Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T05:32:30.671Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Executive Functions Influence on Memory Process in Patients with Paranoid Schizophrenia and Bipolar Disorders with and without Psychotic Symptoms. A Pilot Study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2021

Eduardo García-Laredo*
Affiliation:
Universidad Internacional de La Rioja (Spain)
Miguel Ángel Castellanos
Affiliation:
Universidad Complutense (Spain)
Esperanza Badaya
Affiliation:
The University of Edinburgh (Scotland)
Nuria Paúl
Affiliation:
Universidad Complutense (Spain)
Raquel Yubero
Affiliation:
Universidad Complutense (Spain)
Fernando Maestú
Affiliation:
Universidad Politécnica de Madrid (Spain)
Juan D. Molina
Affiliation:
Universidad Francisco de Vitoria (Spain)
José Chacón
Affiliation:
Universidad Complutense (Spain)
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Eduardo García-Laredo. Universidad Internacional de La Rioja. Psicología General Sanitaria. E-mail: eduardoglaredo@gmail.com

Abstract

The objective of the present study was to evaluate whether declarative memory deficits are related to executive function deficits (EF), since they could be a consequence of a poor organization of the material to memorize. This interaction between both cognitive processes can be studied simultaneously in a single task such as the Test of Memory Strategies (TSM). 23 patients with paranoid schizophrenic disorder, 11 with bipolar disorder with psychotic symptoms, 13 with bipolar disorder without psychotic symptoms and 15 healthy subjects were evaluated with the TSM; with the memory test Texts A and B (subtest of the Barcelona neuropsychological assessment battery), which assesses short-term and immediate recall without the influence of EF; and with the Trail Making Test (TMT): Part A (sustained attention) and Part B (executive control). The patients groups and the control group showed an improvement in memory performance across each of the TSM conditions. However, this facilitating effect of the strategies differed among the groups (the patients with higher EF deficits showed less improvement). Regarding these results, we conclude that this cognitive process cannot be independent of EF. However, due to the pilot nature of this study, it would be recommended to replicate these findings in new studies.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Conflicts of Interest: None.

Funding Statement: This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors

References

Abellán-Martínez, M., Castellanos López, M. Á., Delgado-Losada, M. L., Yubero, R., Paúl, N., & Maestú Unturbe, F. (2019). Executive control on memory test performance across life: Test of Memory Strategies. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 22, E50. https://doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2019.47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Algarabel, S. (1996). Índices de interés psicolingüístico de 1.917 palabras castellanas. [Indexes of psycholinguistic interest of 1,917 Spanish words]. Cognitiva, 8, 4388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amann, B., Gomar, J. J., Ortiz-Gil, J., McKenna, P., Sans-Sansa, B., Sarró, S., Moro, N., Madre, M., Landin-Romero, R., Vieta, E., Goikolea, J. M., Salvador, R., & Pomarol-Clotet, E. (2012). Executive dysfunction and memory impairment in schizoaffective disorder: A comparison with bipolar disorder, schizophrenia and healthy controls. Psychological Medicine, 42(10), 21272135. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291712000104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association (APA). (2002). Manual Diagnóstico y Estadístico de los Trastornos Mentales (4º ed., text rev.). [Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4º ed., text rev.)]. Masson.Google Scholar
Arbuthnott, K., & Frank, J. (2000). Trail Making Test, Part B as a measure of executive control: Validation using a set-switching paradigm. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 22(4), 518528. https://doi.org/10.1076/1380-3395(200008)22:4;1-0;FT518.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. (1996). Exploring the central executive. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 49(1), 528. https://doi.org/10.1080/713755608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 417423. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01538-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. (2002). Fractionating the central executive. In Stuss, D. T. & Knight, R. T. (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (p. 246260). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195134971.003.0016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balanzá-Martínez, V., & Tabarés-Seisdedos, R. (2009). Neuropsicología [Neuropsychology]. In Palomo, T. & Jiménez-Arriero, M. A. (Eds.), Manual de Psiquiatría [Psychiatry manual] (pp. 195205). Ene Life.Google Scholar
Berberian, A. A., Trevisan, B. T., Moriyama, T. S., Montiel, J. M., Oliveira, J. A. C., & Seabra, A. G. (2009). Working memory assessment in schizophrenia and its correlation with executive functions ability. Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria, 31(3), 219226. https://doi.org/10.1590/s1516-44462009000300007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berberian, A. A., Moraes, G. V., Gadelha, A., Brietzke, E., Fonseca, A. O., Scarpato, B. S., Vicente, M. O., Seabra, A. G., Bressan, R. A., & Lacerda, A. L. (2016). Is semantic verbal fluency impairment explained by executive function deficits in schizophrenia? Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria, 38(2), 121126. https://doi.org/10.1590/1516-4446-2015-1663.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bora, E., Vahip, S., Akdeniz, F., Ilerisoy, H., Aldemir, E., & Alkan, M. (2008). Executive and verbal working memory dysfunction in first-degree relatives of patients with bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Research, 161(3), 318324. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2007.09.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bora, E., Yücel, M., & Pantelis, C. (2010). Cognitive impairment in schizophrenia and affective psychoses: Implications for DSM–V criteria and beyond. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36(1), 3642. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbp094.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caixeta, L., Soares, V. L. D., Vieira, R. T., Soares, C. D., Caixeta, V., Ferreira, S. B., & Aversi-Ferreira, T. A. (2017). Executive function is selectively impaired in old age bipolar depression. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, Article 194. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cavanagh, J. T. O., van Beck, M., Muir, W., & Blackwood, D. H. R. (2002). Case-control study of neurocognitive function in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder: An association with mania. British Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 320326. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.180.4.320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, L., Iversen, S. D., & Goodwin, G. M. (2002). Sustained attention deficit in bipolar disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 313319. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.180.4.313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernandes, S. M., Araújo, A. M., Vázquez-Justo, E., Pereira, C., Silva, A., Paul, N., Yubero, R., & Maestú, F. (2018). Effects of aging on memory strategies: a validation of the Portuguese version of the Test of Memory Strategies. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 32(sup1), 133151. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2018.1490456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuster, J.M. (2009). Cortex and memory: Emergence of a new paradigm. Journal of Ccognitive Neuroscience, 21(11), 20472072. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gjerde, P. F. (1983). Attentional capacity dysfunction and arousal in schizophrenia. Psychological Bulletin, 93(1), 5772. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.93.1.57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartman, M., Steketee, M. C., Silva, S., Lanning, K., & McCann, H. (2003). Working memory and schizophrenia: Evidence for slowed encoding. Schizophrenia Research, 59(2–3), 99113. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0920-9964(01)00366-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koechlin, E., Basso, G., Pietrini, P., Panzer, S., & Grafman, J. (1999). The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition. Nature, 399(6732), 148151. https://doi.org/10.1038/20178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Konopaske, G. T., Lange, N., Coyle, J. T., & Benes, F. M. (2014). Prefrontal cortical dendritic spine pathology in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. JAMA Psychiatry, 71(12), 13231331. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.1582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lencz, T., Bilder, R. M., Turkel, E., Goldman, R. S., Robinson, D., Kane, J. M., & Lieberman, J. A. (2003). Impairments in perceptual competency and maintenance on a visual delayed match-to-sample test in first-episode schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(3), 238243. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.60.3.238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logan, J. M., Sanders, A. L., Snyder, A. Z., Morris, J. C., & Buckner, R. L. (2002). Under-recruitment and nonselective recruitment: Dissociable neural mechanisms associated with aging. Neuron, 33(5), 827840. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00612-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logie, R. H. (2003). Spatial and visual working memory: A mental workspace. In Irwin, D. E. & Ross, B. H. (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory: Cognitive vision (Vol. 42, pp. 3778). Academic Press Inc. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(03)01002-8.Google Scholar
Maestú, F., Simos, P. G., Campo, P., Fernández, A., Amo, C., Paul, N., González-Marqués, J., & Ortiz, T. (2003). Modulation of brain magnetic activity by different verbal learning strategies. NeuroImage, 20(2), 11101121. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00309-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manoach, D. S., Schlaug, G., Siewert, B., Darby, D. G., Bly, B. M., Benfield, A., Edelman, R. R., & Warach, S. (1997). Prefrontal cortex fMRI signal changes are correlated with working memory load. Neuroreport, 8(2), 545549. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-199701200-00033.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Major, A. J., Vijayraghavan, S., & Everling, S. (2015). Muscarinic attenuation of mnemonic rule representation in macaque dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during a pro- and anti-saccade task. The Journal of Neuroscience, 35(49), 1606416076. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2454-15.2015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martínez-Arán, A., Goodwin, G., & Vieta, E. (2001). El mito de la ausencia de disfunciones cognitivas en el trastorno bipolar. [The myth of the absence of cognitive dysfunctions in bipolar disorder]. In Vieta, E. (Ed.), Trastornos bipolares: Avances clínicos y terapéuticos [Bipolar disorders: Clinical and therapeutic progress]. (pp. 4981). Panamericana.Google Scholar
Peña-Casanova, J. (1990). Programa integrado de exploración neuropsicológica. Manual. [Integrated program for neuropsychological examination. Manual]. Masson.Google Scholar
Perlstein, W. M., Carter, C. S., Noll, D. C., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Relation of prefrontal cortex dysfunction to working memory and symptoms in schizophrenia. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(7), 11051113. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.158.7.1105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reitan, R. M. (1958). Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indication of organic brain damage. Percept Motor Skills, 8, 271276. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1958.8.3.271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1985). The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and Interpretattion. Neuropsychology Press.Google Scholar
Robinson, L. J., Thompson, J. M., Gallagher, P., Goswami, U., Young, A. H., Ferrier, I. N., & Moore, P. B. (2006). A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 93(1–3), 105115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2006.02.016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savage, C. R., Deckersbach, T., Heckers, S., Wagner, A. D., Schacter, D. L., Alpert, N. M., Fischman, A. J., & Rauch, S. L. (2001). Prefrontal regions supporting spontaneous and directed application of verbal learning strategies: Evidence from PET. Brain, 124, 219231. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/124.1.219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shimamura, A. P. (2000). The role of the prefrontal cortex in dynamic filtering. Psychobiology, 28(2), 207218. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03331979.Google Scholar
Shimamura, A. P. (2002). Memory retrieval and executive control processes. In Stuss, D.T. & Knight, R.T. (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 210220). Oxford University Press. http://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195134971.003.0013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science, 283(5408), 16571661. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.283.5408.1657.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tek, C., Gold, J., Blaxton, T., Wilk, C., McMahon, R. P., & Buchanan, R. W. (2002). Visual perceptual and working memory impairments in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(2), 146153. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.59.2.146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, J. M., Hamilton, C. J., Gray, J. M., Quinn, J. G., Mackin, P., Young, A. H., & Ferrier, I. N. (2006). Executive and visuospatial sketchpad resources in euthymic bipolar disorder: Implications for visuospatial working memory architecture. Memory (Hove, England), 14(4), 437451. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210500464293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trapp, W., Dotterweich, S., Hintner, L., Wollny, H., Lautenbacher, S., & Hajak, G. (2017). Speed and capacity of working memory and executive function in schizophrenia compared to unipolar depression. Schizophrenia Research: Cognition, 10, 16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2017.07.002.Google ScholarPubMed
Vázquez, C., Ochoa, E., Fuentenebro, F., & Santos, J. L. (1989). Cognición esquizofrenia. Memoria, pensamiento y lenguaje. [Cognition schizophrenia. Memory, thought and language]. Monografías de Psiquiatría 1, 2631.Google Scholar
Wongupparaj, P., Kumari, V., & Morris, R. G. (2015). Executive function processes mediate the impact of working memory impairment on intelligence in schizophrenia. European Psychiatry, 30(1), 17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2014.06.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yubero, R., Gil, P., Paul, N., & Maestú, F. (2011). Influence of memory strategies on memory test performance: A study in healthy and pathological aging. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 18(5), 497515. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2011.597840.CrossRefGoogle Scholar