Obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD) is a debilitating neuropsychiatric condition, characterised by repetitive intrusive thoughts and/or repetitive behaviours undertaken according to rigid rules (or in response to obsessions).1 Neurobiological models of OCD emphasise dysfunction of the basal ganglia, implicated in generating habitual behaviour.Reference Fineberg, Apergis-Schoute, Vaghi, Banca, Gillan and Voon2,Reference Graybiel and Rauch3 In a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies, structural abnormalities of the basal ganglia were identified in patients with OCD versus controls, coupled with abnormal functional activation in this region during tests of executive functions, including cognitive flexibility.Reference Pico-Perez, Moreira, de Melo Ferreira, Radua, Mataix-Cols and Sousa4 Other meta-analysis did not find macroscopic volumetric basal ganglia abnormalities in OCD,Reference Boedhoe, Schmaal, Abe, Ameis, Arnold and Batistuzzo5 but inconsistencies may reflect the variable methodological quality of the included data studies. Although most studies focused on total volumes of whole subcortical structures, advances in imaging pipelines have made it possible to examine detailed, localised changes in the surface morphology (i.e. shape) of basal ganglia structures.Reference Shaw, Sharp, Sudre, Wharton, Greenstein and Raznahan6 This is a promising approach to identify familial markers of OCD, because morphological basal ganglia changes were previously found in patients with OCD and their relatives, when conventional volumetric measures were insensitive.Reference Shaw, Sharp, Sudre, Wharton, Greenstein and Raznahan6 Furthermore, understanding the basal ganglia is also relevant for transdiagnostic approaches, because their dysfunction is implicated in other less-studied obsessive–compulsive-related disorders.Reference Macpherson and Hikida7
The basal ganglia play a prominent role in habit learning and regulation (i.e. cognitive flexibility).Reference Macpherson and Hikida7 One common laboratory-assessed form of cognitive flexibility is set-shifting, which is the ability to switch attention from one important aspect of the environment to another during a task, in response to changing reinforcement contingencies.Reference Chamberlain, Blackwell, Fineberg, Robbins and Sahakian8 Set-shifting is classically measured by an extradimensional shift in such tasks, whereby participants inhibit attention away from a previously relevant aspect of the environment, onto a different aspect of the environment that was previously irrelevant. For more detailed discussion of the cognitive neuroscience of extradimensional shifting and its relationship to habit and loss of top-down flexibility, see work by RobbinsReference Robbins9 and Chamberlain et al.Reference Chamberlain, Solly, Hook, Vaghi and Robbins10 Abnormalities in this cognitive function have been extensively implicated in the pathophysiology of OCD.Reference Chamberlain, Blackwell, Fineberg, Robbins and Sahakian8 Impaired set-shifting performance on the intradimensional/extradimensional (IDED) task has been confirmed in a meta-analysis of OCD studies with medium-to-large effect size, and has also been found to extend to clinically asymptomatic first-degree relatives of patients, thereby constituting a candidate familial marker.Reference Chamberlain, Solly, Hook, Vaghi and Robbins10
Given that basal ganglia morphological changes and impaired set-shifting have separately been implicated as candidate familial markers for OCD in prior work, this study sought to demonstrate a link between the two. We hypothesised that basal ganglia shape would relate to set-shifting performance overall; and morphological abnormalities would be found in patients with OCD and their relatives versus controls, in the identified set-shifting-related brain regions.
Method
Participants
The pooled data-set included participants from two studies, where participants underwent conventional imaging measures, reported previouslyReference Menzies, Achard, Chamberlain, Fineberg, Chen and del Campo11,Reference Vaghi, Vertes, Kitzbichler, Apergis-Schoute, van der Flier and Fineberg12 (technical details of the scans are provided in Supplementary File 1 available at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2021.45). The first sample comprised 32 pairs of unaffected first-degree relatives and OCD probands, and 32 age- and gender-matched healthy controls.Reference Macpherson and Hikida7 The second sample comprised 44 patients with OCD and 43 age- and gender-matched healthy controls.Reference Chamberlain, Blackwell, Fineberg, Robbins and Sahakian8
Participants were screened for mental disorders by extended clinical interview conducted by a psychiatrist, supplemented with the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Inventory (MINI).Reference Sheehan, Lecrubier, Sheehan, Amorim, Janavs and Weiller13 OCD severity was quantified with the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (Y-BOCS),Reference Goodman, Price, Rasmussen, Mazure, Fleischmann and Hill14 and IQ was measured by the National Adult Reading Test.Reference Nelson15, Reference Nelson and Willison16 Patients with OCD were excluded if they had any comorbidities, including depression. Controls and first-degree relatives of patients were excluded if they had any history of mental disorders. Controls were excluded if they reported any known history of OCD in first-degree family members. History of serious head injury, substance misuse, epilepsy or magnetic resonance imaging contraindications was exclusionary across all groups.
The study was approved by the Addenbrooke's NHS Trust Local Research Ethics Committee (Cambridge, UK) and Local Research Ethical Committee at the University of Cambridge (Cambridge, UK). The research complied with relevant ethical standards including the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants provided written informed consent after reading information sheets about the study and having the opportunity to ask questions.
Behavioural testing
Before scanning, all participants undertook the computerised IDED set-shift task from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (Cambridge Cognition, Cambridge, UK; see https://www.cambridgecognition.com/cantab/). Two stimuli were displayed on-screen for each trial, and participants attempted to learn underlying rules governing which stimulus was correct, based on computerised feedback. Set-shifting performance on the task was indexed by errors on the crucial extradimensional attentional shift stage. Of the different stages of the task (i.e. learning, intradimensional set-shifting, etc.) extradimensional attentional shift stage was previously found to be selectively impaired in OCD, in meta-analysis.Reference Chamberlain, Solly, Hook, Vaghi and Robbins10 Extradimensional shifting is widely regarded as the archetypal form of set-shifting on such tasks. A more detailed task description is provided in Supplementary File 1.
Data analysis
To address the two study hypotheses, our overall a priori analytic approach was first, to identify morphological regions of basal ganglia structures related to set-shifting performance; and second, to examine whether morphology in these set-shifting-related areas was abnormal in patients with OCD and their relatives versus controls.
Following standard pre-processing pipelines (for details, see Supplementary File 1), vertex analysis was deployed to investigate the shapes of subcortical structures, using FIRST from the FMRIB Software Library (FSL).Reference Patenaude, Smith, Kennedy and Jenkinson17 The vertex locations of each participant were projected onto the surface normal of the average shape template of the 336 training subjects provided by FSL, and the perpendicular distance from the average surface was calculated. A positive value of the vertex indicated deformation in the outward direction, and a negative value of a vertex indicated deformation in the inward direction. ‘Randomise’, a permutation-based, non-parametric testing method implemented in FSL (5000 iterations), was used to identify subcortical structures whose shape was significantly associated (i.e. correlated with) with extradimensional errors, across all participants.Reference Winkler, Ridgway, Webster, Smith and Nichols18 Based on extant models of OCD and structures implicated in flexibility, we focused on the caudate, putamen, globus pallidus and nucleus accumbens.Reference Fineberg, Apergis-Schoute, Vaghi, Banca, Gillan and Voon2,Reference Graybiel and Rauch3 We performed multiple comparison correction at P < 0.05, using threshold-free cluster enhancement.
Group differences in demographic data were explored by ANOVA or chi-squared test, as appropriate. Group differences in subcortical clusters associated with extradimensional performance were explored by non-parametric Kruskal–Wallis test, to identify overall group differences; and Wilcoxon test, for pairwise comparison of groups if overall effects of groups were found. Correlations (Spearman's rho) were examined between patient–relative pairs in terms of curvature in the identified clusters, and between randomly permuted pairs of participants, to identify whether the extradimensional-related structural changes were significantly familial. Statistical significance was defined as P < 0.05 (uncorrected).
Results
The groups did not differ significantly from each other in terms of age, gender and estimated verbal IQ (all P > 0.10; Supplementary Table 1). A total of 51 (67.1%) patients in the OCD group were receiving psychotropic medication. The mean total Y-BOCS score in the OCD group was 21.89 (s.d. 5.30).
Two significant brain clusters were identified in which curvature was significantly associated with the number of extradimensional errors on the IDED task across all study participants (Fig. 1, a and b, left panel). One cluster (overall P = 0.0380) was in the left putamen, maximal at [X = −32, Y = −8, Z = −3] and of extent 363 voxels. The other cluster (overall P = 0.0252) was in the left pallidum, maximal at [X = −21, Y = −15, Z = −2] and of extent 216 voxels. The scanner type did not significantly affect curvature in these clusters (each P > 0.50 by Wilcoxon test, comparing cluster parameters between sites).
Groups differed significantly in curvature in the left putamen cluster (Kruskal–Wallis test, 10.001; P = 0.007), but not significantly in the left pallidum cluster (Kruskal–Wallis test, 2.308; P = 0.315). As shown in Fig. 1a and b (right panel), patients with OCD had significant inward curvature deformation in the left putamen cluster versus controls (P = 0.008), as did their relatives (P = 0.008). The group difference in the left putamen cluster was also significant in each study data-set considered separately: study 1, P = 0.0270; study 2, P = 0.0353. Patients did not differ significantly from relatives on this measure (P = 0.3661).
Mean curvature in the extradimensional-associated putamen cluster was significantly correlated in OCD proband–relative pairs (r 2 = 0.261, P < 0.001), but not randomly permuted pairs (P > 0.10; bootstrap N = 2500, 95% confidence interval for the null model P-value was 0.37–0.39).
Patients with OCD who were receiving psychotropic medication did not differ from patients who were not receiving such medication, in terms of curvature in the putamen cluster (Wilcoxon test, P = 0.78). Curvature in the putamen cluster did not significantly correlate with Y-BOCS total scores in patients with OCD (P = 0.233).
As expected, and previously reported, patients with OCD and their relatives were selectively impaired on the extradimensional stage of the IDED task versus controls (Supplementary File 1).
Discussion
By conducting an analysis of cognitive function and basal ganglia morphology from magnetic resonance imaging data, this study showed that set-shifting performance (extradimensional shifting) correlated with putamen morphology; and demonstrated that patients with OCD and their asymptomatic first-degree relatives had common abnormalities in this set-shifting-related region. Furthermore, the association between the structure of the putamen and the extradimensional errors was found to be significantly familial in OCD proband–relative pairs. These structural brain changes in patients with OCD and their relatives reflect localised (at the millimetre level) changes in the shape of specific basal ganglia structures.
The implication of these findings is that putamen shape relates to set-shifting performance, and that deformation of this structure constitutes a related candidate familial marker for OCD. The findings may reflect clustering of different groups in separate areas of the bidimensional (i.e. extradimensional shift versus brain structure) variable space. Previous translational research indicate that the putamen plays a cardinal role in learning, habitual behaviour and adapting stimulus-response associations.Reference Graybiel and Rauch3,Reference Jackson, Horst, Axelsson, Horiguchi, Cockcroft and Robbins19 Extradimensional errors in OCD were previously found to be associated with reduced frontal connectivity between the dorsal striatum and frontal cortical regions.Reference Vaghi, Vertes, Kitzbichler, Apergis-Schoute, van der Flier and Fineberg12 Thus, predilection toward habitual response patterns and impaired goal-directed control, linked to morphological changes of the basal ganglia, appear to confer familial risk for OCD. The morphological changes do not appear to be diagnostic markers or a consequence of symptoms themselves, and are not secondary to psychotropic medication. Structural abnormalities of the basal ganglia may reflect altered neurodevelopmental trajectories associated with the risk of OCD, but to address this would require longitudinal research.
Supplementary material
To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2021.45.
Data availability
The data are not publicly available as participants did not consent to that.
Acknowledgements
The authors wish to thank all study participants.
Author contributions
All authors contributed to the study design, intellectual content, drafting of the manuscript, interpretation of data, and approved the manuscript for submission.
Funding
This work was funded by a Wellcome Trust clinical fellowship to S.R.C. (UK; reference numbers 110049/Z/15/Z and 110049/Z/15/A). M.I.'s role in this project was funded by a grant-in-aid for scientific research on innovative areas (grant number 16K21720) from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology of Japan, and by the Nippon Foundation international fellowship. T.W.R., A.M.A.S. and M.V. are supported by Wellcome Trust grant 104631/Z/14/Z.
Declaration of interest
S.R.C. consults for Promentis, and receives stipends from Elsevier for journal editorial work. B.J.S. and T.W.R. consult for, and receive royalties from, Cambridge Cognition. N.A.F. declares that in the past 3 years, she has held research or networking grants from the ECNP, UK NIHR, EU H2020 (COST), MRC and University of Hertfordshire; accepted travel and/or hospitality expenses from the BAP, ECNP, Royal College of Psychiatrists, CINP, International Forum of Mood and Anxiety Disorders, World Psychiatric Association, Indian Association for Biological Psychiatry, Sun; received payment from Taylor and Francis and Elsevier for editorial duties; and accepted a paid speaking engagement in a webinar sponsored by Abbott. Previously, N.A.F. has accepted paid speaking engagements in various industry-supported symposia, and recruited patients for various industry-sponsored studies in the field of OCD treatment. N.A.F. leads an NHS treatment service for OCD; holds Board membership (or similar) for various registered charities linked to OCD; and gives advice on psychopharmacology to the UK MHRA. E.T.B. is a National Institute of Health Research Senior Investigator, is full-time employed by the University of Cambridge and was previously (until May 2019) part-time employed by GlaxoSmithKline; is a member of the Scientific Advisory Board of Seiso Heptares; and receives research funding from Janssen, GlaxoSmithKline and Lundbeck as part of the Wellcome Trust Consortium for the Neuroimmunology of Mood Disorders and Alzheimer's Disease. The other authors report no potential conflicts of interest.
eLetters
No eLetters have been published for this article.