Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T11:23:26.093Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Great wits and madness: More near allied?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Kay Redfield Jamison*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Meyer 3-181, The Johns Hopkins Hospital, 600 North Wolfe St, Baltimore, MD 21287, USA. Email: kjamiso1@jhmi.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

A purported association between creativity and psychopathology is ancient, persistent and controversial. Biographical research, studies of living artists and writers, and investigations into the cognitive and temperamental factors linked to both creativity and mood disorders suggest a more specific link to bipolar illness. A new, large and well-designed population-based study adds further support to this connection.

Type
Editorials
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2011 

Kay Redfield Jamison is the Dalio Family Professor in Mood Disorders and Professor of Psychiatry at the Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine. She is also Honorary Professor of English at the University of St Andrews in Scotland.

A link between creativity and madness was anciently declared and assumed by many. Others have disagreed, dismissing the purported association as a naive romanticisation of mental illness or a miscomprehension of the diversity of imagination and temperament necessary for original work. Debate is inevitable and will continue, but the results of a large, well-designed study by Kyaga and his associates in this month’s Journal give support to accumulating evidence showing a disproportionately high rate of mental illness, especially bipolar disorder, in creative individuals. Reference Kyaga, Lichtenstein, Boman, Hultman, Långström and Landén1

Evidence for a link

Biographical studies of eminent artists and writers have found higher rates of psychosis (predominantly mania), institutionalisation, incapacitating depression and suicide in a wide-range of creative individuals, from poets to jazz musicians. Reference Jamison2Reference Ludwig5 Studies of living artists and writers have found similarly high rates of mood disorders, especially bipolar illness. Reference Andreasen6Reference Ludwig8 Both types of studies have been limited by small sample sizes and inconsistency in diagnostic and selection criteria; their results, however, are consistent with converging lines of research linking psychopathology, creativity and high achievement.

Investigators have shown that familial patterns of mental illness and creativity exist as well. Forty years ago, Icelandic researchers found that individuals with psychosis, as well as their first-degree relatives, were far more likely than the general population to be eminent in artistic and intellectual endeavours. Reference Karsson9 (The patients, described by the investigators as ‘schizophrenics’, in more recent years have been re-categorised as having had affective illness.) A later study of writers, 80% of whom met the diagnostic criteria for depression or bipolar illness, found that the first-degree relatives of the writers were more likely to have mood disorders than were the relatives of the controls; they also were more likely to have histories of creative accomplishment. Reference Andreasen6 Harvard researchers, in a study of non-eminent individuals, found higher creativity scores in individuals with bipolar and cyclothymic disorders, as well as their normal first-degree relatives, than among the controls. Reference Richards, Kinney, Lunde, Benet and Merzel10

Further evidence for an association between bipolar illness and high achievement was found in a prospective, 10-year, whole-population cohort study of all individuals in the Swedish national school register (n = 713 876 after exclusions). Reference MacCabe, Lambe, Cnattingius, Sham, David and Reichenberg11 Those who demonstrated excellent school performance were nearly four times as likely to develop bipolar illness as were those who showed only an average level of performance. Excellence in language or music was particularly correlated with an increased risk for developing bipolar disorder. Schizophrenia was associated with a decreased risk for excellence.

Two other large studies have addressed a different pattern of association, one between creative occupation and psychopathology. The first, an analysis of interview data collected from more than 20 000 participants in the Epidemiologic Catchment Area Study, found that individuals with bipolar illness were disproportionately concentrated in the most creative occupational categories. Reference Tremblay, Grosskopf and Yang12 This finding is consistent with the more recent and differently designed Swedish investigation reported in this month’s Journal. Kyaga and his colleagues conducted a nested case–control study using Swedish total population registers in order to examine the relationship between mental illness and occupation. Reference Kyaga, Lichtenstein, Boman, Hultman, Långström and Landén1 The likelihood of being in a creative occupation was compared in individuals who had been hospitalised for schizophrenia, bipolar disorder or unipolar depression, their non-diagnosed relatives and controls. Although having a creative occupation is not the same thing as being creative, the study is large and the inferences from it are strong.

People with bipolar disorder were overrepresented in the creative professions; this was not true for those with schizophrenia or unipolar depression (although individuals with schizophrenia had an increased rate in the artistic subgroup). Additionally, first-degree relatives of people with bipolar disorder and schizophrenia, but not unipolar depression, were more likely than controls to hold creative jobs.

The researchers examined rates of creative occupations in non-affected half-siblings of patients who shared either a mother or a father; no significant differences were found. This, coupled with the finding that the likelihood of creative occupations decreased with increasing familial distance to the patients, led the authors to suggest that genetic rather than environmental factors were more likely to explain the cosegregation of creativity and psychopathology. This is consistent with earlier familial studies of mental illness and creativity. Reference Andreasen6,Reference Karsson9,Reference Richards, Kinney, Lunde, Benet and Merzel10 Not surprisingly, many researchers have suggested that some features of bipolar illness and schizophrenia may confer adaptive advantage to the affected individuals and/or their relatives. Reference Jamison2,Reference Richards, Kinney, Lunde, Benet and Merzel10,Reference Wilson13,Reference Brod and Claridge14 Whatever the genetic and environmental mechanisms for this may be they are complex and will be addressed most decisively by neuroimaging, genetic and neuropsychological research. Reference Keller and Miller15,Reference Keri16

The lack of association between unipolar depression and creative occupation is seemingly inconsistent with studies that have found an elevated rate of depression in artists, writers and composers. Reference Jamison2Reference Ludwig8 This may reflect the underreporting of mild manic states, as well as diagnostic confusion (for example, the complex relationship between bipolar illness, major depressive illness and recurrent depression, and the lack of clarity in differentiating major depression manifested by rapid thinking, agitation and irritability from bipolar illness, or mixed states from agitated depression.) Reference Goodwin and Jamison17

It would seem counterintuitive that a painful, not infrequently lethal illness such as bipolar disorder should be associated with creativity. It would seem particularly odd in the light of the substantial and well-documented neuropsychological deficits that often accompany it, such as impaired concentration, short-term memory deficits and impaired executive function. Reference Goodwin and Jamison17 But other mood, cognitive, behavioural, energy and temperament factors associated with bipolar disorder can enhance creativity. Elevated mood and rapidity of thought, for example, often facilitate creative work; mania and hypomania have been shown to increase associational fluency Reference Henry, Weingartner and Murphy18Reference Levine, Schild, Kimhi and Schreiber20 and combinatory thinking, Reference Solovay, Shenton and Holzman21 both of which are important in creativity. Non-cognitive aspects of bipolar illness – for example, risk-taking, grandiosity, restlessness and discontent, illness-induced introspection and a need to make meaning of, or to ameliorate suffering – exert an influence on imagination and creativity as well. The overlap between the bipolar and creative temperaments has been studied and discussed at length. Reference Jamison2,Reference Santosa, Strong, Nowakowska, Wang, Rennicke and Ketter22

Ethical and clinical implications

No one would argue that there is a straightforward relationship between psychopathology and creativity. Most people who are creative do not have mental illness and most people who are mentally ill are not unusually creative. It is, rather, that there is a disproportionate rate of psychopathology, especially bipolar disorder, in highly creative individuals. A link exists and this will prove to have important ethical, scientific and clinical implications. Genetic testing for bipolar illness is inevitable. Increasingly sophisticated prenatal selection techniques will give new options to those at risk for passing on the illness. This may result in the unintended consequence of selecting against potentially beneficial variance in cognitive styles, drive, risk-taking and temperament. The potential for misuse through the development of pharmacological or other techniques to enhance cognition, energy, and mood can be imagined. Related to this, it is essential to develop treatments that minimise the adverse effects of medication on positive aspects of mood disorders. Bipolar illness is devastating and this should remain the central focus of our scientific and clinical work. But while doing so, we need to keep in mind the anciently observed thin partitions between disease and imagination.

References

1 Kyaga, S, Lichtenstein, P, Boman, M, Hultman, C, Långström, N, Landén, M. Creativity and mental disorder: family study of 300 000 people with severe mental disorder. Br J Psychiatry 2011; 199: 373–9.Google Scholar
2 Jamison, KR. Touched With Fire: Manic-Depressive Illness and the Artistic Temperament. Free Press, 1993.Google Scholar
3 Schildkraut, JJ, Hirshfeld, AJ, Murphy, JM. Mind and mood in modern art, II: Depressive disorders, spirituality, and early deaths in the abstract expressionist artists of the New School. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151: 482–8.Google Scholar
4 Post, F. Creativity and psychopathology. A study of 291 world–famous men. Br J Psychiatry 1994; 165: 2234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5 Ludwig, AM. The Price of Greatness: Resolving the Creativity and Madness Controversy. Guilford Press, 1995.Google Scholar
6 Andreasen, NC. Creativity and mental illness: prevalence rates in writers and their first-degree relatives. Am J Psychiatry 1987; 144: 1288–92.Google ScholarPubMed
7 Jamison, KR. Mood disorders and patterns of creativity in British writers and artists. Psychiatry 1989; 52: 125–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8 Ludwig, AM. Mental illness and creative activity in female writers. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151: 1650–6.Google Scholar
9 Karsson, JL. Genetic association of giftedness and creativity with schizophrenia. Hereditas 1970; 66: 177–82.Google Scholar
10 Richards, R, Kinney, DK, Lunde, I, Benet, M, Merzel, AP. Creativity in manic–depressives, cyclothymes, their normal relatives, and control subjects. J Abnorm Psychol 1988; 97: 281–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11 MacCabe, JH, Lambe, MP, Cnattingius, S, Sham, PC, David, AS, Reichenberg, A, et al. Excellent school performance at age 16 and risk of adult bipolar disorder: national cohort study. Br J Psychiatry 2010; 196: 109–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12 Tremblay, CH, Grosskopf, S, Yang, K. Brainstorm: occupational choice, bipolar illness and creativity. Econ Hum Biol 2010; 8: 233–41.Google Scholar
13 Wilson, DS. Adaptive genetic variation and human evolutionary psychology. Ethol Sociobiol 1994; 15: 219–35.Google Scholar
14 Brod, JH. Creativity and schizotypy. In Schizotypy: Implications for Illness and Health (ed Claridge, GS): 276–98. Oxford University Press, 1997.Google Scholar
15 Keller, MC, Miller, G. Resolving the paradox of common, harmful, heritable mental disorders: which evolutionary genetic models work best? Behav Brain Sci. 2006; 29: 385404.Google Scholar
16 Keri, S. Genes for psychosis and creativity: a promoter polymorphism of the neuregulin 1 gene is related to creativity in people with high intellectual achievement. Psychol Sci 2009; 20: 1070–3.Google Scholar
17 Goodwin, FK, Jamison, KR. Manic–Depressive Illness: Bipolar Disorders and Recurrent Depression. Oxford University Press, 2007.Google Scholar
18 Henry, GM, Weingartner, H, Murphy, DL. Idiosyncratic patterns of learning and word association during mania. Am J Psychiatry. 1971; 128: 564–74.Google Scholar
19 Pons, L, Nurberger, JI, Murphy, DL. Mood-independent aberrancies in associative patterns in bipolar disorder: an apparent stabilizing effect of lithium. Psychiatry Res 1985; 14: 315–22.Google Scholar
20 Levine, J, Schild, K, Kimhi, R, Schreiber, G. Word associative production in affective versus schizophrenia psychoses. Psychopathology 1996; 29: 713.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
21 Solovay, MR, Shenton, ME, Holzman, PS. Comparative studies of thought disorders: I. Mania and schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1987; 44: 1320.Google Scholar
22 Santosa, CM, Strong, CM, Nowakowska, C, Wang, PW, Rennicke, CM, Ketter, TA. Enhanced creativity in bipolar disorder patients: a controlled study. J Affect Disord 2007; 100: 31–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.