Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T10:45:45.223Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Executive Functions and Improvement of Thinking: An Intervention Program to Enhance Deductive Reasoning Abilities

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 December 2022

Juan Antonio García-Madruga*
Affiliation:
Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (Spain)
Isabel Orenes
Affiliation:
Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (Spain)
José Óscar Vila Chaves
Affiliation:
Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (Spain)
Isabel Gómez-Veiga
Affiliation:
Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (Spain)
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Juan Antonio García-Madruga. Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia. Facultad de Psicología. Departamento de Psicología Evolutiva y de la Educación. Juan del Rosal, 10. 28040 Madrid (Spain). E-mail: jmadruga@psi.uned.es

Abstract

Empirical and theoretical advances and application to society are moved at different speed. Application work is frequently developed later because it requires the integration of knowledge from different research areas. In the present paper, we integrate literature coming from diverse areas of research in order to design a deductive reasoning intervention, based on the involved executive functions. Executive functions include working memory (WM)’s online executive processes and other off-line functions such as task revising and planning. Deductive reasoning is a sequential thinking process driven by reasoners’ meta-deductive knowledge and goals that requires the construction and manipulation of representations. We present a new theoretical view about the relationship between executive function and higher-level thinking, a critical analysis of the possibilities and limitations of cognitive training, and a metacognitive training procedure on executive functions to improve deductive reasoning. This procedure integrates direct instruction on deduction and meta-deductive concepts (consistency, necessity) and strategies (search for counterexamples and exhaustivity), together with the simultaneous training of WM and executive functions involved: Focus and switch attention, update WM representations, inhibit and revise intuitive responses, and control the emotional stress yielded by tasks. Likewise, it includes direct training of some complex WM tasks that demands people to carry out similar cognitive assignment than deduction. Our training program would be included in the school curriculum and attempts not only to improve deductive reasoning in experimental tasks, but also to increase students’ ability to uncover fallacies in discourse, to automatize some basic logical skills, and to be able to use logical intuitions.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de la Psicología de Madrid

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Acknowledgement: This research was begun by the first author during a sabbatical period visiting Phil Johnson-Laird at Princeton University and Cesare Cornoldi at the Università degli Studi di Padova. We thank both for their ideas and support. A preliminary version was presented in the Thinking Meeting in Memory of Vittorio Girotto, in London in 2016.

Funding statement: This research was conducted with the financial support of Projects EDU2014–56423 and PID2019–110476RB–I00, from the Ministerio de Economía, Industria, y Competividad, and Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación, Gobierno de España.

Conflict of interest: None.

Data sharing: This research does not include any new data.

References

Ackerman, R., & Thompson, V. (2017). Meta-reasoning: Monitoring and control of thinking and reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(8), 607617. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.05.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alvarez, J. A., & Emory, E. (2006). Executive function and the frontal lobes: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology Review, 16(1), 1742. http://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-006-9002-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ariës, R. J., Ghysels, J., Groot, W., & van den Brink, H Maassen. (2017). Does n-back training in a school-based context improve reasoning? A literature review. TIER Working Paper Series 17/08, 117.Google Scholar
Ariës, R. J., Groot, W., & van den Brink, H. M. (2014). Improving reasoning skills in secondary history education by working memory training. British Educational Research Journal, 41(2), 210228. https://doi.org/10.1002/berj.3142CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (2007). Working memory, thought and action. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198528012.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (2010). Long-term and working memory: How do they interact? In Bäckman, L. & Nyberg, L. (Eds.), Memory, aging and the brain: A festschrift in honor of Lars-Göran Nilsson (pp. 1830). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In Bower, G. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8., pp. 4789). Academic Press. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-1Google Scholar
Baggetta, P., & Alexander, P. A. (2016). Conceptualization and operationalization of executive function. Mind, Brain, and Education, 10(1), 1033. https://doi.org/10.1111/mbe.12100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bago, B., & De Neys, W. (2017). Fast logic? Examining the time course assumption of dual process theory. Cognition, 158, 90109. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.10.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bago, B., Raoelison, M., & De Neys, W. (2019). Second-guess: Testing the specificity of error detection in the bat-and-ball problem. Acta Psychologica, 193, 214228. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2019.01.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnes, J. J., Woolrich, M. W., Baker, K., Colclough, G. L., & Astle, D. E. (2016). Electrophysiological measures of resting state functional connectivity and their relationship with working memory capacity in childhood. Developmental Science, 19(1), 1931. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12297CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrouillet, P., & Lecas, J.-F. (1999). Mental models in conditional reasoning and working memory. Thinking & Reasoning, 5(4), 289302. https://doi.org/10.1080/135467899393940CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrouillet, P., Grosset, N., & Lecas, J.-F. (2000). Conditional reasoning by mental models: Chronometric and developmental evidence. Cognition, 75(3), 237266. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00066-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2014). Closing the achievement gap through modification of neurocognitive and neuroendocrine function: Results from a cluster randomized controlled trial of an innovative approach to the education of children in kindergarten. PLoS ONE, 9(11), Article e112393. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0112393CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blakemore, S.-J., & Frith, U. (2005). The learning brain. Lesson for education. Blackwell Publishing.Google ScholarPubMed
Braine, M. D. S., & O’Brien, D. P. (1998). The theory of mental-propositional logic: Description and illustration. In Braine, M. D. S., O’Brien, D. P., & Braine, M. (Eds.), Mental logic (pp.7989). Psychology Press. http://doi.org/10.4324/9781410603005-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camos, V., & Barrouillet, P. (2018). Working memory in domain-specific developmental theories. In Camos, V. & Barrouillet, P. (Eds.), Working memory in development (pp. 5676). Routledge. http://doi.org/10.4324/9781315660851-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Capon, A., Handley, S., & Dennis, I. (2003). Working memory and reasoning: An individual differences perspective. Thinking & Reasoning, 9(3), 203244. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546781343000222CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carretti, B., Borella, E., Elosúa, M. R., Gómez-Veiga, I., & García-Madruga, J. A. (2017). Improvements in reading comprehension performance after a training program focusing on executive processes of working memory. Journal of Cognitive Enhance, 1, 268279. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41465-017-0012-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheng, P. W., Holyoak, K. J., Nisbett, R. E., & Oliver, L. M. (1986). Pragmatic versus syntactic approaches to training deductive reasoning. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 293328. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(86)90002-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowan, N. (1999). An embedded-processes model of working memory. In Miyake, A. & Shah, P. (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 62101). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139174909.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christoforides, M, Spanoudis, G., & Demetriou, A. (2016). Coping with logical fallacies: A developmental training program for learning to reason. Child Development, 87(6), 18561876. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12557CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cummings, J. L. (1993). Frontal subcortical circuits and human behavior. Archives of Neurology, 50, 873880. http://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.1993.00540080076020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450466. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90312-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deary, I. J., Strand, S., Smith, P., & Fernandes, C. (2007). Intelligence and educational achievement. Intelligence, 35, 1321. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2006.02.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Neys, W. (2012). Bias and conflict: A case for logical intuitions. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 2838. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691611429354CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Neys, W. (Ed.). (2018). Dual process theory 2.0. Routledge. http://doi.org/10.4324/9781315204550CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Neys, W., & Glumicic, T. (2008). Conflict monitoring in dual process theories of thinking. Cognition, 106(3), 12481299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.06.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Neys, W., & Pennycook, G. (2019). Logic, fast and slow: Advances in dual-process theorizing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28, 503509. http://doi.org/10.1177/0963721419855658CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135168. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A., & Ling, D. S. (2016). Conclusions about interventions, programs, and approaches for improving executive functions that appear justified and those that, despite much hype, do not. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 3448. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2015.11.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A., & Ling, D. S. (2020). Review of the evidence on, and fundamental questions about, efforts to improve executive functions, including working memory. In Novick, J. M., Bunting, M. F., Dougherty, M. R., & Engle, R. W. (Eds.), Cognitive and working memory training (pp. 143431). Oxford University Press. http://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780199974467.003.0008Google Scholar
Dunning, D. L., Holmes, J., & Gathercole, S. E. (2013). Does working memory training lead to generalized improvements in children with low working memory? A randomized controlled trial. Developmental Science, 16(6), 915925. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12068CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duque de Blas, G., Gómez-Veiga, I., & García-Madruga, J. A. (2021). Arithmetic word problems revisited: Cognitive processes and academic performance in secondary school. Education Sciences, 11(4), Article 155. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11040155CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engle, R. W. (2001). What is working memory capacity? In Roediger, H. L III, Naime, J. S., Neath, I., & Supremant, A. M. (Eds.), The nature of remembering: Essays in honor of Robert G. Crowder (pp. 297314). American Psychological Association. http://doi.org/10.1037/10394-016CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, J., & Over, D. (2004). If. Oxford University Press. http://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198525134.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T. (1989). Biases in human reasoning. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255278. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093629CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J. St. B. T. (2012). How many dual-process theories do we need? One, two, or many? In Evans, J. St. B. T & Frankish, K. (Eds.), In two minds: Dual processes and beyond (pp. 3354). Oxford Academic. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199230167.003.0002 (Original work published 2009).Google Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T., & Lynch, J. S. (1973). Matching bias in the selection task. British Journal of Psychology, 64, 391397. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1973.tb01365.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 223241. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612460685CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fangmeier, T., Knauff, M., Ruff, C. C., & Sloutsky, V. (2006). fMRI evidence for a three-stage model of deductive reasoning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(3), 320334. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.3.320CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19(4), 2542. https://doi.org/10.1257/089533005775196732CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuster, J. M. (1989). The prefrontal cortex: Anatomy, physiology and neuropsychology of the frontal lobe (2nd Ed.). Raven Press.Google Scholar
García-Madruga, J. A. (1982). Razonamiento silogístico e interpretación de premisas: Un estudio evolutivo [Syllogistic reasoning and interpretation of premises: A developmental study]. Journal for the Study of Education and Development, 5, 175204. http://doi.org/10.1080/02103702.1982.10821951CrossRefGoogle Scholar
García Madruga, J. A. (1983). Un modelo general sobre el razonamiento silogístico: Doble procesamiento y fase de comprobación con verificación [A general model on syllogistic reasoning: double processing and verification phase]. Revista de Psicología General y Aplicada, 38(3), 439466.Google Scholar
García-Madruga, J. A., Elosúa, M. R., Gil, L., Gómez-Veiga, I., Vila, J. Ó., Orjales, I., Contreras, A., Rodríguez, R., Melero, M. A., & Duque, G. (2013). Reading comprehension and working memory’s executive processes: An intervention study in primary school students. Reading Research Quarterly, 48(82), 155174. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.44CrossRefGoogle Scholar
García-Madruga, J. A., Gómez-Veiga, I., & Vila, J. O. (2016). Executive functions and the improvement of thinking abilities: The intervention in reading comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, Article 58. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00058CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
García-Madruga, J. A., Gutiérrez, F., Carriedo, N., Luzón, J. M., & Vila, J. O. (2007). Mental models in propositional reasoning and working memory’s central executive. Thinking & Reasoning, 13(4), 370393. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546780701203813CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Dunning, D. L., Holmes, J., & Norris, D. (2019). Working memory training involves learning new skills. Journal of Memory and Language, 105, 1942. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2018.10.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilhooly, K. J., Logie, R. H., & Wynn, V. (1999). Syllogistic reasoning tasks, working memory, and skill. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 11(4), 473498. https://doi.org/10.1080/095414499382264CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gobet, F., & Sala, G. (2022). Cognitive training: A field in search of a phenomenon. Psychological Science. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916221091830Google Scholar
Gómez-Chacón, I. M., García-Madruga, J. A., Vila, J. S., Elosúa, M. R., & Rodríguez, R. (2014). The dual processes hypothesis in mathematics performance: Beliefs, cognitive reflection, working memory and reasoning. Learning and Individual Differences, 29, 6773. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2013.10.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gómez-Veiga, I., Vila Chaves, J. O., Duque, G., & García-Madruga, J. A. (2018). A new look to a classic issue: Reasoning and academic achievement at secondary school. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 400. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00400CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottfried, A. E. (2019). Academic intrinsic motivation. Theory, assessment and longitudinal research. Advances in Motivation Science, 6, 71109. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.adms.2018.11.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houdé, O., & Borst, G. (2015). Evidence for an inhibitory-control theory of the reasoning brain. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, Article 148. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00148CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Houdé, O., Zago, L., Mellet, E., Moutier, S., Pineau, A., Mazoyer, B., & Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2000). Shifting from the perceptual brain to the logical brain: The neural impact of cognitive inhibition training. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 721728. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892900562525CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Im-Bolter, N., Johnson, J., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2006). Processing limitations in children with specific language impairment: The role of executive function. Child Development, 77, 18221841. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00976.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1955). De la logique de l’enfant à la logique de l’adolescent [From the logic of the child to the logic of the adolescent]. Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1988). A taxonomy of thinking. In Sternberg, R. J. & Smith, E. E. (Eds.), The psychology of human thought (pp. 429457). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1990). The development of reasoning ability. In Butterworth, G. & Bryant, P. (Eds.), Causes of development: Interdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 85110). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2012). How we reason. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199551330.001.0001 (Original published in 2008).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2015). How to improve thinking. In Wegerif, R., Li, L., & Kaufman, J. C. (Eds.), The Routledge international handbook of research on teaching thinking. Routledge.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N., & Byrne, R. M. (1991). Deduction. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N., Byrne, R. M., & Schaeken, W. (1992). Propositional reasoning by model. Psychological Review, 99(3), 418439. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.99.3.418CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jurado, M. B., & Rosselli, M. (2007). The elusive nature of executive functions: A review of our current understanding. Neuropsychology Review, 17(3), 213233. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-007-9040-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. Farrar, Straus & Giroux. https://doi.org/10.1086/674372Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2002). Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. In Gilovich, T., Griffin, D., & Kahneman, D. (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment (pp. 4981). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511808098.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karbach, J., & Unger, K. (2014). Executive control training from middle childhood to adolescence. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 390. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00390CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karbach, J., & Verhaeghen, P. (2014). Making working memory work: A meta-analysis of executive-control and working memory training in older adults. Psychological Science, 25, 20272203. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614548725CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khemlani, S. S., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2017). Illusions in reasoning. Minds & Machines, 27, 1135. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11023-017-9421-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension. A paradigm for cognition. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Klenberg, L., Korkman, M., & Lahti-Nuuttila, P. (2001). Differential development of attention and executive functions in 3- to 12-year-old finish children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20, 407428. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2001_6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klingberg, T., Fernell, E., Olesen, P. J., Johnson, M., Gustafsson, P., Dahlström, K., Gillberg, C. G., Forssberg, H., & Westerberg, H. (2005). Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD—A randomized, controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(2), 177186. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200502000-00010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klingberg, T. (2010). Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 317324. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.05.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lezak, M. D. (1982). The problem of assessing executive functions. International Journal of Psychology, 17, 281297. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207598208247445CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1966). Human brain and psychological processes. Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Markovits, H. (2000). A mental model analysis of young children’s conditional reasoning with meaningful premises. Thinking & Reasoning, 6(4), 335347. https://doi.org/10.1080/135467800750038166CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markovits, H., & Barouillet, P. (2002). The development of conditional reasoning: A mental model account. Developmental Review, 22, 536. https://doi.org/10.1006/drev.2000.0533CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markovits, H., Thompson, V. A., & Brisson, J. (2014). Metacognition and abstract reasoning. Memory and Cognition, 42, 681693. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0488-9Google Scholar
Melby-Lervâg, M., & Hulme, C. I. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 49(2), 270291. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028228CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melby-Lervåg, M., Redick, T. S., & Hulme, C. (2016). Working memory training does not improve performance on measures of intelligence or other measures of “far transfer”: Evidence from a meta-analytic review. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(4), 512534. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616635612CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzer, L., Pollica, L. S., & Barzillai, M. (2007). Executive function in the classroom: Embedding strategy instruction into daily teaching practices. In Meltzer, L. (Ed.), Executive function in education: From theory to practice (pp. 165193). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wagner, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moreau, D., & Conway, A. R. A. (2014). The case for an ecological approach to cognitive training. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 334336. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.03.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moshman, D. (1990). The development of metalogical understanding. In Overton, W. F. (Ed.), Reasoning, necessity and logic: Developmental perspectives (pp. 205225). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Moshman, D. (2011). Adolescent rationality and development: Cognition, morality, and identity (3rd Ed.). Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newell, A. (1990). Unified theories of cognition. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., Fong, G. T., Lehman, D. R., & Cheng, P. W. (1987). Teaching reasoning. Science, 238, 625631. http://doi.org/10.1126/science.3672116CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oaksford, M., & Chater, N. (2003). Conditional probability and the cognitive science of conditional reasoning. Mind and Language, 18, 359379. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-0017.00232CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oaksford, M., & Chater, N. (2020). New paradigms in the psychology of reasoning. Annual Review of Psychology, 71, 305330. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010419-051132CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oberauer, K., & Wilhelm, O. (2003). The meaning(s) of conditionals: Conditional probabilities, mental models, and personal utilities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 29, 680–93. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.29.4.680Google ScholarPubMed
Pennycook, G., Fugelsang, J. A., & Koehler, D. J. (2015). What makes us think? A three-stage dual-process model of analytic engagement. Cognitive Psychology, 80, 3472. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2015.05.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pólya, G. (1945). How to solve it? Princeton University Press. http://doi.org/10.1515/9781400828678CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rips, L. J. (1994). The psychology of proof: Deductive reasoning in human reasoning. Choice Reviews Online, 32(02), 321231. https://doi.org/10.5860/choice.32-1231Google Scholar
Rojas-Barahona, C. A., Moreno-Ríos, S., & García-Madruga, J. A. (2010). Desarrollo del razonamiento deductivo: Diferencias entre condicionales fácticos y contrafácticos [Development of deductive reasoning: Differences between factual and counterfactual conditionals]. Psicológica, 31(1), 124.Google Scholar
Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2017). Working memory training in typically developing children: A meta-analysis of the available evidence. Developmental Psychology, 53(4), 671685. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000265CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2020). Working memory training in typically developing children: A multilevel meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27, 423434. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-019-01681-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sánchez-Pérez, N., Castillo, A., López-López, J. A., Pina, V., Puga, J. L., Campoy, G., González-Salinas, C., & Fuentes, L. J. (2018). Computer-based training in math and working memory improves cognitive skills and academic achievement in primary school children: Behavioral results. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, Article 2327. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02327CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Santamaría, C., Tse, P. P., Moreno-Ríos, S., & García-Madruga, J. A. (2013). Deductive reasoning and metalogical knowledge in preadolescence: A mental model appraisal. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(2), 192200. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2012.743988CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sirin, S. R. (2005). Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: A meta-analytic review of research. Review of Educational Research, 75, 417453. http://doi.org/10.3102/00346543075003417Google Scholar
Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127190. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.127CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 322. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.119.1.3Google Scholar
Soveri, A., Antfolk, J., Karlsson, L., Salo, B., & Laine, M. (2017). Working memory training revisited: A multi-level meta-analysis of n-back training studies. Psychonomic Bulletin Review, 24, 10771096. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1217-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanovich, K. E., West, R. F., & Toplak, M. E. (2011). The complexity of developmental predictions from dual process models. Developmental Review, 31(2), 103118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2011.07.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strobach, T., & Karbach, J. (2016). Cognitive training. An overview of features and applications. Springer Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-42662-4Google Scholar
Swanson, H. L. (2015). Cognitive strategy interventions improve word problem solving and working memory in children with math disabilities. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 1099. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01099CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, V. A. (2012). Dual process theories: A metacognitive perspective. In Evans, J. St. B. T. & Frankish, K. (Ed.), In two minds: Dual processes and beyond (pp. 171195). Oxford Academic. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199230167.003.0008 (Original work published 2009).Google Scholar
Thompson, V. A., Prowse Turner, J. A., & Pennycook, G. (2011). Intuition, reason, and metacognition. Cognitive Psychology, 63, 107140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2011.06.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Titz, C., & Karbach, J. (2014). Working memory and executive functions: Effects of training on academic achievement. Psychological Research, 78(6), 852868. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-013-0537-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. W. (2007). The nature of individual differences in working memory capacity: Active maintenance in primary memory and controlled search from secondary memory. Psychological Review, 114(1), 104132. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033–295X.114.1.104CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Velasco, J., & García-Madruga, J. A. (1997). La búsqueda de contraejemplos como estrategia metalógica: Un estudio sobre la influencia de su entrenamiento en el razonamiento silogístico [Searching for counterexamples as metalogical strategy: A study on the influence of training on syllogistic reasoning]. Journal for the Study of Education and Development, 80, 1735. http://doi.org/10.1174/021037097761396144CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verschueren, N., Schaeken, W., & d’Ydewalle, G. (2005). A dual-process specification of causal reasoning. Thinking & Reasoning, 11, 239278. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546780442000178Google Scholar
von Bastian, C. C., & Oberauer, K. (2014). Effects and mechanisms of working memory training: A review. Psychological Research, 78, 803820. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-013-0524-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, P. D., Carter, A., Reznick, J. S., & Frye, D. (1997). Early development of executive function: A problem-solving framework. Review of General Psychology, 1(2), 198226. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.1.2.198CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelazo, P. D., Müller, U., Frye, D., Marcovitch, S., Argitis, G., Boseovski, J., Chiang, J. K., Hongwanishkul, D., Schuster, B. V., & Sutherland, A. (2003). The development of executive function in early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68(3), vii–137. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.0037-976x.2003.00260.xGoogle ScholarPubMed