Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T07:56:39.567Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Origins of cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia: clues from age at onset

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Eileen Joyce*
Affiliation:
Imperial College Faculty of Medicine, Charing Cross Site, St Dunstan's Road, London W6 8RP, UK. E-mail: e.joyce@imperial.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Editorial
Copyright
Copyright © 2005 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

The age at which someone with schizophrenia first becomes psychotic is a variable trait related to prognosis. An earlier onset of psychosis is associated with a more severe course, irrespective of duration of illness (Reference Suvisaari, Haukka and TanskanenSuvisaari et al, 1998). An understanding of the factors that contribute to age at onset, and how they interact, might therefore provide important information about the pathological mechanisms operating in this disorder. For example, the finding that there is a significant genetic contribution to age at onset suggests that this might be a sensitive phenotype for the detection of susceptibility genes or genes that modify the presentation of the illness (Reference Cardno, Holmans and ReesCardno et al, 2001). This evidence comes from twin studies, which find that age at illness onset correlates much more highly in affected monozygotic twin pairs than in dizygotic twin pairs (e.g. Reference Kendler, Tsuang and HaysKendler et al, 1987), and from family studies, the majority of which find that a younger age at onset is associated with a higher familial risk of schizophrenia (Reference Kendler and MacLeanKendler & MacLean, 1990). Unfortunately, the search for genes linked to early onset has been relatively disappointing (e.g. Reference Cardno, Holmans and ReesCardno et al, 2001). However, Anttila et al (Reference Anttila, Kampman and Illi2003) have now found a highly significant association between the presence of a NOTCH4 gene promoter polymorphism (T25C) and a younger age at onset in male Finnish patients who became psychotic a mean of 4.5 years earlier than patients without this allele. The NOTCH4 gene has previously been implicated as a direct susceptibility gene for schizophrenia (Reference Wei and HemmingsWei & Hemmings, 2000) but most studies have failed to replicate this finding (e.g. Reference McGinnis, Fox and YatesMcGinnis et al, 2001; Reference Sklar, Schwab and WilliamsSklar et al, 2001; Reference Fan, Tang and GuFan et al, 2002). Evidence supporting NOTCH4 as a gene contributing to early onset comes from a study by Takahashi et al (Reference Takahashi, Cui and Kojima2003) who found an association between the presence of a specific polymorphism and age less than 19 years at illness onset. NOTCH is a membrane-bound receptor which influences neural development in a number of ways (see Reference Wassink, Nopoulos and PietilaWassink et al, 2003). This finding implies that the allelic combination of genes that influence brain development may also modify the presentation of schizophrenia in individuals who are already vulnerable.

Obstetric complications too have been associated with younger age at onset (Reference Verdoux, Geddes and TakeiVerdoux et al, 1997), indicating that environmental factors are also active in this regard. The availability of obstetric records of patients and family members identified from large case registers has enabled the mechanism by which obstetric complications contribute to risk of schizophrenia to be specified. In studies of three different populations, foetal hypoxia was associated with an increased rate of schizophrenia (Reference Cannon, Rosso and HollisterCannon et al, 2000; Reference Rosso, Cannon and HuttunenRosso et al, 2000; Reference Dalman, Allebeck and CullbergDalman et al, 2001; Reference Thomas, Dalman and DavidThomas et al, 2001). In two of these studies (Reference Cannon, Rosso and HollisterCannon et al, 2000; Reference Rosso, Cannon and HuttunenRosso et al, 2000) foetal hypoxia was specifically related to early onset. Other findings from the latter two studies suggest that foetal hypoxia alone is insufficient to cause schizophrenia but interacts with, or adds to, a pre-existing genetic vulnerability. An explanatory model, proposed by the authors of these two studies, suggests that foetal hypoxia causes neurotoxic damage to the developing temporal lobe, and that this brings forward the age at onset because it reduces the amount of synaptic pruning required to cross the ‘psychosis threshold’ in adolescence.

EPISODIC MEMORY IMPAIRMENT AND AGE AT ONSET

In the September 2004 issue of the Journal, Tuulio-Henriksson and colleagues provided behavioural evidence relevant to the findings of both genetic and environmental influences on age at onset of schizophrenia (Reference Tuulio-Henriksson, Partonen and SuvisaariTuulio-Henriksson et al, 2004). In the context of an epidemiological study of Finnish families having at least one child with schizophrenia, these workers assessed 239 patients with tests of IQ, memory and executive function. Age at onset was significantly and specifically associated with performance of the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT), in that poorer word list learning and delayed recognition memory were both associated with a younger age at onset. The patients in the study had been ill for a mean of 12 years, but when the researchers controlled statistically for the possible contributions of age at assessment, duration of illness and severity of illness, the age at onset effect remained the same. Thus, impaired memory may be a risk factor for - rather than a consequence of - an earlier onset. This finding supports that of Jeste et al (Reference Jeste, McAdams and Palmer1998), who found a significant correlation between age at onset and learning, measured by a composite memory score derived from a number of tests including the CVLT, in 82 people with schizophrenia.

A previous finding from the study of Finnish families was that age at onset decreased as family risk of schizophrenia increased (Reference Suvisaari, Haukka and TanskanenSuvisaari et al, 1998). However, Tuulio-Henriksson et al (Reference Tuulio-Henriksson, Partonen and Suvisaari2004) found no association between CVLT performance and familial loading for schizophrenia. This implies that genetic vulnerability to schizophrenia and memory impairment were operating as independent risk factors for a young age at onset in this population. It also suggests that memory impairment might be more related to environmental than genetic triggers. A clue to the origin of the memory impairment in schizophrenia comes from the observation that foetal hypoxia is an environmental risk factor for an early onset of schizophrenia, as described above. Learning and episodic memory, as assessed by the CVLT, are thought to reflect the integrity of the hippocampus and amygdala, and these medial temporal lobe structures are well known to be particularly vulnerable to neurotoxic damage caused by a number of noxious insults including not only hypoxia but also viral infection, prolonged febrile seizures and even prolonged psychological stress. Thus, exposure to any of these hazards might produce further damage to an already vulnerable limbic system and, by doing so, bring forward the onset of psychosis. In this regard, it is tempting to speculate that drug misuse, a prominent environmental hazard which also seems to bring forward the age at onset of schizophrenia (e.g. Reference Addington and AddingtonAddington & Addington, 1998), may be operating by this mechanism. Exactly how severity of memory impairment is related to earlier onset of psychosis is a matter of speculation, but a plausible explanation is that it is indicative of the degree of damage to limbic structures implicated in the generation of psychotic symptoms, rather than an intervening variable with causal significance.

EXECUTIVE DYSFUNCTION AND RISK OF SCHIZOPHRENIA

Tuulio-Henriksson and colleagues found that worse visuospatial working memory was related to increased genetic susceptibility to schizophrenia (Reference Tuulio-Henriksson, Partonen and SuvisaariTuulio-Henriksson et al, 2004). This finding is consistent with that of Glahn et al (Reference Glahn, Therman and Manninen2003), who found evidence from a twin study that visuospatial working memory is an endophenotype of schizophrenia. This suggests that impaired executive processes, reflecting the integrity of the prefrontal cortex, might be more related to the function of susceptibility or modifying genes than impaired memory processes. This finding may also be relevant to the observation that a NOTCH4 polymorphism is associated with earlier onset, as described above. Wassink et al (Reference Wassink, Nopoulos and Pietila2003), like most other researchers, failed to find any link between NOTCH4 polymorphisms and the schizophrenia trait. However, they did find an intriguing correlation between the presence of one particular (CTG) n allele, and increased frontal grey matter on magnetic resonance imaging and superior performance on the executive Wisconsin Card Sort Task (WCST). This is direct evidence for a genetic influence on a cognitive function thought to be a core feature of schizophrenia. Curiously, the reverse finding was true in controls: the presence of this allele was correlated with decreased amounts of frontal grey matter and worse WCST performance. Thus, the expression of this particular polymorphism may be modified by other genetic factors present in schizophrenia (Reference Wassink, Nopoulos and PietilaWassink et al, 2003).

The finding that expressions of the same gene appear to have an impact on executive function and age at onset of schizophrenia implies that some modifying genes might exert their action on the prefrontal cortex. Executive dysfunction was not associated with age at onset in the study by Tuulio-Henriksson et al (Reference Tuulio-Henriksson, Partonen and Suvisaari2004). However, the tasks used to assess executive function in that study were arguably less probing of cognition than the CVLT, which was used to assess memory. These tasks assessed the ability to reverse presented sequences of digits or elements in a spatial array (reverse digit and spatial span); many would consider that more appropriate tests are ones that tax the ability to manipulate and update changing information in working memory necessary for ongoing task performance. It is possible that performance on such tasks might also have been related to age at onset. For example, Jeste et al (Reference Jeste, McAdams and Palmer1998) found a positive correlation between executive measures of abstraction and flexibility derived from tests such as the WCST, a demanding task considered to rely heavily on working memory processes. Furthermore, there are hints from the Tuulio-Henriksson study itself that some executive functions might be implicated in risk of early onset. For example, the word list in the CVLT consists of groups of words belonging to specific categories. The strategic use of semantic clusters to aid encoding and retrieval is considered a prefrontal function. Tuulio-Henriksson et al (Reference Tuulio-Henriksson, Partonen and Suvisaari2004) found that the degree to which the patients used this strategy was inversely related to a younger age at onset.

THE SEARCH FOR SUITABLE ENDOPHENOTYPES

We are now entering a phase of schizophrenia research in which a major focus will be the search for more sophisticated endophenotypes to aid the identification of susceptibility and modifying genes. Possibly, these will be based on combinations of imaging and cognitive features. These features are prey to the environmental influences that contribute to the schizophrenia phenotype. The power of studies such as that by Tuulio-Henriksson and colleagues in this regard is that they allow environmental and genetic influences to be teased apart in an unbiased way. The addition of cognitive assessments to this type of epidemiological study, although time-consuming, labour-intensive and expensive, should be encouraged.

References

Addington, J. & Addington, D. (1998) Effect of substance misuse in early psychosis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 172 (suppl. 33), s134s136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anttila, S., Kampman, O., Illi, A., et al (2003) NOTCH4 gene promoter polymorphism is associated with the age of onset in schizophrenia. Psychiatric Genetics, 13, 6164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cannon, T. D., Rosso, I. M., Hollister, J. M., et al (2000) A prospective cohort study of genetic and perinatal influences in the aetiology of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 26, 351366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cardno, A. G., Holmans, P. A., Rees, M. I., et al (2001) A genome wide linkage study of age at onset in schizophrenia. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 105, 439445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dalman, C., Allebeck, P., Cullberg, J., et al (2001) Obstetric complications and the risk of schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 179, 403408.Google ScholarPubMed
Fan, J. B., Tang, J. X., Gu, N. F., et al (2002) A family-based and case–control association study of the NOTCH4 gene and schizophrenia. Molecular Psychiatry, 7, 100103.Google Scholar
Glahn, D., Therman, S., Manninen, M., et al (2003) Spatial working memory as an endophenotype for schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 53, 624626.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jeste, D. V., McAdams, L. A., Palmer, B. W., et al (1998) Relationship of neuropsychological and MRI measures to age of onset of schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 98, 156164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S. & MacLean, C. J. (1990) Estimating familial effects on age at onset and liability to schizophrenia. I. Results of a large sample family study. Genetic Epidemiology, 7, 409417.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Tsuang, M. T. R. & Hays, P. (1987) Age at onset in schizophrenia: a familial perspective. Archives of General Psychiatry, 440, 881890.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGinnis, R. E., Fox, H., Yates, P., et al (2001) Failure to confirm NOTCH4 association with schizophrenia in a large population-based sample from Scotland. Nature Genetics, 28, 128129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosso, I. M., Cannon, T. D., Huttunen, T., et al (2000) Obstetric risk factors for early-onset schizophrenia in a Finnish birth cohort. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 801807.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sklar, P., Schwab, S. G., Williams, N. M., et al (2001) Association analysis of NOTCH4 loci in schizophrenia using family and population-based controls. Nature Genetics, 28, 126128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suvisaari, J. M., Haukka, J., Tanskanen, A., et al (1998) Age at onset and outcome in schizophrenia are related to the degree of familial loading. British Journal of Psychiatry, 173, 494500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Takahashi, S., Cui, Y. H., Kojima, T., et al (2003) Family-based association study of the NOTCH4 gene in schizophrenia using Japanese and Chinese samples. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 129135.Google Scholar
Thomas, H. V., Dalman, C., David, A. S., et al (2001) Obstetric complications and risk of schizophrenia: effect of gender, age at diagnosis and maternal history of psychosis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 179, 409414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tuulio-Henriksson, A., Partonen, T., Suvisaari, J., et al (2004) Age at onset and cognitive functioning in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 185, 215219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verdoux, H.J., Geddes, J. R., Takei, N., et al (1997) Obstetric complications and age at onset in schizophrenia: an international collaborative metaanalysis of individual patient data. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 12201227.Google ScholarPubMed
Wassink, T. H., Nopoulos, P., Pietila, J., et al (2003) NOTCH4 and the frontal lobe in schizophrenia. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 118B, 17.Google ScholarPubMed
Wei, J. & Hemmings, G. P. (2000) The NOTCH4 locus is associated with susceptibility to schizophrenia. Nature Genetics, 25, 376377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.