Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T05:07:38.315Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Functional MRI and parental responsiveness: a new avenue into parental psychopathology and early parent-child interactions?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Sarah Squire*
Affiliation:
Section of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry University of Oxford, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, UK
Alan Stein
Affiliation:
Section of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry University of Oxford, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, UK
*
Sarah Squire, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Warneford Hospital, Oxford OX3 7JX, UK. Tel: 01865 223922; e-mail: sarah.squire@psych.ox.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Editorial
Copyright
Copyright © 2003 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

The advent of functional neuroimaging techniques has led to great advances in our understanding of some of the biological aspects of psychiatric disorders. Tools such as positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) allow us to complement research from genetic, animal and clinical studies by providing information about the patterns of brain activation underlying particular behaviours, and have even begun to challenge current theories and treatments. For example, the finding that hypoperfusion of Broca's area occurs during symptom provocation in post-traumatic stress disorder suggests that these patients might find it difficult to label and describe their emotions; ‘talking therapies’ might therefore be of limited value during some phases of post-traumatic stress disorder (Reference HullHull, 2002).

However, certain fields of research, despite an extensive ‘non-imaging’ literature, appear so far to have escaped the neuroimaging revolution. One field of great relevance to psychiatry is that of parental responsiveness and parent–child interactions. At first glance, the lack of research in this area may be explained by the apparent absurdity of trying to study these issues within, for example, the confines of an MRI scanner. However, on closer inspection, relatively straightforward functional neuroimaging studies could be conducted that would help to illuminate the neural processes underlying parental emotional responses to children. Furthermore, because distortions from ‘normal’ interactions (especially in the context of parental psychiatric disorder) might adversely influence child development, it is important that this line of research be pursued.

SENSITIVE PARENT-CHILD INTERACTIONS

Good quality parent–infant interactions require reciprocity. One example of reciprocity can be observed in a ‘protoconversation’, where the mother and infant demonstrate turn-taking patterns of vocalisations (we refer to ‘the mother’ merely because most of the research has been conducted with mothers). The responses of each partner are contingent on those of the other, and a matching of infant and maternal emotional expressions occurs. These early types of communication are thought to promote language development and emotional regulation in infants. Infants as young as 2 months of age are sensitive to the timing and emotion of maternal expressions, and become confused and distressed if the mother is instructed to keep a still face and ignore the infant's emotional cues (Reference Trevarthen and AitkenTrevarthen & Aitken, 2001).

Of particular concern to clinicians are cases where mothers fail to respond sensitively to infants' emotional cues. For example, mothers with postnatal depression may show intrusive or withdrawn behaviour in interactions with their infants. These impaired interactions have been shown to be associated with adverse effects on the child's later socio-emotional and cognitive development (Reference Murray and CooperMurray & Cooper, 1997).

Electroencephalographic studies have further revealed that infants of depressed mothers often show asymmetries in frontal activation, which may affect their social, attentional and emotional regulation development (Reference Dawson, Ashman and NelsonDawson & Ashman, 2000).

RECOGNITION AND RESPONSE TO INFANT CUES RESPONSE

At the simplest level, mother–child interactions are built up from the mother and the infant recognising and responding to each other. It is the parental recognition of, and emotional response to, infant cues that we propose could be studied using fMRI. Techniques based on simple infant responses (e.g. non-nutritive sucking to elicit the presentation of a particular stimulus) have revealed that even 2-day-old infants recognise their mother's face, voice and odour of her breast milk (Reference Bremner, Slater and ButterworthBremner et al, 1997; Reference Porter and WinbergPorter & Winberg, 1999). This recognition is reciprocal; mothers can recognise their infants by sight, by their cry, by smell and even touch within a few hours of birth (Reference Kaitz, Lapidot and BronnerKaitz et al, 1992). If mothers and infants are predisposed to attend to sensory cues from one another, it might be expected that there could be a biological basis for this recognition. Papoušek (Reference Papoušek, Osofsky and Fitzgerald2000) noted that some maternal responses to infant cues occur so quickly (within 200–400 ms) that they are considered too fast for conscious perception. In earlier work, Papoušek also observed mothers responding to infant behavioural cues (such as different hand positions during different states of alertness), even though they reported being unaware of such signals. This further suggests that there may be some relatively automatic parenting responses to infant-specific sensory and behavioural cues (‘intuitive parenting’ responses) (Reference Papoušek, Osofsky and FitzgeraldPapoušek, 2000).

Animal models highlight the biological basis of parent–child interactions; infant-specific stimuli (e.g. visual appearance, odour, vocalisation) can elicit maternal behaviour in many species (Reference Rosenblatt and SnowdonRosenblatt & Snowdon, 1996). These maternal behaviours (e.g. nest building, pup-licking, grooming, carrying and arched-back nursing) are particularly easy to observe in rodents, and many of the neurobiological, genetic and hormonal determinants of these behaviours are now well established (Reference Leckman and HermanLeckman & Herman, 2002). Lesion studies in rats have implicated roles for various brain regions (e.g. the medial preoptic area of the hypothalamus, the ventral part of the bed nucleus of stria terminalis and the lateral septum). Although comparisons between species should be made with caution, these findings provide a starting point for hypothesis-driven investigations into the patterns of activation that one might expect to find in humans.

NEUROIMAGING HUMAN MATERNAL BEHAVIOUR

To date, it has been difficult to examine the biological origins of parental responsiveness in humans. It is generally not feasible to use fMRI with infants, but fMRI has expanded our understandibiological basis of emotion perception in adults and has provided implications for psychiatry (Reference PhillipsPhillips, 2003). Adult faces are the stimuli that are typically presented, but similar procedures using photographs or video clips of infant faces would provide an easy starting point for examining patterns of adult brain activation in response to infant cues. Increasingly, fMRI is being used with olfactory and tactile cues and, in the future, it may present the opportunity to examine brain activity in response to infant-specific cues in these other sensory modalities. The existing neuroimaging evidence also allows hypotheses concerning areas of activation that one might expect to find; these might include the amygdala, hippocampus, anterior cingulate and insula, which are all important in memory of emotionally salient material and social signalling (Reference PhillipsPhillips, 2003). Some of these areas also overlap with those believed to be involved in maternal behaviour (e.g. hippocampus and amygdala; see Reference Leckman and HermanLeckman & Herman, 2002).

Functional MRI also provides the opportunity to identify changes in neuronal processing independently of any measurable behavioural change (such as amygdala activation during subconscious processing of fearful faces; see Reference Phan, Wager and TaylorPhan et al, 2002). Therefore, fMRI might further our knowledge of the neural basis of ‘subconscious’ or ‘automatic’ parental responses, such as those described by Papoušek (Reference Papoušek, Osofsky and Fitzgerald2000). Combined with measurements of physiological variables (to help interpret blood-oxygenlevel-dependent signals), fMRI could reveal more about variations in parental responsiveness that have been demonstrated sometimes in the absence of behavioural differences. For example, when viewing videotapes of smiling and crying infants, mothers with and without a childhood history of physical abuse report similar affective states but show differences in patterns of skin conductance (Reference Casanova, Domanic and McCanneCasanova et al, 1994).

An extensive literature search of peerreviewed journals identified only one study using fMRI to study maternal human behaviour (Reference Lorberbaum, Newman and HorwitzLorberbaum et al, 2002). In this study, brain activation was recorded as mothers heard the sound of an infant crying, white noise sounds (matched for intensity with the cry) or nothing. The results provide some evidence for the role of thalamocingulate circuitry in maternal responses and revealed that, in general, the activations observed were consistent with neuroanatomical studies of rodent maternal behaviour (including activation in the bed nucleus of stria terminalis and in the lateral septum). It was also noted that activation unique to the cry signal was predominantly right-sided, consistent with accounts of emotion lateralisation (Reference Best, Womer and QueenBest et al, 1994). However, Lorberbaum et al (Reference Lorberbaum, Newman and Horwitz2002) did not present mothers with the sound of their own infant crying, and behavioural evidence suggests that this is likely to influence the response (Reference Cismaresco and MontagnerCismaresco & Montagner, 1990). Additionally, studies of different types (e.g. basic v. pain) of infant cry and cries from different groups of infants (e.g. full term v. preterm) have been shown to elicit different subjective and physiological responses in adults. Although Lorberbaum et al used two cry stimuli – one from a low-risk and one from a high-risk infant – they do not report whether these different cries produced different patterns of brain activations. Because differences in the fundamental frequency of the cry may reflect the neurophysiological status of the infant signal, it is also problematic that the white noise stimulus used by Lorberbaum et al was not frequency-matched with the cry.

Close examination of the innovative Lorberbaum et al study therefore highlights the need for carefully designed control stimuli (as with all functional neuroimaging studies) if we are to be able to draw conclusions concerning maternal responsiveness. Nevertheless, the study illustrates the feasibility of using functional neuroimaging to investigate maternal responsiveness and raises many questions for future investigation.

IMPLICATIONS FOR PSYCHIATRY

One question raised by Lorberbaum et al's study is whether mothers with depression, who have been shown behaviourally to be less responsive than non-depressed mothers to infant distress cries, show systematic differences in brain activation in response to hearing infant cries. Similar questions could be posed of parents with different psychiatric disorders and in response to different infant cues (e.g. facial expressions). Recent work by Harmer et al (Reference Harmer, Hill and Taylor2003) suggests that antidepressants can lead to changes in healthy adult responses to facial expressions. If it were found that such medications modulated mothers' responsiveness to infant facial cues, then this could be important in guiding treatment (e.g. psychotherapy v. medication) for depression in the postnatal period. Some behavioural interventions aimed at improving maternal sensitivity (in both ‘normal’ and high-risk groups) have proved successful already, particularly when the interventions involve fewer than five sessions, are focused at enhancing sensitivity and start at least 6 months after birth (Reference Bakersmans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn and JufferBakersmans-Kranenburg et al, 2003). The finding that the success of interventions is dependent on their specificity and timing suggests further that there may be central pathways of parental responsiveness at work. If the neural correlates of parental behaviour could be identified, then eventually it may be possible to target interventions more specifically and evaluate their mechanisms of action.

In the same way that neuroimaging has provided a framework for research and evaluation of treatment in post-traumatic stress disorder, we suggest that fMRI has the potential to elucidate pathways of parental responsiveness that might potentially inform the development and evaluation of interventions in parental sensitivity.

DECLARATION OF INTEREST

None. Funding from the Wellcome Trust for our research on the effects of. parental psychiatric disorders on child development.

Acknowledgement

The authors thank Professor Guy Goodwin for his helpful comments on an. earlier draft of this manuscript.

References

Bakersmans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H. & Juffer, F. (2003) Less is more: meta-analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 195215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Best, T., Womer, J. S. & Queen, H. F. (1994) Hemispheric asymmetries in adults' perception of infant emotional expressions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 751765.Google Scholar
Bremner, G., Slater, A. M. & Butterworth, G. (1997) Infant Development: Recent Advances. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Casanova, G. M., Domanic, J., McCanne, T. R., et al (1994) Physiological responses to child stimuli in mothers with and without a childhood history of physical abuse. Child Abuse and Neglect, 18, 9951004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cismaresco, A. & Montagner, H. (1990) Mothers' discrimination of their neonates' cry in relation to cry acoustics: the first week of life. Early Child Development and Care, 65, 311.Google Scholar
Dawson, G. & Ashman, S. B. (2000) On the origins of a vulnerability to depression: the influence of the early social environment on the development of psychobiological systems related to risk of affective disorder. In The Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology (Volume 31): The Effects of Early Adversity on Neurobehavioral Development (ed. Nelson, C. A.), pp. 245279. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Harmer, C., Hill, S. A., Taylor, M. J., et al (2003) Towards a neuropsychological theory of antidepressant drug action: potentiation of norepinephrine activity increases positive emotional bias. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 990992.Google Scholar
Hull, A. M. (2002) Neuroimaging findings in post-traumatic stress disorder. Systematic review. British Journal of Psychiatry, 181, 102110.Google Scholar
Kaitz, M., Lapidot, P., Bronner, R., et al (1992) Parturient women can recognize their infants by touch. Developmental Psychology, 28, 3539.Google Scholar
Leckman, J. F. & Herman, A. E. (2002) Maternal behavior and developmental psychopathology. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 2743.Google Scholar
Lorberbaum, J. P., Newman, J. D., Horwitz, A. R., et al (2002) A potential role for thalamocingulate circuitry in human maternal behavior. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 431445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, L. & Cooper, P. J. (1997) Effects of postnatal depression on infant development. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 77, 99101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Papoušek, H. (2000) Intuitive parenting. In WAIMH Handbook of Infant Mental Health (Volume 3): Parenting and Child Care (eds Osofsky, J. D. & Fitzgerald, H. E.), pp. 310321. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Phan, K. L., Wager, T., Taylor, S. F., et al (2002) Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. Neurolmage, 16, 331348.Google Scholar
Phillips, M. L. (2003) Understanding the neurobiology of emotion perception: implications for psychiatry. British Journal of Psychiatry, 182, 190192.Google Scholar
Porter, R. H. & Winberg, J. (1999) Unique salience of maternal breast odors for newborn infants. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 23, 439449.Google Scholar
Rosenblatt, J. S. & Snowdon, C. T. (1996) Advances in the Study of Behavior (Volume 25): Parental Care, Evolution, Mechanisms, and Adaptive Significance. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. & Aitken, K. (2001) Infant intersubjectivity: research, theory and clinical applications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 348.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.