Most research into factors involved in antisocial behaviour investigates societal factors. This has produced important correlational evidence but does not demonstrate that such factors have causal relationships with antisocial behaviour, as this requires more rigorous experimental designs (Reference AltmanAltman, 1991; Reference Rutter, Tizard and WhitmoreRutter, 1995). Such designs are difficult to achieve in sociological research, which leaves questions of volition, culpability, prevention and rehabilitation open, if cause and effect are more precisely understood. It is noteworthy that a Director of the US National Institute of Mental Health suggested that treatment programmes for offenders tend not to be rigorously evaluated: ‘It's easy to fool yourselves about efficacy if you haven't done a proper clinical trial’ (Reference MarshalMarshal, 2000). It has, however, been suggested that deeper knowledge of the biology of antisocial behaviours will help interventions (Reference Virkkunen and HuttunenStone & Kelner, 2000).
Background
When Sinclair persuaded the wartime British government in 1942 to supplement the diet of all children with cod-liver oil and orange juice, he speculated that among other ills, poor diets could lead to antisocial behaviour. Since that time, evidence has grown to support this link (Reference MoynahanMoynahan, 1976; Reference Walsh, Isaacson and RehmanVirkkunen & Huttunen, 1982; Reference Benton and CookBenton & Cook, 1991; Stevens et al, Reference Stevens, Zentall and Abate1995, Reference Stone and Kelner1996; Reference Hamazaki, Sawazaki and ItomuraHamazaki et al, 1996; Reference Smith, Rutter and SmithSchoenthaler et al, 1997; Reference Zigmond and SnaithWalsh et al, 1997; Reference Hibbeln, Umhau and LinnoilaHibbeln et al, 1998; Reference Bjork, Dougherty and MoellerBjork et al, 1999; Reference Golomb, Stattin and MednickGolomb et al, 2000). If there is a causal relationship between micronutrient deficiencies and antisocial behaviour, then where such deficiencies exist supplementing the diet with appropriate nutrients should improve behaviour. With the approval of the Home Office, this was tested empirically. The study was approved by the University of Surrey Ethics Committee and conformed to the Declaration of Helsinki. The findings have been subject to a 10-month Home Office review.
METHOD
Experimental study
Participation
Before recruitment, the purpose of the project was explained carefully to each participant. Participants had to be 18 years of age or over. Each volunteer recruited (n=231) signed an informed consent form, which was countersigned by a member of the prison staff. The trial had to conform to the normal operations of the institution where participants would leave for reasons such as parole or requirements of cell space. Thus, the analysis allowed for participation to vary from a minimum period of 2 weeks to 9 months in both baseline and supplementation periods. The average time spent on supplementation was 142 days for the placebo group and 142.62 for the active group. No individuals were withdrawn as a result of ill effects from supplementation. Participants were debriefed about their participation by written report.
Materials
It was agreed with the HM Prison Service and the Home Office to use nutritional supplements that were available ‘over the counter.’ Although improvements in dietary intakes of micronutrients could be achieved through diet, nutritional supplements provide a known quantity of micronutrients and allowed for the use of a double-blind, placebo-controlled design to test the hypothesis that supplementary vitamins, minerals and essential fatty acids would significantly reduce the rate of disciplinary incidents. An expert advisory group with no commercial interest in the outcome undertook the selection of nutritional supplements, based on formulation. The University of Surrey conducted assays to check manufacturing tolerance and assessed the match between active and placebo supplements. The vitamin/mineral supplement ‘Forceval’ is licensed for prescription purposes in the UK as a therapeutic adjunct where intake of vitamins and minerals is sub-optimal (UK product licence number PL 0528/5008R). It is formulated in line with European Economic Council Directive 90/496/EEC on international labelling for foodstuffs (24 September 1990) and the UK Dietary Reference Values for Food Energy and Nutrients for the United Kingdom (Department of Health, 1991). Potency is presented in Table 1. A vegetable oil-based placebo with an identical opaque bi-coloured gelatine shell was employed.
Nutrient | Potency | UK RNIs2 | EU RDAs | US DRIs3 |
---|---|---|---|---|
Vitamin A (μg)1 | 750 | 700 | 800 | 900 |
Vitamin D (μg) | 10 | — | 5 | 5 |
Vitamin BI (mg) | 1.2 | 1 | 1.4 | 1.2 |
Vitamin B2 (mg) | 1.6 | 1.3 | 1.6 | 1.3 |
Vitamin B6 (mg) | 2 | 1.4 | 2 | 1.3 |
Vitamin B12 (μg) | 3 | 1.5 | 1 | 2.4 |
Vitamin C (mg) | 60 | 40 | 60 | 90 |
Vitamin E (mg) | 10 | — | 10 | 15 |
Vitamin KI (μg) | — | — | — | 120 |
Biotin (μg) | 100 | — | 150 | 30 |
Nicotinamide (mg) | 18 | 17 | 18 | 16 |
Pantothenic acid (mg) | 4 | — | 6 | 5 |
Folic acid (μg) | 400 | 200 | 200 | 400 |
Calcium (mg) | 100 | 700 | 800 | 1000 |
Iron (mg) | 12 | 8.7 | 14 | 8 |
Copper (mg) | 2 | 1.2 | — | 0.9 |
Magnesium (mg) | 30 | 300 | 300 | 400 |
Zinc (mg) | 15 | 9.5 | 15 | 11 |
Iodine (μg) | 140 | 140 | 150 | 150 |
Manganese (mg) | 3 | — | — | 2.3 |
Potassium (mg) | 4 | 3500 | — | — |
Phosphorus (mg) | 77 | 550 | 800 | 700 |
Selenium (μg) | 50 | 75 | — | 55 |
Chromium (μg) | 200 | — | — | 35 |
Molybdenum (μg) | 250 | — | — | 45 |
Both omega-6 and omega-3 essential fatty acids have been found to be deficient among violent offenders (Reference Corrigan, Gray and StrathdeeCorrigan et al, 1994). For this reason, an essential fatty acid supplement was also employed. ‘Efamol Marine’ provides omega-6 and omega-3 essential fatty acids without an obvious after-taste, a factor that could otherwise have compromised the blind. The daily dosage was four capsules providing 1260 mg linoleic acid, 160 mg gamma linolenic acid, 80 mg eicosapentaenoic acid and 44 mg docosahexaenoic acid. A vegetable oil-based placebo of identical colour and clear gelatine shell was used.
Measurements
Antisocial behaviour
Antisocial behaviours resulting in disciplinary action were adjudicated through Governor or minor reports. Governor reports adjudicate more serious incidents such as those involving violence and may involve loss of remission. Minor reports typically adjudicate on a failure to comply with requirements. The construction of the offence and the standard of proof ‘beyond reasonable doubt’ was the same for both types of report. Thus, Governor and minor reports proven in adjudication, over a specified time period, formed the measurement of antisocial behaviour. Before breaking the blind, Governor and minor reports were categorised into those occurring during the baseline or supplementation period for each participant.
Dietary intake
The dietary intake of the participants was assessed using 7-day food diaries. The nutrient content of each prisoner's diet was determined using a database based on McCance and Widdowson's The Composition of Foods (Reference Holland, Welch and UnwinHolland et al, 1996). As all foods consumed by the prisoners originated in the prison, it was possible to devise a diary where the participants indicated which of the available choices they had eaten and how much (a quarter, a half, three-quarters portion, all or a second portion). Portion weights were determined from the serveries. They were asked to report the number of items consumed, including spread on bread and milk and sugar in beverages. A list of food items (e.g. chocolate) that could be purchased from the prison shop was also included to record consumption. The quantity and type of all food consumed was entered into a computer package (‘Superdiet’) for each of the 7 days.
Psychological tests
The following psychological measures were employed: verbal ability and intelligence derived from the General Aptitude Test Battery (USES 1967) (Reference HammondHammond, 1984); emotional control from the Emotional Control Questionnaire (Reference Roger and NajarianRoger & Nesshoever, 1987; Reference Rutter, Rutter and SmiithRoger & Najarian, 1989); measurement of anger and aggression from the Survey Anger Scales (Reference Roger and NesshoeverO'Rourke, 1994); self-reported health status from the Malaise Inventory (Reference Rutter, Giller and HagellRutter et al, 1970); and the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Questionnaire (Zigmond & Snaith, 1983). The reliability and validity of these measures have been demonstrated previously (Reference Bramley, Easton and MorleyBramley et al, 1988).
Procedures
Supplement distribution
Nutritional supplements were packed into blister packs containing one vitamin/mineral capsule and four essential fatty acid capsules. Blister packs contained either all active or all placebo supplements and were stamped with an 11-digit alphanumeric code during manufacture. To facilitate double-blind allocation, research staff were only provided with details of the respective alphanumeric code allocated for each participant. Each day, coded packs were labelled with the participant's name, cell and prison number, and as prison officers routinely locked inmates in their cell at lunchtime they also gave the packs to participants. Compliance was monitored and logged through officers returning the used packs each day and routine cell searches. The log recorded if a participant's supplement packs were returned empty, full or at all. The HM Prison Service gave permission for the trial under the Declaration of Helsinki on the understanding that we provided active nutritional supplements for 3 months after the trial, so that all participants received benefit.
Randomisation
Participants initially entered the trial enbloc at its commencement in September 1996. They underwent psychometric assessment and their baseline disciplinary records were obtained. A stratified randomisation was conducted on the population of participants in each of the four main wings of the institution, employing a random number generator to allocate to groups. Thus, each wing formed a stratum so that the placebo and active groups were matched in terms of disciplinary incidents and also progress in the prison regime. Participants that were recruited subsequently over the following 8 months were first grouped by their location (wing) and then randomly allocated to treatment conditions using a random number generator.
Statistical analysis
Disciplinary data were analysed using negative binomial (mixed Poisson) regression analysis (Reference LawlessLawless, 1987). This analysis was used because disciplinary incidents constitute a series of discrete events over time and the basic model for this is a Poisson process, which cannot have normal distribution. In a Poisson process, however, it is assumed that the average probability of a person committing an offence remains constant over time and is independent of previous outcomes. Because we knew some individuals were more prone to behaviour resulting in disciplinary incidents than others, we accounted for these variations in the individual rates of disciplinary incidents by modifying the Poisson distribution into to a negative binomial distribution. Goodness-of-fit tests confirmed that the rates of disciplinary incidents were indeed a good match with this predicted negative binomial (mixed Poisson) distribution, so we were able to use this distribution as the basis for our regression analysis. The regression was based on two main parameters, λ and γ. The parameters λP and λA are the mean underlying rates of disciplinary offences per day in the institution for the placebo and active groups, respectively, and were used to compare baseline rates of offending. The parameter γ is the ratio of the underlying rates of disciplinary offences supplementation to baseline; γ P and γA being the respective ratios for the placebo and active groups. The analysis controlled for those individuals who had no offences at baseline and took into account how long a person had been in the trial. Statistical power was calculated from a one-dimensional Wald test, where the deviation of the value of γ from its hypothesised value of unity (i.e. λ remains unchanged) in the null hypothesis is compared to its standard error. The institution's population was typically 220 individuals. We estimated that we would recruit 75% of the population over 9 months. Taking all offences together, we estimated that statistical power for correctly rejecting the null hypothesis, with significance at the 1% level, would be 92%.
RESULTS
Experimental study
Compliance
Based on the daily log, the mean compliance rate for supplementation was 89.83% (95% CI 87.43-92.23%) for the placebo group and 90.67% (95% CI 88.47-92.87%) for the active group. The difference in compliance between groups was not statistically significant (normal deviate z=-0.53).
Adverse effects
The institution's senior medical officer reported no adverse reactions to supplementation.
Dietary intake
Participants (n=112; 57 active, 55 placebo) completed a 7-day food diary. Table 2 shows the average nutrient intake of the active and placebo groups. No differences were considered clinically significant, so the groups were considered equivalent.
Nutrient | Placebo (n=55) | 95% CI | Active (n=57) | 95% CI |
---|---|---|---|---|
Energy source | ||||
Energy (kj) | 10 363 | 11290.2-9435.8 | 10 335 | 11069.6-9600 |
Carbohydrate (g) | 314.5 | 343.9-285.1 | 325 | 352-298 |
Fat (g) | 108.5 | 119.2-97.8 | 104 | 110.8-97.2 |
Protein (g) | 77.5 | 82.9-72.1 | 75.5 | 80.6-70.4 |
Fibre (g) | 18.6 | 20.3-16.9 | 19.5 | 20.9-18.1 |
%E as carbohydrate | 48.5 | 49.5-47.5 | 49.9 | 50.7-49.1 |
%E as fat | 38.5 | 39.4-37.6 | 37.5 | 38.4-36.6 |
%E as protein | 13 | 13.4-12.6 | 12.6 | 13-12.2 |
Vitamins | ||||
A (μg) | 742.7 | 813.6-671.8 | 742 | 806.9-677.1 |
Thiamine (mg) | 1.74 | 1.84-1.64 | 1.84 | 2.04-1.64 |
Riboflavin (mg) | 2.1 | 2.4-1.8 | 2 | 2.5-1.5 |
Niacin (mg) | 17.3 | 18.6-16 | 17.1 | 21.6-12.6 |
Folate (μg) | 253 | 272.7-233.3 | 273 | 301.3-244.7 |
B6 (μg) | 2.15 | 2.35-1.95 | 2.3 | 2.5-2.1 |
B12 (μg) | 4.6 | 5.1-4.1 | 4.2 | 4.7-3.7 |
C (mg) | 81.4 | 97.3-65.5 | 98.7 | 119.3-78.1 |
D (μg) | 3.5 | 4-3 | 3.5 | 3.9-3.1 |
Minerals | ||||
Calcium (mg) | 1032 | 1159.6-904.4 | 1031 | 1154.4-907.6 |
Iron (mg) | 12.9 | 13.9-11.9 | 13.1 | 14.1-12.1 |
Zinc (mg) | 9.3 | 10-8.6 | 8.9 | 9.5-8.3 |
Magnesium (mg) | 261 | 285.3-236.7 | 271 | 295.6-246.4 |
Phosphorus (mg) | 1392 | 1519.8-1264.2 | 1357 | 1475.7-1238 |
Chloride (mg) | 5354 | 5762-4946 | 5123 | 5441.2-4805 |
Sodium (mg) | 3556 | 3844.3-3267.7 | 3431 | 3647.2-3215 |
Selenium (μg) | 43.9 | 47.8-40 | 45 | 48.9-41.1 |
Copper (mg) | 1.1 | 1.2-1 | 1.1 | 1.2-1 |
Iodine (μg) | 124 | 140.2-107.8 | 123 | 138.6-107.4 |
Potassium (mg) | 3083 | 3324.6-2841.4 | 3198 | 3472.4-2924 |
Psychometric assessment
There were no statistically significant differences between the active and placebo groups at baseline on any of the measures of intelligence, verbal ability, anger, anxiety, malaise and depression (Table 3).
Placebo (n=71) | 95% CI | Active (n=66) | 95% CI | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Verbal IQ | 97.23 | 99.4-95.1 | 97.63 | 99.8-95.4 |
Anxiety | 18.07 | 18.8-17.4 | 18.04 | 18.7-17.3 |
Depression | 16.43 | 17.0-15.8 | 16.37 | 16.9-15.9 |
Attitude towards anger | 7.08 | 7.9-6.3 | 6.83 | 7.5-6.1 |
Expression of anger | 6.09 | 6.8-5.4 | 6.1 | 6.8-5.4 |
Provocation to anger | 7.27 | 8.3-6.2 | 7.32 | 8.3-6.4 |
Somatic anger | 5.06 | 5.7-4.4 | 5.28 | 6.0-4.5 |
Duration of anger | 4.95 | 5.7-4.2 | 4.57 | 5.2-4.0 |
Sensitivity | 7.1 | 8.1-6.1 | 7.1 | 8.2-6.2 |
Victimisation | 8.63 | 9.4-7.8 | 9.32 | 10.0-8.7 |
Assaultativeness | 8.94 | 9.7-8.2 | 8.94 | 9.7-8.2 |
Consequences of anger | 7.9 | 8.7-7.1 | 7.57 | 8.4-6.7 |
Anger control | 6 | 6.7-5.3 | 6.82 | 7.5-6.1 |
Malaise | 5.42 | 6.0-4.8 | 6.25 | 6.8-5.7 |
Placebo effectiveness
At the end of the trial, participants were asked to complete and sign a form recording whether they thought they had received active or placebo supplements; a total of 97 did so (Table 4). The proportions of those that guessed correctly in the active and placebo groups were very similar and did not differ from that expected by chance (χ2=1.26; d.f.=1, NS).
Judgement | |||
---|---|---|---|
Placebo | Active | Total | |
Actual | |||
Placebo | 28 | 23 | 51 |
Active | 26 | 20 | 46 |
Total | 54 | 43 | 97 |
Testing the null hypothesis
The null hypothesis, that there was no difference between the change of rates of disciplinary incidents during active and placebo supplementation (i.e. where γA=γP), was first tested on an ‘intent-to-treat’ basis. Here all participants (n=231) recruited to the trial were analysed involving 532 Governor reports and 601 minor reports. Those who received the active capsules committed on average 11.8 infringements per 1000 person-days, a reduction of 26.3% (95% CI 8.3-44.3%) compared to those who received placebos. This difference between groups was statistically significant at P<0.03 (two-tailed).
Actual effect of treatment on those treated
Intent-to-treat analysis is typically applied to test efficacy in the ‘real world’ where some people do not take their medication, so it assesses effects on the observed mixture of compliers and non-compliers. As a consequence, it is biased towards showing no effect compared with any effect that might occur in those actually receiving treatment (Reference Korhonen, Loeys and GoetghebeurKorhonen et al, 2000). The intent-to-treat analysis above included 13 participants who prison staff reported did not take their capsules, 6 who were prescribed psychotropic medication and 40 that did not stay in the trial for 2 weeks, leaving 90 subjects who had received placebo and 82 who had received the active supplements (Fig. 1). In addition, the test used to reject the null hypothesis (i.e. γ A=γP) makes no assumption that the two groups were matched at baseline in terms of rates of disciplinary incidents and as such it is a good test for efficacy but a conservative test of effect. Therefore, a more detailed analysis for the actual effect of treatment was undertaken on these 172 participants that were treated for a minimum of 2 weeks. In this analysis, each individual becomes his/her own control, and provided both groups are matched at baseline rates of offending, we can predict that the rates of disciplinary incidents for the placebo group will remain unchanged, whereas the rate should fall in the active group (i.e. where γ P=1, γA<1).
The null hypothesis (that there was no difference between the change of rates of disciplinary incidents during active and placebo supplementation) was first retested with the 172 participants (now based on 338 Governor and 416 minor reports) and once again the disciplinary infringements of those that received active supplements fell significantly compared with those receiving placebo (P<0.03, two-tailed). Because the null hypothesis can be rejected with the ‘treated’ 172 participants, average compliance was high (average 90.25%) and there were no significant differences between the groups in compliance or drop out for other reasons (z=-0.53), we investigated baseline rates of offending to ensure the groups were matched at baseline.
Based on 172 participants, the active and placebo groups were found to be equivalent in average rates of disciplinary incidents prior to supplementation. The parameters λP and λA are the average underlying rates of disciplinary incidents for the placebo group (n=90) and active group (n=82), respectively. Based on 754 Governor and minor reports, λP and λA were 0.016, so both groups would commit on average 16 disciplinary incidents per thousand person-days. As an additional check, the minimum cut-off for length of participation was applied at 21 and 28 days (rather than 14 days) and consistent results were obtained.
The findings for this more specific test of the actual effect of treatment (i.e. where γP=1, γA<1), were that the active group showed a significant (P<0.001, two-tailed) average reduction in disciplinary incidents, this time from 16 to 10.4 incidents per thousand person-days, i.e. by 35.1% (95% CI 16.3-53.9%), whereas the placebo group reduced their rate of offending by only 6.7% (95% CI −15.3 to 28.7%) (negative figures in CI range indicate an increased rate of offending), which is again within the standard error (P>0.1, two-tailed). It should be noted that similar results are obtained when all participants (n=231) are included in this form of analysis.
The greatest reduction occurred for the most serious incidents (including violence) dealt with by Governor reports. Based on an analysis of 338 Governor reports, the active group achieved a significant (P<0.005, two-tailed) average reduction in Governor reports of 37% (95% CI 11.6-62.4%), whereas the placebo group reduced their Governor reports by only a non-significant (P>0.1, two-tailed) 10.1% (95% CI-16.9 to 37.1% (within standard error)).
Minor reports dealt with less serious incidents (there is a degree of overlap with Governor reports). Based on an analysis of 416 minor reports the placebo group again showed little reduction (P>0.1, two-tailed) 6.5% (95% CI −28.5 to 41.5 (within standard error)), whereas the active group showed a significant (P<0.025, two-tailed) reduction in minor reports of 33.3% (95% CI 0.9-65.7%) compared with baseline.
DISCUSSION
The experimental, placebo-controlled, double-blind methodology has demonstrated that supplementing prisoners' diets with physiological dosages of vitamins, minerals and essential fatty acids caused a reduction in antisocial behaviour to a remarkable degree. It is not advocated that nutrition is the only cause of antisocial behaviour but the difference in outcome between the active and placebo groups could not be explained by ethnic or social factors, as they were controlled for by the randomised design. If these findings are replicated, and they need to be, this nutritional approach to antisocial behaviour has the advantage that deficits in nutrition can be readily identified and remedied.
Limitations
Interpretation
It can be argued that behaviour in institutions is untypical and this will reduce the extent to which these findings can be broadened in their interpretation. As a balance to this argument, the observed effect on behaviour is physiological and unlikely to be limited to prisons as there is no evidence that imprisonment affects the essentiality of these nutrients for human metabolism. Indeed, a greater effect could be predicted where baseline dietary intakes are worse. From experience, baseline dietary intakes among serious young offenders in the community are likely to be worse than in custody where regular meals are provided.
Statistical analysis
The analysis did not take account of the possibility that conducting such a project in a confined setting could influence the findings. It is likely, however, that interactions between the groups in a confined setting would weaken any observed effect because the active group could have influenced the atmosphere for the entire population. Hence, the findings could be an underestimate of the true effect size. With larger institutional studies it would be possible to test the frequencies of disciplinary offences involving two or more individuals for randomness between groups.
Biochemical measures
Further investigations should include assessments of nutritional status from blood before and during supplementation. Although blood analysis would have allowed correlations between behavioural and biochemical changes, we are confident from a considerable body of previous research (e.g. Reference Blonk, Bilo and NautaBlonk et al, 1990; Reference Bunout, Garrido and SuazoBunout et al, 2000) that the nutritional supplements would have raised the prisoners' vitamins, minerals and essential fatty acids by significant amounts. More recent research of violent and non-violent subjects has, for example, found omega-3 plasma essential fatty acids predicted levels of the metabolites of serotonin and dopamine taken from cerebrospinal fluid (Reference Hibbeln, Umhau and LinnoilaHibbeln et al, 1998). Findings such as these suggest that further improvements in behaviour could be achieved by providing a formulation with proportionally more omega-3 fatty acids.
Clinical implications
Re-assessment of risk factors
If these findings are replicated, a potential implication is that the dietary requirements for good health are also supportive of social behaviour. Indeed, like humans, food has both a physiological and social component. This suggests that food is an additional means to reduce antisocial behaviour but it also may improve our understanding of established risk factors. There is a great deal of research into factors that affect the behaviour of juveniles (Reference Stevens, Zentall and DeckSmith, 1995, Reference SchaussRutter et al, 1998), including for instance the breakdown of families (Reference Heiss, Rutter and SmithHeiss, 1995). However, one of the social functions of families is to provide food, it would be illuminating to investigate the extent to which diets are affected by such breakdowns.
Dietary education
Dietary analyses of the participants' food diaries showed that the diets provided for the prisoners were close to current UK dietary recommendations. We found, however, that some prisoners did not possess the most basic knowledge to choose a healthy diet; some had not heard of vitamins. Poor food choices by the prisoners typically resulted in lower nutrient intakes, most notably of minerals. Despite availability, a high percentage of our participants consumed on average less than the UK reference nutrient intakes (RNI) of selenium (97%), magnesium (74%), potassium (74%), iodine (73%) and zinc (66%). Although intakes below the RNI are not necessarily evidence of inadequate intake, most micronutrients were raised significantly in the active group by supplementation, suggesting the intervention could be welcomed on health grounds alone. It should come as no surprise therefore that the institution's medical staff observed no adverse reactions to supplementation and no individuals were withdrawn as a result of supplementation. The findings do suggest, however, a need to improve dietary education as well as providing more nourishing diets. Indeed, one early study (Reference Schoenthaler, Amos and DorazSchauss, 1978) conducted in the community claimed that such dietary education proved more effective at reducing recidivism than conventional probation programmes employed at that time.
Current dietary standards
This research strongly suggests that the effect of diet on antisocial behaviour has been underestimated and more attention should be paid to offenders' diets. It should be noted, however, that the current dietary standards by which dietary adequacy are judged barely take behaviour into account. Thus, having demonstrated empirically an effect on antisocial behaviour, we are only at the start of understanding the potential of this intervention.
CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS
-
• Dietary interventions should be considered when addressing antisocial behaviours.
-
• Without rigorous experimental designs, nutritional effects on antisocial behaviours may be subsumed within effects currently attributed to social risk factors.
-
• Dietary standards should be re-assessed to take account of behavioural effects.
LIMITATIONS
-
• Behavioural effects may be most apparent in those consuming the poorest diets.
-
• Reductions in antisocial behaviours attributed to nutrition may be underestimated due to interactions between groups in prison.
-
• Biochemical measures were not available and will be required in any replication to explore the utilisation of nutrients and also mediating mechanisms.
Acknowledgements
The research was conducted with the permission of the Home Office and HM Prison Service. It was supported by charitable donations to the charity Natural Justice by the Hayward Foundation, the AIM Foundation, the Wacker Foundation, the Wates Foundation, the Esmée Fairbairn Foundation, the Allen Lane Foundation, the Appeldore Trust, Hugh Cavendish Charitable Trust, the Sir Charles Jessel Charitable Trust, Larkhall Laboratories, Group 4 Prison Services Ltd and the Home Office.
Note: The research was conducted under the auspices of the charity Natural Justice with the involvement of the Home Office throughout its development and evaluation. Donations to Natural Justice had no commercial undertakings whatsoever and were arranged during the chairmanship of the Right Reverend Bishop Montefiore and The Earl Kitchener. Although Larkhall Laboratories and Group 4 Prison Services Ltd both made modest donations to the work accounting for 3.5% of costs, the terms of their charitable donations were the same as grants from the grant-making trusts where no commercial arrangements were entered into whatsoever. In addition, the Home Office required that both supplement manufacturers provide letters stating that they would not seek commercial advantage from advertising the selection of their products other than being cited in the report of the findings. The Home Office subsequently made a modest contribution to Natural Justice when the findings were accepted.
eLetters
No eLetters have been published for this article.