Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T06:35:09.149Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Lerdahl and Jackendoff's Grouping Structure Rules in the Performance of a Hindemith Sonata

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 January 2013

Jose A. Ordoñana*
Affiliation:
Universidad Pública de Navarra (Spain) Universidad del País Vasco (Spain)
Ana Laucirica
Affiliation:
Universidad Pública de Navarra (Spain)
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jose A. Ordoñana. Departamento de Psicología y Pedagogía. Facultad de Ciencias Humanas y Sociales. Universidad Pública de Navarra. Campus de Arrosadía s/n. 31006 Pamplona. Navarra. (Spain). Phone: +34-948169245. Fax: +34-948169891. E-mail: jose.ordonana@unavarra.es

Abstract

In the last decades, musical cognitive psychology has intervened to build a bridge towards the comprehension of musical structures. The present paper studies the behavior of several students and music professionals in relation to grouping structure and it does so through the performance analysis of the first movement of Hindemith's sonata for flute and piano, according to the rules formulated by Lerdahl and Jackendoff, movement chosen because of its tonal ambiguity. The participants of this experiment are three medium level female Conservatory students. They performed the aforementioned piece after studying it for a whole trimester. The performance of the same piece by three professional musicians was also analyzed, using the appropriate commercial recordings for this purpose. This analysis includes the choice of group limits used in performance by both professionals and students and the comparison of both groups' results. Differences were observed in the placement of said limits by the different groups but all of them respected the set of rules formulated by Lerdahl and Jackendoff.

En las últimas décadas la psicología cognitiva de la música ha intervenido en el acercamiento hacia la comprensión de las estructuras musicales. Este trabajo estudia el comportamiento de algunos estudiantes y profesionales de la música en relación con la estructura de agrupación. Se trata de un análisis de la interpretación del primer movimiento de la sonata para flauta y piano de Hindemith según las reglas formuladas por Lerdahl y Jackendoff, movimiento elegido por su ambigüedad tonal. En este experimento participan tres alumnas de grado medio de conservatorio. Las alumnas interpretarán la obra mencionada después de haberla estudiado durante un trimestre completo. Además, se analiza la interpretación de la misma obra por parte de tres profesionales, utilizando para ello las grabaciones comerciales correspondientes. Se analizan los límites de grupo realizados en la interpretación, tanto por parte de las alumnas como de los profesionales y, a continuación, se comparan los resultados entre los grupos. Se aprecian diferencias en cuanto a los puntos elegidos para la colocación de los límites, pero todos ellos pueden encuadrarse dentro de las reglas formuladas por Lerdahl y Jackendoff.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, J. (2000). A provisional history of spectral music. Contemporary Music Review, 19, 722.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Best, J. B. (2001). Psicología Cognitiva. Madrid: Paraninfo.Google Scholar
Cambouropoulos, E. (2006). Musical parallelism and melodic segmentation: A computational approach. Music Perception, 23, 249267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague: MoutonCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, G., & Meyer, L. B. (1960). The rhythmic structure of music. Chicago. University of Chicago.Google Scholar
De la Motte-Haber, H. (1994). Principales théories scientifiques en psychologie de la musique: les paradigmes. In Zenatti, A. (Ed.), Psychologie de la musique (pp. 2753). Paris. Presses Universitaires de France.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deliège, C. (2005). Sources et ressources d'analyses musicales. Liège, Belgique: Mardaga.Google Scholar
Deliège, I. (1987). Grouping conditions in listening to music: An approach to Lerdahl and Jackendoff's grouping preference rules. Music Perception, 4, 325360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deutsch, D. (1994). La perception des structures musicales. In Zenatti, A. (Ed.), Psychologie de la musique (pp. 115144). Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drake, C., & Palmer, C. (1993). Accent structures in music performance. Music Perception, 10, 343378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frankland, B. W., & Cohen, A. J. (2004). Parsing of melody: Quantification and testing of the local grouping rules of Lerdahl and Jackendoff's A Generative Theory of Tonal Music. Music Perception, 21, 499543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goetz, J. P., & LeCompte, M. D. (1988). Etnografía y diseño cualitativo en investigación educativa. Madrid: Morata.Google Scholar
Imberty, M. (2003). Estilo y pensamiento caótico. In Martínez, I. C. & Mauléon, C.. (Eds.), Actas de la tercera reunión de la Sociedad Argentina para las Ciencias Cognitivas de la Música. CDRom. La Plata, Buenos Aires.Google Scholar
Imberty, M. (2005). La musique creuse le temps. París. L'Harmattan.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R., & Lerdahl, F. (2006). The capacity for music: What is it, and what's special about it? Cognition, 100, 3372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of Gestalt psychology. New York: Harcourt, Brace and World.Google Scholar
Krumhansl, C. L. (1996). A perceptual analysis of Mozart's piano sonata K. 282: Segmentation, tension and musical ideas. Music Perception, 13, 401432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laucirica, A. (2004). Discriminación tonal e interválica en la percepción general y en el oído absoluto. Revista Mexicana de Psicología, 21, 8392.Google Scholar
Lerdahl, F. (1989). Contraintes cognitives sur les systèmes compositionnels. Actes du Symposium “Composition et Perception Musicales”. Université de Genève, March, 1987. Contrechamps, 10, 2557.Google Scholar
Lerdahl, F., & Jackendoff, R. (1983). A generative theory of tonal music. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Marsden, A. (2005). Generative structural representation of tonal music. Journal of New Music Research, 34, 409428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, R. P. (1991). Twentieth-century music. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Narmour, E. (1991). The top-down and bottom-up systems of musical implication: Building on Meyer's theory of emotional syntax. Music Perception, 9, 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neuhaus, C., & Knösche, T. R. (2006). Processing of rhythmic and melodic Gestalts. An ERP study. Music Perception, 24, 209222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pineau, M., & Tillmann, B. (2001). Percevoir la musique: une activité cognitive. Paris: L'Harmattan.Google Scholar
Rampal, J. P., & Veyron-Lacroix, R. (1992). P. Hindemith. Sonate pour flûte et piano en ré majeur. (Digital recording from an original analogic recording from 1957). In 20th Century Flute Master Pieces, Compact Disc 2. Erato Disques.Google Scholar
Schenker, H. (1993). L'Écriture libre. Vol. 1: Textes. Liège: Mardaga.Google Scholar
Schulz, W., & Bognár, F. (1996). Sonata for flute and piano. (Recording from 1994). In Hindemith Kleine Kammermusik. Sonaten. Sony Classical.Google Scholar
Válek, J., & Hála, J. (1995). Sonata for flute and piano. (Recording from 1978). In Twentieth-century flute sonatas. Supraphon Records.Google Scholar