisman2019; Kerr et al., Reference Kerr, Stattin and Trost1999). However, family processes extend beyond parent–child relationships. The study findings confirm the importance not only of the parent–child relationship (Martinez et al., Reference Martinez, Murgui, Garcia and Garcia2021; Smetana et al., Reference Smetana, Metzger, Gettman and Campione-Barr2006) but also the general relationships among all family members (family functioning) (Kapetanovic & Skoog, Reference Kapetanovic and Skoog2021; Olson et al., Reference Olson, Sprenkle and Russell1979).

However, some aspects of family processes are negatively associated with family self-concept. Specifically, communication problems and avoidant communication were both negatively related to family self-concept, although the strength of these relationships was weaker than that observed in other family processes analyzed (i.e., open communication and family functioning). In addition, avoidant communication was negatively related to both the family and emotional components of the self to the same degree. Avoidant communication may reflect the unavailability of parental figures as referents (Fraley & Roisman, Reference Fraley and Roisman2019). These findings are crucial, as they suggest a risk factor associated with the emotional dimension of self-concept, particularly in relation to a specific form of communication: avoidant communication. In stressful and problematic situations, children are likely to avoid communicating, which may not reduce their emotional arousal and could affect their confidence and emotional skills (Kapetanovic & Skoog, Reference Kapetanovic and Skoog2021; Segrin & Flora, Reference Segrin and Flora2018).

This study not only examined family processes and their relationship with self-concept in different ways (positively or negatively) and to varying extents but it also extends previous research on the relationships between mental health deterioration and the five dimensions of self-concept by showing a strong relationship with emotional self-concept (Gross & Muñoz, Reference Gross and Muñoz1995; Young et al., Reference Young, Sandman and Craske2019; Zapf et al., Reference Zapf, Boettcher, Haukeland, Orm, Coslar and Fjermestad2024). However, family self-concept had a relationship with mental health deterioration that is at least equal to, or even greater than, that of emotional self-concept. Although psychological distress and depressive symptoms are non-specific family indicators, family self-concept was closely related to them according to the findings from the present study. Psychological distress showed the highest relationship with both family and emotional self-concept. However, regarding depressive symptoms, the highest relationship was found with family self-concept, followed by emotional self-concept in second place. In addition, social self-concept consistently had a weaker relationship compared to family self-concept across both indicators of mental health deterioration: psychological distress and depressive symptoms.

Previous research has identified that psychological distress and depressive symptoms both represent forms of serious emotional disturbance (Lamborn et al., Reference Lamborn, Mounts, Steinberg and Dornbusch1991; Van Dijk et al., Reference Van Dijk, Branje, Keijsers, Hawk, Hale and Meeus2014). The findings showed that psychological distress was highly related to emotional self-concept, which is to be expected due to the principle of specificity of self-concept, as psychological distress represents a form of overactivity related to the management of emotions (Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Puzia and Yaptangco2015). However, the strength of the relationship between psychological distress and emotional self-concept was no greater than in family self-concept: emotional and family self-concept had the highest relation to psychological distress, followed by physical and social self-concept in the second place, and academic self-concept in the third place. Therefore, the findings from the present study indicated that family self-concept was not only related to family-specific indicators (i.e., parent–child communication and family functioning) but also was highly and negatively related to psychological distress, one of the two indicators of mental health deterioration.

Regarding depressive symptoms, the other indicator of mental health, family self-concept had the highest relationship, followed by emotional self-concept in second place, physical and social self-concept in third place, and academic self-concept in fourth. Previous research has shown that depressive symptoms represent a sense of sadness and loss of pleasure in previously enjoyed activities (Garaigordobil et al., Reference Garaigordobil, Ignacio Perez and Mozaz2008; Orth et al., Reference Orth, Robins, Widaman and Conger2014; Young et al., Reference Young, Sandman and Craske2019). These findings are crucial, as depressive symptoms were negatively related to emotional, family, and physical self-concept, although to different extents. Among the self-concept dimensions, family self-concept showed the highest relationship with depressive symptoms, even though depressive symptoms are a non-family-specific indicator. According to previous literature, depressive symptoms are associated with an overall deterioration of self, particularly in emotional regulation (Gross & Muñoz, Reference Gross and Muñoz1995; Rubach et al., Reference Rubach, Dicke, Lazarides, Simpkins and Eccles2020; Young et al., Reference Young, Sandman and Craske2019; Zapf et al., Reference Zapf, Boettcher, Haukeland, Orm, Coslar and Fjermestad2024). However, not feeling loved and valued in home—family self-concept, a dimension of the self that is often overlooked—was, according to the present results, even more closely related than the emotional dimension. Social self-concept was also negatively related to depressive symptoms but to a much lesser degree than family self-concept. These findings suggest the need to distinguish between family self-concept and social self-concept in the study of mental health.

However, this study is not without limitations. The investigation relied on data obtained through a cross-sectional design. It is recommended that future research continue to explore the multidimensional facets of self-concept by employing longitudinal designs and even experimental methodologies. Moreover, the collective administration of surveys carries the risk of eliciting a considerable proportion of random responses. To mitigate this issue, future studies should incorporate measures designed to detect infrequent responses and identify social desirability biases. Furthermore, the use of self-report measures introduces inherent limitations, as respondents may encounter challenges in accurately articulating their own behaviors, cognitions, and affective states.

In sum, this study reveals that self-concept presents congruent relationships not only with family processes but also with mental health indicators, although to different extents. Based on the original theoretical model of self-concept, it had been assumed that social self-concept is representative of family self-concept. The present study employs sensitive tests (i.e., effect size and their confidence intervals) to address the limitations of most previous research that did not allow for comparisons between the sizes of variable relationships. The findings suggest that family self-concept is distinct from social self-concept due to its high relationship with family processes. In addition, for mental health deterioration, family self-concept is found to be equally or even more related than the ability to regulate one’s own emotions (i.e., emotional self-concept). Mental health deterioration seems to be closely linked to not feeling loved and valued by one’s own family.

Data availability statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Author contribution

M.E.: Conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, methodology, software, validation, visualization, writing – original draft, writing – review & editing. J.C.S.S.: Conceptualization, data curation, investigation, methodology, project administration, resources, validation, and visualization. O.F.G.: Conceptualization, methodology, supervision, validation, visualization, writing – original draft, writing – review & editing. M.E.V.G.: Conceptualization, data curation, investigation, methodology, project administration, resources, validation, and visualization. F.G.: Conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, funding acquisition, methodology, project administration, software, supervision, validation, visualization, writing – original draft, writing – review & editing.

Funding statement

This work was supported by the Conselleria for Innovación, Universidades, Ciencia y Sociedad Digital of the Generalitat Valenciana (grant number CIAICO/2021/252).

Competing interest

None.

References

Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52(3), 317332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alcaide, M., Garcia, O. F., Queiroz, P., & Garcia, F. (2023). Adjustment and maladjustment to later life: Evidence about early experiences in the family. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, 1059458. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1059458CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Archuleta, W. P., Kaminski, P. L., & Ross, N. D. (2024). The roles of shame and poor self-concept in explaining low social connection among adult survivors of childhood emotional maltreatment. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 16(7), 11491157. https://doi.org/10.1037/tra0001559Google ScholarPubMed
Barnes, H. L., & Olson, D. H. (1982). Barnes and Olson Communication Scale. APA PsycTests, https://doi.org/10.1037/t56782-000CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F. (2010). The self. In Baumeister, R. F., & Finkel, E. J. (Eds.), Advanced social psychology: The state of the science (pp. 139175). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Campbell, J. D., Krueger, J. I. I., & Vohs, K. D. (2003). Does high self-esteem cause better performance, interpersonal success, happiness, or healthier lifestyles? Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 4(1), 144. https://doi.org/10.1111/1529-1006.01431CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumrind, D. (1968). Authoritarian vs authoritative parental control. Adolescence, 3, 255272.Google Scholar
Bean, C. A. L., Mueller, S. B., Abitante, G., Ciesla, J. A., Cho, S., & Cole, D. A. (2024). Improved scoring of the center for epidemiologic studies depression scale – revised: An item response theory analysis. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 46(3), 783792. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-024-10155-yCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beaulieu-Prevost, D. (2006). Confidence Intervals: From tests of statistical significance to confidence intervals, range hypotheses and substantial effects. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 2(11) https://doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.02.1.p011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, L., & Alexander, J. (1991). Self-Esteem Index. PRO-ED.Google Scholar
Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1992). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Sociological Methods and Research, 21, 230258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunner, M., Keller, U., Dierendonck, C., Reichert, M., Ugen, S., Fischbach, A., & Martin, R. (2010). The structure of academic self-concepts revisited the nested marsh/shavelson model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(4), 964981. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019644CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burbach, H. J., & Bridgemen, B. (1976). Dimensions of self-concept among black and white fifth grade children. Journal of Negro Education, 45(4), 448458. https://doi.org/10.2307/2966857CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, B. M. (1984). The general/academic self-concept nomological network: A review of construct validation research. Review of Educational Research, 54(3), 427456. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543054003427CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, B. M. (2006). Structural equation modeling with EQS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming (2nd ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Byrne, B. M., & Shavelson, R. J. (1996). On the structure of social self-concept for pre-, early, and late adolescents: A test of the Shavelson, Hubner, and Stanton (1976) model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 599613.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campo-Arias, A., & Caballero-Domínguez, C. C. (2021). Confirmatory factor analysis of the family APGAR Questionnaire. Revista Colombiana de Psiquiatría (English Ed.), 50(4), 234237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rcpeng.2021.10.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chansky, T. E., & Kendall, P. C. (1997). Social expectancies and self-perceptions in anxiety-disordered children. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 11(4), 347363. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0887-6185(97)00015-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, F., Garcia, O. F., Alcaide, M., Garcia-Ros, R., & Garcia, F. (2024). Do we know enough about negative parenting? Recent evidence on parenting styles and child maladjustment. The European Journal of Psychology Applied to Legal Context, 16, 3748. https://doi.org/10.5093/ejpalc2024a4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, F., Garcia, O. F., Fuentes, M. C., Garcia-Ros, R., & Garcia, F. (2020). Self-concept in China: Validation of the Chinese version of the Five-Factor Self-Concept (AF5) Questionnaire. Symmetry-Basel, 12(798), 113. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym12050798Google Scholar
Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating Goodness-of-Fit Indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 9(2), 233255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Climent-Galarza, S., Alcaide, M., Garcia, O. F., Chen, F., & Garcia, F. (2022). Parental socialization, delinquency during adolescence and adjustment in adolescents and adult children. Behavioral Sciences, 12(11), 448. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12110448CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203771587Google Scholar
Connolly, J. (1989). Social self-efficacy in adolescence: Relations with self-concept, social adjustment, and mental health. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science / Revue Canadienne des Sciences du Comportement, 21(3), 258269. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0079809CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooley, C. H. (1902). Looking-glass self. The Production of Reality: Essays and Readings on Social Interaction, 6(1902), 126128.Google Scholar
Coopersmith, S. (1967). The antecedents of self-esteem. Freeman.Google Scholar
Cornella-Font, M., Vinas-Poch, F., Juarez-Lopez, J. R., & Malo-Cerrato, S. (2020). Risk of addiction: Its prevalence in adolescence and its relationship with security of attachment and self-concept. Clinica y Salud, 31(1), 2125. https://doi.org/10.5093/clysa2020a1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowell, S. E., Puzia, M. E., & Yaptangco, M. (2015). The ontogeny of chronic distress: Emotion dysregulation across the life span and its implications for psychological and physical health. Current Opinion in Psychology, 3, 9199. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.03.023CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cummings, E. M., Davies, P., & Campbell, S. B. (2000). Developmental psychopathology and family process: Theory, research, and clinical implications. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Darling, N., & Steinberg, L. (1993). Parenting style as context: An integrative model. Psychological Bulletin, 113(3), 487496. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.113.3.487CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eaton, W. W., Muntaner, C., Smith, C., Tien, A., Ybarra, M., & Maruish, M. E. (2004). Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale: Review and revision (CESD and CESD-R). The use of psychological testing for treatment planning and outcomes assessment (pp. 363377). Routledge New York (NY).Google Scholar
Fall, A., & Roberts, G. (2012). High school dropouts: Interactions between social context, self-perceptions, school engagement, and student dropout. Journal of Adolescence, 35(4), 787798. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.11.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 11491160. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, S. S., & Rosenthal, D. A. (2000). The effect of communication characteristics on family members’ perceptions of parents as sex educators. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 10(2), 119150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferguson, C. J. (2016). An effect size primer: A guide for clinicians and researchers. American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/14805-020Google Scholar
Fisher, R. A. (1925). Statistical methods for research workers, 11th ed. rev. Oliver and Boyd.Google Scholar
Fitts, W. H. (1965). Manual for Tennessee Self-concept Scale. Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Fraley, R. C., & Roisman, G. I. (2019). The development of adult attachment styles: Four lessons. Current Opinion in Psychology, 25, 2630. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.02.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuentes, M. C., Alarcón, A., Gracia, E., & Garcia, F. (2015). School adjustment among Spanish adolescents: Influence of parental socialization. [El ajuste escolar en los adolescentes españoles: Influencia de la socialización parental] Cultura y Educacion, 27(1), 132. https://doi.org/10.1080/11356405.2015.1006847CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuentes, M. C., Garcia, F., Gracia, E., & Lila, M. (2011). Self-concept and drug use in adolescence. Adicciones, 23(3), 237248. https://doi.org/10.20882/adicciones.148CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuentes, M. C., Garcia, O. F., Alcaide, M., Garcia-Ros, R., & Garcia, F. (2022). Analyzing when parental warmth but without parental strictness leads to more adolescent empathy and self-concept: Evidence from Spanish homes. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 1060821. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1060821CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuentes, M. C., Garcia, O. F., & Garcia, F. (2020). Protective and risk factors for adolescent substance use in Spain: Self-esteem and other indicators of personal well-being and ill-being. Sustainability, 12(15), 5967. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12155962CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furukawa, T. A., Kessler, R. C., Slade, T., & Andrews, G. (2003). The performance of the K6 and K10 screening scales for psychological distress in the Australian National Survey of Mental Health and well-being. Psychological Medicine, 33(2), 357362. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291702006700CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garaigordobil, M., Ignacio Perez, J., & Mozaz, M. (2008). Self-concept, self-esteem and psychopathological symptoms. Psicothema, 20(1), 114123.Google ScholarPubMed
Garcia, F., Gracia, E., & Zeleznova, A. (2013). Validation of the english version of the five-factor self-concept questionnaire. Psicothema, 25(4), 549555. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2013.33CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garcia, F., Martinez, I., Balluerka, N., Cruise, E., Garcia, O. F., & Serra, E. (2018). Validation of the five-factor self-concept questionnaire AF5 in Brazil: Testing factor structure and measurement invariance across language (Brazilian and Spanish), gender, and age. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2250. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02250CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garcia, F., & Musitu, G. (1999). AF5: Self-concept form 5 [AF5: Autoconcepto Forma 5]. TEA editions.Google Scholar
Garcia, F., Serra, E., Garcia, O. F., Martinez, I., & Cruise, E. (2019). A third emerging stage for the current digital society? Optimal parenting styles in Spain, the United States, Germany, and Brazil. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 16(13), 2333. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16132333CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garcia, J. F., Musitu, G., Riquelme, E., & Riquelme, P. (2011). A confirmatory factor analysis of the “Autoconcepto Forma 5” Questionnaire in young adults from Spain and Chile. Spanish Journal of Psychology, 14, 648658. https://doi.org/10.5209/rev_SJOP.2011.v14.n2.13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garcia, J. F., Musitu, G., & Veiga, F. H. (2006). Self-concept in adults from Spain and Portugal. Psicothema, 18(3), 551556.Google ScholarPubMed
Garcia, J. F., Pascual, J., Frias, M. D., Van Krunckelsven, D., & Murgui, S. (2008). Design and power analysis: n and confidence intervals of means. Psicothema, 20(4), 933938.Google ScholarPubMed
Garcia, O. F., Alcaide, M., Musitu-Ferrer, D., Pons-Benavent, L., & Garcia, F. (2024). Parental socialization based on warmth and strictness among adolescents and young adults: Which parenting dimension is related to greater adjustment? Sage Open, 14(4), 117. https://doi.org/10.1177/21582440241289684CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gimenez-Serrano, S., Garcia, F., & Garcia, O. F. (2022). Parenting styles and its relations with personal and social adjustment beyond adolescence: Is the current evidence enough? European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 19(5), 749769. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2021.1952863CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Good, M. D., Smilkstein, G., Good, B. J., Shaffer, T., & Arons, T. (1979). The Family APGAR Index: A study of construct validity. The Journal of Family Practice, 8(3), 577582. https://cdn.mdedge.com/files/s3fs-public/jfp-archived-issues/1979-volume_8-9/JFP_1979-03_v8_i3_the-family-apgar-index-a-study-of-constr.pdfGoogle Scholar
Gracia, E., Garcia, F., & Musitu, G. (1995). Macrosocial determinants of social integration: Social-class and area effect. Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 5(2), 105119. https://doi.org/10.1002/casp.2450050204CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gross, J. J., & Muñoz, R. F. (1995). Emotion regulation and mental health. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 2(2), 151.Google Scholar
Grusec, J. E. (2002). Parenting socialization and children’s acquisition of values. (pp. 143167). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.Google Scholar
Grusec, J. E., & Lytton, H. (1988). Socialization and the Family. In Grusec, J. E., & Lytton, H. (Eds.), Social Development: History, Theory, and Research (pp. 161212). Springer New York. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-3768-6_5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harter, S. (1988). Developmental processes in the construction of the self. In Yawkey, T. D., & Johnson, J. E. (Eds.), Integrative processes and socialization: Early to middle childhood (pp. 4578). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Herrero, J., & Gracia, E. (2007). Una medida breve de la sintomatología depresiva (CESD-7). Salud Mental, 30(5), 4046.Google Scholar
Jhang, F. (2017). Economically Disadvantaged Adolescents’ Self-Concept and Academic Achievement as Mediators between Family Cohesion and Mental Health in Taiwan. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 15(2), 407422. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-017-9737-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kapetanovic, S., & Skoog, T. (2021). The role of the family’s emotional climate in the links between parent-adolescent communication and adolescent psychosocial functioning. Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 49, 141154. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-020-00705-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keijsers, L., Branje, S. J. T., VanderValk, I. E., & Meeus, W. (2010). Reciprocal effects between parental solicitation, parental control, adolescent disclosure, and adolescent delinquency. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 20(1), 88113. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2009.00631.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keijsers, L., & Poulin, F. (2013). Developmental changes in parent–child communication throughout adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 49(12), 23012308. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032217CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kerr, M., Stattin, H., & Trost, K. (1999). To know you is to trust you: parents’ trust is rooted in child disclosure of information. Journal of Adolescence, 22(6), 737752. https://doi.org/10.1006/jado.1999.0266CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Andrews, G., Colpe, L. J., Hiripi, E., Mroczek, D. K., Normand, S. T., Walters, E. E., & Zaslavsky, A. M. (2002). Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in non-specific psychological distress. Psychological Medicine, 32, 959976. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291702006074CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kganyago, L. (2023). The impact of dysfunctional families on the mental health of children. In Silva, Teresa (Ed.), Parenting in modern societies (pp. 1035). IntechOpen. https://doi.org/10.5772/intechopen.110565CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knopp, K., Rhoades, G. K., Allen, E., Parsons, A., Ritchie, L. L., Markman, H. J., & Stanley, S. M. (2017). Within- and between-family associations of marital functioning and child well-being. Journal of Marriage and Family, 79(2), 451461. https://doi.org/10.1111/jomf.12373CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koerner, A. F., & Fitzpatrick, M. A. (2002). Understanding family communication patterns and family functioning: The roles of conversation orientation and conformity orientation. Annals of the International Communication Association, 26(1), 3665. https://doi.org/10.1080/23808985.2002.11679010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krauss, S., Orth, U., & Robins, R. W. (2020). Family environment and self-esteem development: A longitudinal study from age 10 to 16. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 119(2), 457478. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000263CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamborn, S. D., Mounts, N. S., Steinberg, L., & Dornbusch, S. M. (1991). Patterns of competence and adjustment among adolescents from authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent, and neglectful families. Child Development, 62(5), 10491065. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01588.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lila, M., Garcia, F., & Gracia, E. (2007). Perceived paternal and maternal acceptance and children’s outcomes in Colombia. Social Behavior and Personality, 35(1), 115124. https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2007.35.1.115CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. A. (1983). Socialization in the context of the family: Parent–child interaction. In Mussen, P. H. (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (pp. 1101). Wiley.Google Scholar
Mañez, I., Skrobiszewska, N., Descals, A., Cantero, M. J., Cerdan, R., Garcia, O. F., & Garcia-Ros, R. (2024). Channelling feedback through audiovisual presentations: Do higher education students perceive, use and benefit from video feedback compared to written feedback? Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 40, 18861897. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcal.12993CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsh, H. W., Byrne, B. M., & Shavelson, R. J. (1988). A multifaceted academic self-concept: Its hierarchical structure and its relation to academic achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(3), 366380. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.80.3.366CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsh, H. W., & Craven, R. G. (2006). Reciprocal effects of self-concept and performance from a multidimensional perspective: Beyond seductive pleasure and unidimensional perspectives. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(2), 133163. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00010.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, H. W., & Hau, K. T. (1996). Assessing goodness of fit: Is parsimony always desirable? Journal of Experimental Education, 64(4), 364390. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220973.1996.10806604CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsh, H. W., & Martin, A. J. (2011). Academic self-concept and academic achievement: Relations and causal ordering. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(1), 5977. https://doi.org/10.1348/000709910X503501CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, Herbert, & O’Neill, R. (1984). Self-Description Questionnaire III: The construct validity of multidimensional self-concept ratings by late adolescents. Journal of Educational Measurement, 21(2), 153174. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-3984.1984.tb00227.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martín-Albo, J., Núñez, J. L., Navarro, J. G., & Grijalvo, F. (2007). The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale: Translation and validation in university students. Spanish Journal of Psychology, 10, 458467. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1138741600006727CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin-Blesa, E., Garcia, O. F., Alcaide, M., & Garcia, F. (2024). Parental socialization and its relationship to child adjustment and maladjustment in socialization and beyond. Ansiedad y Estrés, 30(3), 147156. https://doi.org/10.5093/anyes2024a19Google Scholar
Martinez, I., Cruise, E., Garcia, O. F., & Murgui, S. (2017). English validation of the Parental Socialization Scale—ESPA29. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 865. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00865CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martinez, I., Garcia, F., Musitu, G., & Yubero, S. (2012). Family socialization practices: Factor confirmation of the Portuguese version of a scale for their measurement. Revista de Psicodidactica, 17(1), 159178. https://ehu.es/ojs/index.php/psicodidactica/article/view/1306/4034Google Scholar
Martinez, I., Murgui, S., Garcia, O. F., & Garcia, F. (2021). Parenting and adolescent adjustment: The mediational role of family self-esteem. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 30(5), 11841197. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-021-01937-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martinez-Escudero, J. A., Garcia, O. F., Alcaide, M., Bochons, I., & Garcia, F. (2023). Parental socialization and adjustment components in adolescents and middle-aged adults: How are they related? Psychology Research and Behavior Management, 16, 11271139. https://doi.org/10.2147/PRBM.S394557CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martinez-Escudero, J. A., Villarejo, S., Garcia, O. F., & Garcia, F. (2020). Parental socialization and its impact across the lifespan. Behavioral Sciences, 10(6), 101. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs10060101CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murgui, S., Garcia, C., Garcia, A., & Garcia, F. (2012). Self-concept in young dancers and non-practitioners: Confirmatory factor analysis of the AF5 Scale. [Autoconcepto en jóvenes practicantes de danza y no practicantes: Análisis factorial confirmatorio de la escala AF5] Revista de Psicologia del Deporte, 21(2), 263269.Google Scholar
Olson, D. H., Sprenkle, D. H., & Russell, C. S. (1979). Circumplex model of marital and family systems:I. Cohesion and adaptability dimensions, family types, and clinical applications. Family Processes, 18(1), 328. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1545-5300.1979.00003.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olson, D. H., Waldvogel, L., & Schlieff, M. (2019). Circumplex model of marital and family systems: An update. Journal of Family Theory & Review, 11(2), 199211. https://doi.org/10.1111/jftr.12331CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orth, U., Robins, R. W., Widaman, K. F., & Conger, R. D. (2014). Is low self-esteem a risk factor for depression? Findings from a longitudinal study of Mexican-origin youth. Developmental Psychology, 50(2), 622633. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033817CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palacios, I., Garcia, O. F., Alcaide, M., & Garcia, F. (2022). Positive parenting style and positive health beyond the authoritative: Self, universalism values, and protection against emotional vulnerability from Spanish adolescents and adult children. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 1066282. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1066282CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pérez, J. F. G., Navarro, D. F., & Llobell, J. P. (1999). Statistical power of Solomon design. Psicothema, 11(2), 431436.Google Scholar
Piers, E. V., & Harris, D. B. (1964). Age and other correlates of self-concept in children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 55(2), 9195. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0044453CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poston, J. M., & Hanson, W. E. (2010). Meta-analysis of psychological assessment as a therapeutic intervention. Psychological Assessment, 22(2), 203212. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018679CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Powers, M. B., & Emmelkamp, P. M. G. (2008). Virtual reality exposure therapy for anxiety disorders: A meta-analysis. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22(3), 561569. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.04.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (1992). When small effects are impressive. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 160164. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.160CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnick, D. L., & Bornstein, M. H. (2016). Measurement invariance conventions and reporting: The state of the art and future directions for psychological research. Developmental Review, 41, 7190. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2016.06.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1(3), 385401. https://doi.org/10.1177/014662167700100306CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radloff, L. S. (1991). The use of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale in adolescents and young adults. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 20(2), 149166. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01537606CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reyes, M., Garcia, O. F., Perez-Gramaje, A., Serra, E., Melendez, J., Alcaide, M., & Garcia, F. (2023). Which is the optimum parenting for adolescents with low vs. high self-efficacy? Self-concept, psychological maladjustment and academic performance of adolescents in the Spanish context. Anales de Psicologia, 39, 446457. https://doi.org/10.6018/analesps.517741CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rock, P. L., Roiser, J. P., Riedel, W. J., & Blackwell, A. D. (2014). Cognitive impairment in depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 44(10), 20292040. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291713002535CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubach, C., Dicke, A., Lazarides, R., Simpkins, S., & Eccles, J. S. (2020). Addressing adolescents’ depressive symptoms and risky behavior: The role of perceived parents’ and teachersSocial Support. Journal of Organizational Psychology, 20(4)Google Scholar
Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (2001). A scaled difference chi-square test statistic for moment structure analysis. Psychometrika, 66(4), 507514. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02296192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Segrin, C., & Flora, J. (2018). Family communication (3rd ed.). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781351132596CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shavelson, R. J., Hubner, J. J., & Stanton, G. C. (1976). Self-concept: Validation of construct interpretations. Review of Educational Research, 46, 407441. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543046003407CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smetana, J. G., Metzger, A., Gettman, D. C., & Campione-Barr, N. (2006). Disclosure and secrecy in adolescent-parent relationships. Child Development, 77(1), 201217. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00865.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smilkstein, G. (1978). Family Apgar - Proposal for a family function test and its use by physicians. Journal of Family Practice, 6(6), 12311239.Google ScholarPubMed
Smilkstein, G., Ashworth, C., & Montano, D. (1982). Validity and reliability of the Family APGAR as a test of family function. The Journal of Family Practice, 15(2), 303311.Google ScholarPubMed
Sroufe, L. A. (1996). Emotional development: The organization of emotional life in the early years. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511527661CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steinberg, L. (2001). We know some things: Parent-adolescent relationships in retrospect and prospect. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 11, 119. https://doi.org/10.1111/1532-7795.00001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steinberg, L. (2007). Risk taking in adolescence - New perspectives from brain and behavioral science. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(2), 5559. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00475.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steinberg, L., Lamborn, S. D., Darling, N., Mounts, N. S., & Dornbusch, S. M. (1994). Over-time changes in adjustment and competence among adolescents from authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent, and neglectful families. Child Development, 65(3), 754770. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00781.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. S. (2001). Adolescent development. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 83110. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.83CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, J. (1992). Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences (2nd ed.). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tomás, J. M., & Oliver, A. (2004). Confirmatory factor analysis of a Spanish Multidimensional Scale of Self-concept. Revista Interamericana de Psicología, 38(2), 285293.Google Scholar
Vacha-Haase, T., & Thompson, B. (2004). How to estimate and interpret various effect sizes. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 51(4), 473481. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.51.4.473CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Wal, A., Valkenburg, P. M., & van Driel, I. I. (2024). In their own words: How adolescents use social media and how it affects them. Social Media + Society, 10(2), 20563051241248591. https://doi.org/10.1177/20563051241248591CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Dijk, M. P. A., Branje, S., Keijsers, L., Hawk, S. T., Hale, W. W. III, & Meeus, W. (2014). Self-concept clarity across adolescence Longitudinal associations with open communication with parents and internalizing symptoms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 43(11), 18611876. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-013-0055-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vargas-Terrez, B. E., Villamil-Salcedo, V., Rodríguez-Estrada, C., Pérez-Romero, J., & Cortés-Sotres, J. (2011). Validity of the Kessler 10 (k–10) Scale in detecting depression and anxiety disorders in the primary care services. Psychometric Properties, 34(4)Google Scholar
Veiga, F. H., Festas, I., Garcia, O. F., Oliveira, Í M., Veiga, C. M., Martins, C., Covas, F., & Carvalho, N. A. (2023). Do students with immigrant and native parents perceive themselves as equally engaged in school during adolescence? Current Psychology, 42, 1190211916. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-02480-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Villarejo, S., Garcia, O., Alcaide, M., Gonzalez, M. E., & Garcia, F. (2024). Early family experiences, drug use, and psychosocial adjustment across the life span: Is parental strictness always a protective factor? Psychosocial Intervention, 33(1), 1527. https://doi.org/10.5093/pi2023a16CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Villarejo, S., Martinez-Escudero, J. A., & Garcia, O. F. (2020). Parenting styles and their contribution to children personal and social adjustment. Ansiedad y Estrés, 26(1), 18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anyes.2019.12.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waizenhofer, R. N., Buchanan, C. M., & Jackson-Newsom, J. (2004). Mothers’ and fathers’ knowledge of adolescents’ daily activities: Its sources and its links with adolescent adjustment. Journal of Family Psychology, 18(2), 348360. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.18.2.348CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weaver, B., & Koopman, R. (2014). An SPSS macro to compute confidence intervals for Pearson’s correlation. Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 10(1), 2939. https://doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.10.1.p029CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, K. S., Sandman, C. F., & Craske, M. G. (2019). Positive and negative emotion regulation in adolescence: Links to anxiety and depression. Brain Sciences, 9(4)https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9040076CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zapf, H., Boettcher, J., Haukeland, Y., Orm, S., Coslar, S., & Fjermestad, K. (2024). A systematic review of the association between parent-child communication and adolescent mental health. JCPP Advances, 4(2), e12205. https://doi.org/10.1002/jcv2.12205CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zapf, H., Boettcher, J., Haukeland, Y., Orm, S., Coslar, S., Wiegand-Grefe, S., & Fjermestad, K. (2023). A systematic review of parent–child communication measures: Instruments and their psychometric properties. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 26(1), 121142. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-022-00414-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, Y., Wu, W., Gu, S., Cui, L., & Wang, Y. (2024). Longitudinal relationships between perceived social support and social behaviors in preadolescence and early adolescence: The mediating role of social self-concept. Current Psychology: A Journal for Diverse Perspectives on Diverse Psychological Issues. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-024-06448-wGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Confirmatory factor analysis and analysis for the invariance between sex and age groups

Figure 1

Table 2. Confirmatory factor analysis loadings, variances, covariances, and errors in the most constrained model across sex

Figure 2

Table 3. Confirmatory factor analysis loadings, variances, covariances, and errors in the most constrained model across age

Figure 3

Table 4. Correlations (r) between self-concept and parent–child communication, family functioning, psychological distress, and depressive symptoms

Figure 4

Table 5. Cohen’s (d) effect size values between self-concept, and parent–child communication, family functioning, psychological distress, and depressive symptoms

Figure 5

Figure 1. Cohen’s (d) effect size and 95% CI values between mother–child and father–child open communication, communication problems, and avoidant communication with five self-concept dimensions.

Figure 6

Figure 2. Cohen’s (d) effect size and 95% CI values between family functioning, with five self-concept dimensions.

Figure 7

Figure 3. Cohen’s (d) effect size and 95% CI values between psychological distress, and depressive symptoms with five self-concept dimensions.