Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T05:01:11.451Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Precursors and correlates of transient and persistent longitudinal profiles of psychotic experiences from late childhood through early adulthood

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 October 2021

Alexandros Rammos*
Affiliation:
Centre for Academic Mental Health, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, UK; and MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, School of Medicine, Cardiff University, UK
Sarah A. Sullivan
Affiliation:
Centre for Academic Mental Health, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, UK
Daphne Kounali
Affiliation:
Centre for Academic Mental Health, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, UK
Hannah J. Jones
Affiliation:
Centre for Academic Mental Health, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, UK
Gemma Hammerton
Affiliation:
Centre for Academic Mental Health, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, UK
Lindsey A. Hines
Affiliation:
Centre for Academic Mental Health, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, UK
Glyn Lewis
Affiliation:
Institute of Mental Health, University College London, UK
Peter B. Jones
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, UK
Mary Cannon
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland, Ireland
Andrew Thompson
Affiliation:
Division of Mental Health and Wellbeing, University of Warwick, UK; and Orygen, The National Centre of Excellence in Youth Mental Health, Australia
Dieter Wolke
Affiliation:
Division of Mental Health and Wellbeing, University of Warwick, UK
Jon Heron
Affiliation:
Centre for Academic Mental Health, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, UK
Stanley Zammit
Affiliation:
Centre for Academic Mental Health, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, UK; and MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, School of Medicine, Cardiff University, UK
*
Correspondence: Alexandros Rammos. Email: rammosa@cardiff.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Psychotic experiences are reported by 5–10% of young people, although only a minority persist and develop into psychotic disorders. It is unclear what characteristics differentiate those with transient psychotic experiences from those with persistent psychotic experiences that are more likely to be of clinical relevance.

Aims

To investigate how longitudinal profiles of psychotic experiences, created from assessments at three different time points, are influenced by early life and co-occurring factors.

Method

Using data from 8045 individuals from a birth cohort study, longitudinal profiles of psychotic experiences based on semi-structured interviews conducted at 12, 18 and 24 years were defined. Environmental, cognitive, psychopathological and genetic determinants of these profiles were investigated, along with concurrent changes in psychopathology and cognition.

Results

Following multiple imputations, the distribution of longitudinal profiles of psychotic experiences was none (65.7%), transient (24.1%), low-frequency persistent (8.4%) and high-frequency persistent (1.7%). Individuals with high-frequency persistent psychotic experiences were more likely to report traumatic experiences, other psychopathology, a more externalised locus of control, reduced emotional stability and conscientious personality traits in childhood, compared with those with transient psychotic experiences. These characteristics also differed between those who had any psychotic experiences and those who did not.

Conclusions

These findings indicate that the same risk factors are associated with incidence as with persistence of psychotic experiences. Thus, it might be that the severity of exposure, rather than the presence of specific disease-modifying factors, is most likely to determine whether psychotic experiences are transient or persist, and potentially develop into a clinical disorder over time.

Type
Paper
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Royal College of Psychiatrists

Psychotic experiences are not uncommon, with at least 5–10% of individuals experiencing a psychotic experience during their lifetime.Reference McGrath, Saha, Al-Hamzawi, Alonso, Bromet and Bruffaerts1,Reference Sullivan, Kounali, Cannon, David, Fletcher and Holmans2 Although most experiences occur outside the context of a psychotic disorder, the risk of developing a psychotic disorder such as schizophrenia in adulthood is increased in those reporting psychotic experiences during childhood and adolescence.Reference Sullivan, Kounali, Cannon, David, Fletcher and Holmans2,Reference Poulton, Caspi, Moffitt, Cannon, Murray and Harrington3

Psychotic experiences can be highly distressing and are associated with adverse outcomes such as impaired social and occupational functioning and suicidal thoughts.Reference Asher, Zammit, Sullivan, Dorrington, Heron and Lewis4Reference Sullivan, Lewis, Gunnell, Cannon, Mars and Zammit8 However, in most cases psychotic experiences are transient, only ever occurring on a few instances.Reference McGrath, Saha, Al-Hamzawi, Alonso, Bromet and Bruffaerts1,Reference Dominguez, Wichers, Lieb, Wittchen and van Os9,Reference Bartels-Velthuis, Wigman, Jenner, Bruggeman and van Os10 Studying such transient experiences is likely to be less informative for understanding the aetiology or prediction of later psychiatric disorder compared with studying persistent or frequently recurring psychotic experiences.Reference Sullivan, Kounali, Cannon, David, Fletcher and Holmans2,Reference Dominguez, Wichers, Lieb, Wittchen and van Os9,Reference Zammit, Kounali, Cannon, David, Gunnell and Heron11,Reference Hanssen, Bak, Bijl, Vollebergh and van Os12 Longitudinal studies with repeated measures of psychotic experiences allow researchers to study trajectories of psychotic experiences over time, while minimising misclassification error from single time-point assessments.Reference Kalman, Bresnahan, Schulze and Susser13 The few studies that have been able to quantify longitudinal profiles of psychotic experiences have shown that substance use, other psychopathology, and victimisation are more common in those with increasing or persistently high probabilities of having psychotic experiences across time.Reference Rossler, Riecher-Rossler, Angst, Murray, Gamma and Eich6,Reference Mackie, Castellanos-Ryan and Conrod14Reference Bourque, Afzali, O'Leary-Barrett and Conrod18 However, as the baseline class in these studies combined individuals with either no or low levels of psychotic experiences, they do not provide information on factors that differentiate between incidence and persistence of psychotic experiences. Additionally, these studiesReference Wigman, van Winkel, Raaijmakers, Ormel, Verhulst and Reijneveld15Reference Bourque, Afzali, O'Leary-Barrett and Conrod18 have relied on self-reported measures of psychotic experiences that overestimate the presence of psychotic experiences compared with semi-structured interview measures,Reference Zammit, Kounali, Cannon, David, Gunnell and Heron11,Reference van Os, Linscott, Myin-Germeys, Delespaul and Krabbendam19 potentially leading to biased (most likely underestimated) estimates of association.

Aim

To address these limitations, we aimed to (a) define temporal longitudinal profiles of psychotic experiences, using semi-structured interviews assessed at three time points over a 12-year period in the population-based Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) birth cohort; (b) investigate environmental, cognitive, psychopathological and genetic precursors of these longitudinal profiles; and (c) describe concurrent changes in other psychopathology, cognition and social functioning over this 12-year period.

Method

Sample

The ALSPAC cohort initially comprised offspring of pregnant women resident in Avon, UK, with expected delivery dates between 1 April 1991 and 31 December 1992 (N = 14 541; 13 988 infants were alive at 1 year). Further recruitment of eligible cases resulted in a sample of 15 454 pregnancies, of which 14 901 infants were alive at 1 year of age. For information about data available see http://www.bris.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/data-access/data-dictionary/. Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee and the Local Research Ethics Committees. Informed consent for the use of data collected via questionnaires and clinics was obtained from participants, following the recommendations of the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee at the time. Study data after 2014 were collected with REDCap (Research Electronic Data CAPture tools); a secure web application for online data collection, hosted at the University of Bristol (https://catalyst.harvard.edu/redcap/).Reference Harris, Taylor, Thielke, Payne, Gonzalez and Conde20,Reference Harris, Taylor, Minor, Elliott, Fernandez and O'Neal21

The sample used for this study comprised 8045 individuals who participated in at least one Psychosis-Like Symptoms (PLIKS) interview (see below) from the assessments at 12 (n = 6822), 18 (n = 5213) and 24 (n = 3862) years of age. Although the original cohort was representative of the target population,Reference Fraser, Macdonald-Wallis, Tilling, Boyd, Golding and Davey Smith22Reference Northstone, Lewcock, Groom, Boyd, Macleod and Timpson24 individuals included in this sample differed from the original cohort in that they were more likely to be female and have slightly higher verbal and performance IQ (Supplementary Table 1 available at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2021.145).

Outcome measures

Longitudinal profiles of psychotic experiences

The semi-structured PLIKS interview was used at ages 12 (mean 12.8, s.d. 0.23), 18 (mean 17.8, s.d. 0.46) and 24 (mean 24.1, s.d. 0.85) years, to assess current (past 6 months) psychotic experiences.Reference Sullivan, Kounali, Cannon, David, Fletcher and Holmans2,Reference Zammit, Kounali, Cannon, David, Gunnell and Heron11,Reference Horwood, Salvi, Thomas, Duffy, Gunnell and Hollis25 The PLIKS interview assesses 12 key psychotic experiences, including hallucinations, delusions and experiences of thought interference. Structured stem questions are followed by cross-questioning to establish whether the experience was psychotic or not, and to establish the frequency of these experiences over the previous 6 months. Coding of psychotic experiences followed glossary definitions and rating rules for the Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry.Reference Wing, Babor, Brugha, Burke, Cooper and Giel26 Interviewers rated psychotic experiences as not present, suspected or definitely present (see Supplementary material for more detail).

We used an empirical approach rather than a latent model approach to derive our profiles of psychotic experiences over time, as latent models were unstable and underlying assumptions could not be met. To generate psychotic experience longitudinal profiles, a measure at each time point was constructed that reflected the current (average over past 6 months) frequency of the most frequently occurring suspected or definite psychotic experience (0: ‘no psychotic experiences’; 1: ‘low-frequency psychotic experiences’, defined as psychotic experiences occurring less than once per week; 2: ‘high-frequency psychotic experiences’, defined as psychotic experiences occurring weekly or daily). These were then used to create four longitudinal profiles (based on the balance between the number of groups that could be meaningfully examined and greatest discrimination of patterns over time) that summarised the psychotic experience data across the three time points, and maximised the use of the available information:

  1. (a) No psychotic experiences: individuals without a psychotic experience at any time point.

  2. (b) Transient psychotic experiences: individuals with a psychotic experience rated at only one time point, regardless of frequency (reference group for primary analyses comparing persistent and transient profiles).

  3. (c) Low-frequency persistent psychotic experiences: individuals with a low-frequency psychotic experience at two or more time points, or with a low-frequency rating at one time point and a high-frequency rating at another.

  4. (d) High-frequency persistent psychotic experiences: individuals with a high-frequency psychotic experience rated at two or more time points.

As a secondary analysis, we also examined age 12–18 years profiles and age 18–24 years profiles, to see whether predictors of persistence differed across developmental stages (see Supplementary material).

Precursors

Family psychiatric history

We collected data on the presence of depression or schizophrenia in the parents and grandparents of participants.

Sociodemographic characteristics

Data on gender, maternal social class (higher versus lower), and maternal education (one or more O-level versus lower) were collected from parental questionnaires administered around the time of birth.

Pregnancy and birth measures

These included binary measures of self-reported maternal cigarette smoking during pregnancy, self-reported maternal infection during third trimester of pregnancy, and hypoxia at birth (obstetric records).

Cognitive, psychopathology and trauma measures

All measures were continuous and standardised unless otherwise stated. Verbal IQ and performance IQ were assessed at 8 years of age, using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children.Reference Wechsler31 External locus of control was assessed at 8 years of age, using the 12-item Children's Nowicki Strickland Internal–External Control Scale.Reference Nowicki, Iles-Caven, Gregory, Ellis and Golding32 Emotional and behavioural difficulties were assessed with the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire total score,Reference Goodman33 and depression was assessed with the Short Moods and Feelings Questionnaire,Reference Costello and Angold34 both administered at 11 years of age. Borderline personality disorder traits covering the nine DSM-IV criteria for disorder were assessed at 11 years of age, and a binary variable was derived, using a cut-off of five or more criteria to define those at highest risk of having a disorder.Reference Wolke, Schreier, Zanarini and Winsper35 The Big Five personality domains (extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, emotional stability and intellect/openness) were assessed at 14 years (hence measured after the start of the profiles, but included here as they are trait measures, so likely reflecting pre-psychotic experience characteristics), using the International Personality Item Pool.Reference Hofstee, de Raad and Goldberg36 A categorical measure reflecting the number of types (0–4) of childhood trauma exposure (ages 0–10 years) was derived with data from assessments completed by the parents or self-reported by the participants.Reference Croft, Heron, Teufel, Cannon, Wolke and Thompson37 Self-harm (binary measure of child reporting whether they had ‘hurt him/herself on purpose’) was assessed at 11 years of age. The existence of nightmares or night terrors (binary measure) was assessed during a semi-structured interview at 12 years of age.

Concurrent measures

Additional measures assessed concurrently to the psychotic experience measures (i.e. between ages 12 and 24 years) were examined to relate patterns of these over time to the psychotic experience profiles: tobacco use (at least weekly compared with non-weekly smoking at ages 10, 12, 15 and 24 years); cannabis use (at least weekly compared with non-weekly use at ages 12, 15, 17 and 24 years); negative symptoms (assessed with the Community Assessment of Psychic Experience questionnaire,Reference Mossaheb, Becker, Schaefer, Klier, Schloegelhofer and Papageorgiou38 administered at ages 16 (past month), 23 (past year) and 24 (past year) years); past-year self-harm (at ages 16, 18, 21, 24 and 25 years); depression and generalised anxiety disorder (current, assessed with the CIS-R,Reference Lewis, Pelosi, Araya and Dunn39 administered at ages 18 and 24 years); vocabulary and digit symbol scores (assessed as part of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for ChildrenReference Wechsler31 at ages 8, 15 (digit symbol score only) and 24 years); and friendship quality (using the item ‘I talk with my friends about my problems’ from the Cambridge Friendship Questionnaire,Reference Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright40 administered at ages 8, 14, 17 and 24 years).

Missing data

The number of individuals participating in one, two or all three of the PLIKS interviews was 2931, 2371 and 2743, respectively. The proportion of people with missing data on the precursors/concurrent measures ranged from 0 to 44.6% (see Supplementary Table 2 for more detail). We used multiple imputation to minimise the selection bias likely from using a complete-case approach.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were undertaken with R version 3.6.0 for Windows or Stata version 15.1 for Windows. We performed multiple imputation, using the R package ‘mice’, to impute values (and uncertainty around these) for all missing data up to the sample who had participated in at least one PLIKS interview (n = 8045). All precursor, concurrent and outcome variables described above were used to impute any missing data. Additionally, when imputing psychotic experience data, we also used self-reported measures of psychotic-like experiences assessed with the PLIKS questionnairesReference Thapar, Heron, Jones, Owen, Lewis and Zammit16 at ages 11, 13, 14, 16 and 22 years, to make the missing at random assumption more plausible (see Supplementary material for more details). The psychotic experience profiles were passively imputed with the underlying composite frequency variables actively imputed in each instance.

The associations between the precursors and psychotic experience profiles were examined with univariable multinomial logistic regressions separately in each of the imputed data-sets, with Rubin's rulesReference Rubin41 used to create pooled estimates (effects in results referred to as odds ratios for clarity). Prevalence/means of concurrent measures at each age are plotted as figures, stratified by psychotic experience profiles.

Results

Longitudinal profiles of psychotic experiences

The proportions of participants in the imputed sample who were classified within each of the longitudinal profiles were as follows: no psychotic experiences (n = 5259, 65.4%), transient psychotic experiences (n = 1959, 24.3%), low-frequency persistent psychotic experiences (n = 687, 8.5%) and high-frequency persistent psychotic experiences (n = 140, 1.7%). There was a higher proportion of individuals with transient, low-frequency persistent and high-frequency persistent psychotic experiences in the imputed compared with the complete-case data, whereas the opposite was observed for the no psychotic experiences profile (Table 1; see also Supplementary Table 3 for more details on the complete-case sample).

Table 1 Proportion or mean (s.d.) of demographic, genetic cognitive and psychopathological characteristics stratified by psychotic experience profile in the imputed sample (n = 8045)

Profiles were based on current psychotic experiences (past 6 months at ages 18 and 24 years; average past 8 months at age 12 years). Transient indicates transient or episodic psychotic experiences; persistent low indicates persistent or recurrent psychotic experiences with a frequency of less than once per week; persistent high indicates persistent or recurrent psychotic experiences with a frequency of weekly or daily; persistent (all) indicates persistent or recurrent psychotic experiences regardless of frequency. Complete-case refers to everyone with psychotic experience data at all three time points, whereas imputed refers to everyone with psychotic experience data on at least one time point. PRS, polygenic risk score; SDQ, Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire; MFQ, Moods and Feelings Questionnaire; BPD, borderline personality disorder.

Precursors of psychotic experience profiles

The demographic and childhood psychopathological and cognitive characteristics for the four profiles are summarised in Table 1, and comparisons between the transient and persistent profiles are presented in Figs 1 and 2, as well as Supplementary Table 4.

Fig. 1 Univariable multinomial logistic regressions of persistent versus transient psychotic experiences (reference): sociodemographic characteristics, family history and childhood trauma. PRS, polygenic risk score.

Fig. 2 Univariable multinomial logistic regressions of persistent versus transient psychotic experiences (reference): psychopathology and cognition.

Compared with those with an outcome of transient psychotic experiences, there was evidence that individuals with an outcome of persistent psychotic experiences (low- and high-frequency combined) were more likely to be female (odds ratio 1.38, 95% CI 1.12–1.72) and have mothers who smoked during pregnancy (odds ratio 1.35, 95% CI 1.06–1.73). Additionally, they were more likely to have childhood emotional and behavioural problems (odds ratio 1.16, 95% CI 1.05–1.27), depression (odds ratio 1.13, 95% CI 1.05–1.26), borderline personality disorder traits (odds ratio 1.10, 95% CI 1.06–1.13), self-harming behaviours (odds ratio 1.93, 95% CI 1.35–2.76), nightmares (odds ratio 1.76, 95% CI 1.41–2.21), an externalised locus of control (odds ratio 1.08, 95% CI 1.02–1.14) and to have experienced traumatic events (odds ratio 1.28, 95% CI 1.11–1.48), compared with individuals with transient psychotic experiences. Individuals with persistent psychotic experiences also differed on two of the personality traits, scoring lower on conscientiousness (odds ratio 0.84, 95% CI 0.75–0.94) and emotional stability (odds ratio 0.79, 95% CI 0.71–0.88).

For all of these characteristics, with the exception of female gender and maternal education, the effect estimates for the high-frequency persistent profiles were more extreme (i.e. further away from the transient profile average value) than those for the low-frequency persistent profile, although the confidence intervals for these two profiles overlapped (see Figs 1 and 2, and Supplementary Table 4).

There was weaker evidence that lower social class (odds ratio 1.19, 95% CI 0.92–1.54), maternal education (odds ratio 1.24, 95% CI 0.98–1.56) and family history of mental health problems (odds ratio 1.22, 95% CI 0.98–1.51) were more common in those with persistent compared with transient psychotic experiences, and little evidence that polygenic risk scores, maternal infection during pregnancy, birth hypoxia or IQ indices differed between the transient and persistent psychotic experience profiles. There was little evidence that predictors of persistence differed across developmental stages (see Supplementary Table 9).

Comparison with no psychotic experiences

Most of the characteristics that differed between persistent and transient psychotic experience profiles also differed between those with and those without psychotic experiences at any time point over the 12-year period (Supplementary Figs 1 and 2, and Supplementary Table 5). In other words, there were no characteristics that appeared to be related only to the persistence of psychotic experiences rather than to both the incidence and subsequent persistence of these experiences. There was stronger evidence, however, that poorer performance for both verbal and performance IQ in childhood was associated with the presence of any psychotic experiences in adolescence/adulthood, despite there being little evidence that IQ distinguished between transient and persistent psychotic experience profiles. Risk for transient experiences lay somewhere between that for no psychotic experiences and persistent psychotic experiences for all precursors examined, apart from birth hypoxia and schizophrenia polygenic risk score.

Concurrent correlates of psychotic experience profiles

Individuals with high-frequency persistent psychotic experiences had more negative symptoms, current self-harm, depressive episodes and generalised anxiety at all ages, and showed a clear separation from the transient psychotic experiences group, which was itself separate from the no psychotic experiences profile (Fig. 3 and Supplementary Tables 6 and 7). Although 27% of individuals with no psychotic experiences had developed anxiety or depression at some point in their life, that proportion was 44.4% in the transient, 53.3% in the low-frequency persistent and 79.8% in the high-frequency persistent psychotic experiences profiles. Additionally, the cumulative risk of self-harm by 24 years of age followed a similar pattern, ranging from 31.5% in those with no psychotic experiences to 79.3% in those with high-frequency persistent psychotic experiences (Supplementary Table 8).

Fig. 3 Trajectories of temporal correlates of psychotic experiences. All ages are shown in years. Negative symptoms scale assessed using the Community Assessment of Psychic Experience questionnaire.Reference Mossaheb, Becker, Schaefer, Klier, Schloegelhofer and Papageorgiou38

A reverse pattern was present in relation to the vocabulary and digit symbol coding tests between ages 8 and 24 years, with those in the high-frequency persistent profile scoring consistently lower on these measures than those in the other three profiles, and with these differences seemingly increasing with age. For weekly tobacco and cannabis use, there were no discernible differences until 15 years of age, when there was a sharper rise in the use of both in individuals with high-frequency persistent psychotic experiences compared with the rest of the profiles. There was generally little evidence of any differences in friendship quality, although there was weak evidence that this was deteriorating in the high-frequency persistent profile compared with the other profiles.

Discussion

The aim of our study was to investigate environmental, cognitive and genetic antecedents and co-occurring traits that discriminate between transient and persistent longitudinal profiles of psychotic experiences. To achieve that, we utilised longitudinal data from a birth cohort to create temporal profiles of psychotic experiences from late childhood through to early adulthood. We found that the main childhood characteristics that distinguished between transient and persistent psychotic experience profiles were that the latter were characterised by having greater general psychopathology (borderline personality traits, emotional and behavioural difficulties, depression, self-harm, parasomnia-related disturbances); fewer emotional stability and conscientiousness personality traits; more traumatic events and a more externalised locus of control. These differences were more pronounced when individuals with transient psychotic experiences were compared with those with high-frequency persistent psychotic experiences. Although female gender and markers of lower socioeconomic status were also associated with persistent psychotic experiences, these characteristics were more common in individuals with low-frequency compared with high-frequency persistent psychotic experiences. Finally, there was weak evidence that persistence of psychotic experiences was greater in those with a family psychiatric history, but no evidence that this was because of excess schizophrenia polygenic loading in those with persistent psychotic experiences.

When relating psychotic experience profiles to concurrent characteristics across adolescence and early adulthood, there was a greater proportion of substance use and comorbid psychopathology (anxiety, depression, negative symptoms, self-harm) among individuals with persistent psychotic experiences compared with those with transient psychotic experiences and no psychotic experiences. The proportion of individuals with these traits increased over time, and this was especially true for anxiety disorders in those with high-frequency persistent psychotic experiences. The only exception to this pattern was for negative symptoms, although these were still consistently more common in those with persistent compared with transient psychotic experiences profiles.

Additionally, individuals with high-frequency persistent psychotic experiences scored lower in both cognitive tasks compared with the other groups, and this difference again seemed to increase with age, particularly compared with the no psychotic experiences group (where performance remained relatively stable). However, there was little evidence to support a difference between the transient and persistent psychotic experiences groups (Supplementary Tables 6 and 7).

Implications

All precursors that were associated with persistence in our study were also associated with incidence of psychotic experience, whereas for almost all precursors examined, risk for transient experiences lay somewhere between that for no psychotic experiences and persistent psychotic experiences. Our findings provide little evidence to support the presence of specific disease-modifying factors, i.e. characteristics that have little impact on aetiology, but primarily affect severity after onset. Insights gained into aetiology and prevention strategies are therefore likely to be very similar, whether we want to prevent the onset of psychotic experiences or impede the persistence of these and subsequent transition to psychotic disorder over time. It is possible, however, that other measures not included in our study, such as proteomic, lipidomic or other biomarkers, might affect only persistence or severity rather than onset of psychotic phenomena, as has been described, albeit rarely, in other areas of medicine.Reference Onitilo, Engel, Glurich, Stankowski, Williams and Doi42

Our results, if reflecting causal effects, suggest there might be multiple avenues for prevention of onset and persistence of psychotic experience, including treating childhood psychopathology and parasomnias, improving cognitive skills and emotional stability, and reducing exposure to trauma (for example, through parenting or bullying-reduction programmesReference Chen and Chan43,Reference Gaffney, Ttofi and Farrington44 ) or addressing post-traumatic symptoms (for example, through trauma-focused therapiesReference van den Berg, de Bont, van der Vleugel, de Roos, de Jongh and Van Minnen45). These highlight the importance of current initiatives aimed at early identification and treatment of mental health problems in children and young people. Furthermore, the constellation of characteristics (borderline traits, emotional instability, self-harm, nightmares and trauma history) associated with the high-frequency persistent psychotic experiences group indicates similarity to complex post-traumatic stress disorder, consistent with conceptualisations of psychotic disorders as complex manifestations of post-traumatic psychological mechanisms.Reference Hardy46

In our study, over 75% of those with high-frequency persistent psychotic experiences met the ICD-10 criteria for an anxiety disorder or for moderate or severe depression at either age 18 or 24 years, compared with 44% of those with transient experiences, and the cumulative risk of these disorders is likely to have been even higher if we had measures of depression and anxiety that spanned the whole period from adolescence to early adulthood. Similarly, approximately 80 and 60%, respectively, of those with high-frequency and low-frequency persistent psychotic experiences had self-harmed by 24 years of age, highlighting that individuals with recurrent or persistent psychotic experiences represent a group of young people with a substantial need for clinical intervention or support.

It was not possible to determine the temporal relationship between the psychotic experience profiles and other psychopathology over the same time period, which may have facilitated inference of causality, although the strength of support for causal effects of most exposures that we examine here on psychotic experiences has previously been documented.Reference van Os, Linscott, Myin-Germeys, Delespaul and Krabbendam19,Reference Linscott and van Os47 Nevertheless, our findings suggest that the vast majority of those with high-frequency persistent psychotic experiences will have required help for other mental health problems at some stage, and thus there are likely to be opportunities for identifying and monitoring those who are at highest risk of developing a clinical psychotic disorder.Reference Sullivan, Kounali, Cannon, David, Fletcher and Holmans2,Reference Dominguez, Wichers, Lieb, Wittchen and van Os9,Reference Zammit, Kounali, Cannon, David, Gunnell and Heron11,Reference Hanssen, Bak, Bijl, Vollebergh and van Os12

Strengths and limitations

This study has a number of strengths, including the use of prospectively assessed and often repeated measures of precursors and correlates of psychotic experience profiles, allowing for a more comprehensive examination of characteristics that discriminate between persistent and non-persistent experiences than previous studies. Ours is also the first study to use semi-structured interview measures to assess psychotic experiences, thus minimising information bias. Additionally, although previous studies (with one exceptionReference Rossler, Riecher-Rossler, Angst, Murray, Gamma and Eich6) examined trajectories over relatively short periods of time (2–6 years), our study is able to provide information on longer-term persistence of psychotic experiences over a timespan of more than 12 years, with our findings being similar when examining profiles at different developmental stages (ages 12–18 years and 18–24 years). Nevertheless, the findings described here must also be interpreted in the context of a number of limitations.

First, as with most other large cohort studies that span long periods of time, there was a substantial amount of missing data. To address this we used multiple imputation and included a large number of covariates to make the missing at random assumption more plausible; nevertheless, it remains possible that our results are affected by selection bias. Second, for variables included in our repeat-measure correlates, we were unable to tease out the direction of effect in relation to the psychotic experience profiles. However, the aim of our study was not to determine whether the associations we observed are likely to be causal or not, but to identify markers that characterise persistence of psychotic experiences once they occur, and which could potentially inform future studies of prediction of psychotic disorder. Third, although we examined a broad range of measures encompassing markers of sociodemographic status, genetic risk, psychopathology, cognition and behaviours in relation to psychotic experience profiles, we did not examine all cognitive or psychological constructs, or other biological or neuroimaging data.

Finally, we used an empirical approach rather than a latent model approach to derive our profiles of psychotic experiences over time, as, because of the small numbers in the non-zero classes, no latent model was sufficiently stable despite the size of our study. We utilised information on frequency and persistence of experiences to help create profiles to represent psychotic experience trajectories that were guided by our research questions; nevertheless, there may be some misclassification of individuals. Additionally, it would have been of interest to include information on distress in the derivation of the psychotic experience profiles as distress, as both distress and frequency are likely to index psychotic experience severity,Reference Fusar-Poli, Yung, McGorry and van Os48 but unfortunately this was not available at all assessments.

Summary

In this study, we identified a number of characteristics that differentiated between longitudinal profiles of psychotic experiences across adolescence and early adulthood, including other psychopathology, substance use, cognitive deficits and biases, personality traits and childhood trauma. There was little evidence, however, that any of these characteristics affected only the course rather than the onset of psychotic experiences, suggesting that it is the severity of exposure rather than specific disease-modifying factors that most strongly determines whether psychotic experiences are transient or persist over time.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2021.145

Data availability

Scripts used for the analyses conducted in this study are available on request from the corresponding author, A.R. The data that support the findings of this study are available from ALSPAC (see http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/access/)

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful to all the families who took part in the study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists and nurses.

Author contributions

G.L., P.B.J., M.C., A.T., D.W., S.Z. contributed to data aquisition, all co-authors contributed at analysis or interpretation of the data. A.R., J.H. and S.Z. drafted the manuscript which was critically revised for important intellectual content by all co-authors.

Funding

The UK Medical Research Council (MRC) and Wellcome Trust (grant number 217065/Z/19/Z) and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. A comprehensive list of grant funding is available on the ALSPAC website (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/external/documents/grant-acknowledgements.pdf); this research was specifically funded by MRC grant MR/M006727/1. S.Z., H.J.J., D.K. and S.A.S. acknowledge support from the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre (BRC) at University Hospitals Bristol and Weston NHS Foundation Trust and the University of Bristol; G.L. acknowledges support from the NIHR BRC at University College London Hospital; P.B.J. acknowledges support from the NIHR Collaborations for Leadership in Applied Health Research and Care East of England (grant number NIHRPGfARRP-PG-0616-20003(TYPPEX)) and the Wellcome Trust Neuroscience in Psychiatry Network (grant number 095844/Z/11/Z); M.C. acknowledges support from a European Research Council Consolidator Award (number iHEAR 724809); and G.H. and L.A.H. from Wellcome Trust fellowships (numbers 209138/Z/17/Z and 209158/Z/17/Z). The views expressed in this article are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the National Health Service, the NIHR or the Department of Health and Social Care.

Declaration of interest

M.C. is part of the editorial board for the British Journal of Psychiatry, but did not take part in the review or decision-making process of this paper. The authors declare no other conflicts of interest.

References

McGrath, JJ, Saha, S, Al-Hamzawi, A, Alonso, J, Bromet, EJ, Bruffaerts, R, et al. Psychotic experiences in the general population: a cross-national analysis based on 31,261 respondents from 18 countries. JAMA Psychiatry 2015; 72(7): 697705.10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0575CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, SA, Kounali, D, Cannon, M, David, AS, Fletcher, PC, Holmans, P, et al. A population-based cohort study examining the incidence and impact of psychotic experiences from childhood to adulthood, and prediction of psychotic disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2020; 177(4): 308–17.10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.19060654CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poulton, R, Caspi, A, Moffitt, TE, Cannon, M, Murray, R, Harrington, H. Children's self-reported psychotic symptoms and adult schizophreniform disorder: a 15-year longitudinal study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000; 57(11): 1053–8.10.1001/archpsyc.57.11.1053CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asher, L, Zammit, S, Sullivan, S, Dorrington, S, Heron, J, Lewis, G. The relationship between psychotic symptoms and social functioning in a non-clinical population of 12year olds. Schizophr Res 2013; 150(2–3): 404–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, J, Sullivan, S, Zammit, S. Adverse life outcomes associated with adolescent psychotic experiences and depressive symptoms. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2018; 53(5): 497507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rossler, W, Riecher-Rossler, A, Angst, J, Murray, R, Gamma, A, Eich, D, et al. Psychotic experiences in the general population: a twenty-year prospective community study. Schizophr Res 2007; 92(1–3): 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelleher, I, Lynch, F, Harley, M, Molloy, C, Roddy, S, Fitzpatrick, C, et al. Psychotic symptoms in adolescence index risk for suicidal behavior: findings from 2 population-based case-control clinical interview studies. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2012; 69(12): 1277–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, SA, Lewis, G, Gunnell, D, Cannon, M, Mars, B, Zammit, S. The longitudinal association between psychotic experiences, depression and suicidal behaviour in a population sample of adolescents. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2015; 50(12): 1809–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dominguez, MD, Wichers, M, Lieb, R, Wittchen, HU, van Os, J. Evidence that onset of clinical psychosis is an outcome of progressively more persistent subclinical psychotic experiences: an 8-year cohort study. Schizophr Bull 2011; 37(1): 8493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartels-Velthuis, AA, Wigman, JT, Jenner, JA, Bruggeman, R, van Os, J. Course of auditory vocal hallucinations in childhood: 11-year follow-up study. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2016; 134(1): 615.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zammit, S, Kounali, D, Cannon, M, David, AS, Gunnell, D, Heron, J, et al. Psychotic experiences and psychotic disorders at age 18 in relation to psychotic experiences at age 12 in a longitudinal population-based cohort study. Am J Psychiatry 2013; 170(7): 742–50.10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12060768CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanssen, M, Bak, M, Bijl, R, Vollebergh, W, van Os, J. The incidence and outcome of subclinical psychotic experiences in the general population. Br J Clin Psychol 2005; 44(Pt 2): 181–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalman, JL, Bresnahan, M, Schulze, TG, Susser, E. Predictors of persisting psychotic like experiences in children and adolescents: a scoping review. Schizophr Res 2019; 209: 32–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mackie, CJ, Castellanos-Ryan, N, Conrod, PJ. Developmental trajectories of psychotic-like experiences across adolescence: impact of victimization and substance use. Psychol Med 2011; 41(1): 4758.10.1017/S0033291710000449CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wigman, JT, van Winkel, R, Raaijmakers, QA, Ormel, J, Verhulst, FC, Reijneveld, SA, et al. Evidence for a persistent, environment-dependent and deteriorating subtype of subclinical psychotic experiences: a 6-year longitudinal general population study. Psychol Med 2011; 41(11): 2317–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thapar, A, Heron, J, Jones, RB, Owen, MJ, Lewis, G, Zammit, S. Trajectories of change in self-reported psychotic-like experiences in childhood and adolescence. Schizophr Res 2012; 140(1–3): 104–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mackie, CJ, O'Leary-Barrett, M, Al-Khudhairy, N, Castellanos-Ryan, N, Struve, M, Topper, L, et al. Adolescent bullying, cannabis use and emerging psychotic experiences: a longitudinal general population study. Psychol Med 2013; 43(5): 1033–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bourque, J, Afzali, MH, O'Leary-Barrett, M, Conrod, P. Cannabis use and psychotic-like experiences trajectories during early adolescence: the coevolution and potential mediators. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2017; 58(12): 1360–9.10.1111/jcpp.12765CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Os, J, Linscott, RJ, Myin-Germeys, I, Delespaul, P, Krabbendam, L. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychosis continuum: evidence for a psychosis proneness-persistence-impairment model of psychotic disorder. Psychol Med 2009; 39(2): 179–95.10.1017/S0033291708003814CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, PA, Taylor, R, Thielke, R, Payne, J, Gonzalez, N, Conde, JG. Research electronic data capture (REDCap)–a metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. J Biomed Inform 2009; 42(2): 377–81.10.1016/j.jbi.2008.08.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, PA, Taylor, R, Minor, BL, Elliott, V, Fernandez, M, O'Neal, L, et al. The REDCap consortium: building an international community of software platform partners. J Biomed Inform 2019; 95: 103208.10.1016/j.jbi.2019.103208CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fraser, A, Macdonald-Wallis, C, Tilling, K, Boyd, A, Golding, J, Davey Smith, G, et al. Cohort profile: the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: ALSPAC mothers cohort. Int J Epidemiol 2013; 42(1): 97110.10.1093/ije/dys066CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyd, A, Golding, J, Macleod, J, Lawlor, DA, Fraser, A, Henderson, J, et al. Cohort profile: the ‘children of the 90s'–the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Int J Epidemiol 2013; 42(1): 111–27.10.1093/ije/dys064CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Northstone, K, Lewcock, M, Groom, A, Boyd, A, Macleod, J, Timpson, N, et al. The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC): an update on the enrolled sample of index children in 2019. Wellcome Open Res 2019; 4: 51.10.12688/wellcomeopenres.15132.1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horwood, J, Salvi, G, Thomas, K, Duffy, L, Gunnell, D, Hollis, C, et al. IQ and non-clinical psychotic symptoms in 12-year-olds: results from the ALSPAC birth cohort. Br J Psychiatry 2008; 193(3): 185–91.10.1192/bjp.bp.108.051904CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wing, JK, Babor, T, Brugha, T, Burke, J, Cooper, JE, Giel, R, et al. SCAN. Schedules for clinical assessment in neuropsychiatry. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1990; 47(6): 589–93.10.1001/archpsyc.1990.01810180089012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, HJ, Heron, J, Hammerton, G, Stochl, J, Jones, PB, Cannon, M, et al. Investigating the genetic architecture of general and specific psychopathology in adolescence. Transl Psychiatry 2018; 8(1): 145.10.1038/s41398-018-0204-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pardinas, AF, Holmans, P, Pocklington, AJ, Escott-Price, V, Ripke, S, Carrera, N, et al. Common schizophrenia alleles are enriched in mutation-intolerant genes and in regions under strong background selection. Nat Genet 2018; 50(3): 381–9.10.1038/s41588-018-0059-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Major Depressive Disorder Working Group of the Psychiatric GWAS Consortium, Ripke, S, Wray, NR, Lewis, CM, Hamilton, SP, Weissman, MM, et al. A mega-analysis of genome-wide association studies for major depressive disorder. Mol Psychiatry 2013; 18(4): 497511.Google ScholarPubMed
Smith, DJ, Escott-Price, V, Davies, G, Bailey, ME, Colodro-Conde, L, Ward, J, et al. Genome-wide analysis of over 106 000 individuals identifies 9 neuroticism-associated loci. Mol Psychiatry 2016; 21(6): 749–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children: Manual. Psychological Corporation, 1949.Google Scholar
Nowicki, S, Iles-Caven, Y, Gregory, S, Ellis, G, Golding, J. The impact of prenatal parental locus of control on children's psychological outcomes in infancy and early childhood: a prospective 5 year study. Front Psychol 2017; 8: 546.10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00546CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, R. Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2001; 40(11): 1337–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costello, EJ, Angold, A. Scales to assess child and adolescent depression: checklists, screens, and nets. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1988; 27(6): 726–37.10.1097/00004583-198811000-00011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolke, D, Schreier, A, Zanarini, MC, Winsper, C. Bullied by peers in childhood and borderline personality symptoms at 11 years of age: a prospective study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2012; 53(8): 846–55.10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02542.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofstee, WK, de Raad, B, Goldberg, LR. Integration of the big five and circumplex approaches to trait structure. J Pers Soc Psychol 1992; 63(1): 146–63.10.1037/0022-3514.63.1.146CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Croft, J, Heron, J, Teufel, C, Cannon, M, Wolke, D, Thompson, A, et al. Association of trauma type, age of exposure, and frequency in childhood and adolescence with psychotic experiences in early adulthood. JAMA Psychiatry 2019; 76(1): 7986.10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.3155CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mossaheb, N, Becker, J, Schaefer, MR, Klier, CM, Schloegelhofer, M, Papageorgiou, K, et al. The Community Assessment of Psychic Experience (CAPE) questionnaire as a screening-instrument in the detection of individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res 2012; 141(2-3): 210–4.10.1016/j.schres.2012.08.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, G, Pelosi, AJ, Araya, R, Dunn, G. Measuring psychiatric disorder in the community: a standardized assessment for use by lay interviewers. Psychol Med 1992; 22(2): 465–86.10.1017/S0033291700030415CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S, Wheelwright, S. The friendship questionnaire: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism, and normal sex differences. J Autism Dev Disord 2003; 33(5): 509–17.10.1023/A:1025879411971CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubin, DB. Multiple Imputation for Nonresponse in Surveys. John Wiley & Sons, 1987.10.1002/9780470316696CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Onitilo, AA, Engel, JM, Glurich, I, Stankowski, RV, Williams, GM, Doi, SA. Diabetes and cancer I: risk, survival, and implications for screening. Cancer Causes Control 2012; 23(6): 967–81.10.1007/s10552-012-9972-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, M, Chan, KL. Effects of parenting programs on child maltreatment prevention: a meta-analysis. Trauma Violence Abuse 2016; 17(1): 88104.10.1177/1524838014566718CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaffney, H, Ttofi, MM, Farrington, DP. Evaluating the effectiveness of school-bullying prevention programs: an updated meta-analytical review. Aggress Violent Behav 2019; 45: 111–33.10.1016/j.avb.2018.07.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van den Berg, DP, de Bont, PA, van der Vleugel, BM, de Roos, C, de Jongh, A, Van Minnen, A, et al. Prolonged exposure vs eye movement desensitization and reprocessing vs waiting list for posttraumatic stress disorder in patients with a psychotic disorder: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry 2015; 72(3): 259–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardy, A. Pathways from trauma to psychotic experiences: a theoretically informed model of posttraumatic stress in psychosis. Front Psychol 2017; 8: 697.10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00697CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linscott, RJ, van Os, J. An updated and conservative systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiological evidence on psychotic experiences in children and adults: on the pathway from proneness to persistence to dimensional expression across mental disorders. Psychol Med 2013; 43(6): 1133–49.10.1017/S0033291712001626CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fusar-Poli, P, Yung, AR, McGorry, P, van Os, J. Lessons learned from the psychosis high-risk state: towards a general staging model of prodromal intervention. Psychol Med 2014; 44(1): 1724.10.1017/S0033291713000184CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Proportion or mean (s.d.) of demographic, genetic cognitive and psychopathological characteristics stratified by psychotic experience profile in the imputed sample (n = 8045)

Figure 1

Fig. 1 Univariable multinomial logistic regressions of persistent versus transient psychotic experiences (reference): sociodemographic characteristics, family history and childhood trauma. PRS, polygenic risk score.

Figure 2

Fig. 2 Univariable multinomial logistic regressions of persistent versus transient psychotic experiences (reference): psychopathology and cognition.

Figure 3

Fig. 3 Trajectories of temporal correlates of psychotic experiences. All ages are shown in years. Negative symptoms scale assessed using the Community Assessment of Psychic Experience questionnaire.38

Supplementary material: File

Rammos et al. supplementary material

Rammos et al. supplementary material

Download Rammos et al. supplementary material(File)
File 119.6 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.