Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T07:43:57.212Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Effect of Genetic Predisposition to Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Traits on Recruitment Bias in a Study of Cognitive Aging

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 July 2023

Lina M. Gomez*
Affiliation:
QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
Brittany L. Mitchell
Affiliation:
QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
Kerrie McAloney
Affiliation:
QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
Jessica Adsett
Affiliation:
QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
Natalie Garden
Affiliation:
QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
Madeline Wood
Affiliation:
QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
Santiago Diaz-Torres
Affiliation:
QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia School of Biomedical Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
Luis M. Garcia-Marin
Affiliation:
QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia School of Biomedical Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
Michael Breakspear
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Sciences, The University of Newcastle, Newcastle, New South Wales, Australia
Nicholas G. Martin
Affiliation:
QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
Michelle K. Lupton
Affiliation:
QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia School of Biomedical Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia School of Biomedical Sciences, Faculty of Health, Queensland University of Technology, Queensland, Australia
*
Corresponding author: Lina Gomez; Email: lina.gomez@qimrberghofer.edu.au

Abstract

The recruitment of participants for research studies may be subject to bias. The Prospective Imaging Study of Ageing (PISA) aims to characterize the phenotype and natural history of healthy adult Australians at high future risk of Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Participants approached to take part in PISA were selected from existing cohort studies with available genomewide genetic data for both successfully and unsuccessfully recruited participants, allowing us to investigate the genetic contribution to voluntary recruitment, including the genetic predisposition to AD. We use a polygenic risk score (PRS) approach to test to what extent the genetic risk for AD, and related risk factors predict participation in PISA. We did not identify a significant association of genetic risk for AD with study participation, but we did identify significant associations with PRS for key causal risk factors for AD, IQ, household income and years of education. We also found that older and female participants were more likely to take part in the study. Our findings highlight the importance of considering bias in key risk factors for AD in the recruitment of individuals for cohort studies.

Type
Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of International Society for Twin Studies

The recruitment process for participants in research studies can be biased due to factors affecting their likelihood to volunteer, resulting in a sample that is not necessarily representative of the general population (Oswald et al., Reference Oswald, Wand, Zhu and Selby2013). Recruitment bias can be exacerbated when the target population is composed of individuals with a specific disorder or related history, which may affect their willingness to participate (Patten, Reference Patten2000). For instance, individuals with lower cognitive function may be more willing to enrol in research, as they may perceive themselves as being in greater need of interventions (Li et al., Reference Li, Kim, Sereika, Nissley and Lingler2022). Similarly, people with dementia or a family history of dementia may be motivated to participate in research as a way to contribute to the development of treatment strategies (Milani et al., Reference Milani, Cottler and Striley2021). Several factors, such as lack of confidence, depressive symptoms (Ghanbari et al., Reference Ghanbari, Lovasi and Bader2023), unemployment and low educational attainment (Haring et al., Reference Haring, Alte, Völzke, Sauer, Wallaschofski, John and Schmidt2009) may lead to a decline in participation, while willingness to participate has been shown to be associated with favorable profiles, such as younger age and higher education attainment (Enzenbach et al., Reference Enzenbach, Wicklein, Wirkner and Loeffler2019; Ganguli et al., Reference Ganguli, Lee, Hughes, Snitz, Jakubcak, Duara and Chang2015).

Recruitment bias can impact prevalence estimates. In addition, it can affect association estimates, caused by collider bias, where two variables independently influence a third collider variable. Conditioning on the collider (in this case, study participation) can contribute to a spurious association between the two variables (Zheng et al., Reference Zheng, Levine, Shen, Gogarten, Laurie and Weir2012).

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is a weakening neurological condition, characterized by progressive neurodegeneration and decline of cognitive function leading to dementia (Andrews et al., Reference Andrews, Fulton-Howard, O’Reilly, Marcora and Goate2021). The symptomatic burden of dementia typically occurs late in life, but it is preceded by a long preclinical phase, characterized by neuropathological impairment. Therefore, much of the ongoing dementia research is focused on elucidating early pathological changes (Jack et al., Reference Jack, Bennett, Blennow, Carrillo, Dunn, Haeberlein, Holtzman, Jagust, Jessen, Karlawish, Liu, Molinuevo, Montine, Phelps, Rankin, Rowe, Scheltens, Siemers and Snyder2018). However, no study has analyzed the effect of genetic predisposition to AD and causal traits on study participation in middle-aged and older individuals.

The Prospective Imaging Study of Ageing: Genes, Brain and Behaviour (PISA) aims to characterize the phenotype and natural history of healthy adult Australians at high future risk of AD (Lupton et al. Reference Lupton, Robinson, Adam, Rose, Byrne, Salvado, Pachana, Almeida, McAloney, Gordon, Raniga, Fazlollahi, Xia, Ceslis, Sonkusare, Zhang, Kholghi, Karunanithi, Mosley and Breakspear2021). Potential PISA participants were selected from previous genetic cohort studies on the condition that they had genomewide genetic data already available. This offers a unique opportunity to investigate the genetic contribution to voluntary recruitment, enabling us to test for differences between those successfully recruited into PISA and those who were not.

In the present study, we aimed to investigate whether the likelihood of participating in the PISA study was influenced by factors associated with a higher risk of AD, including genetic risk of AD and causally associated risk factors. Using Mendelian randomization, we and others have previously shown that out of several potentially modifiable AD risk factors, educational attainment (EA), intelligence (measured by intelligence quotient; IQ) and socioeconomic status (measured by household income; HI) have a causal association with AD (Thorp et al., Reference Thorp, Mitchell, Gerring, Ong, Gharahkhani, Derks and Lupton2022). Therefore, we used polygenic risk scores (PRS) to assess the relationship between the genetic risk for AD and educational attainment, intelligence, and household income. By examining the association between these factors and the likelihood of participating in the PISA study, we aimed to assess whether there is a heritable component that is mediating agreement to participate in PISA, and whether this could create bias in the investigation of risk factors and prodromal disease markers for AD.

Materials and Methods

Sample Recruitment

PISA is a prospective cohort study of healthy Australians in mid to late adulthood. The sample recruitment pool consisted of 15,531 participants with available genomewide genotyping, derived from extensive in-house cohorts, including studies of Australian twins and their families, and genomewide association studies (GWAS) for physical or psychiatric conditions conducted over four decades at QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute (Heath et al., Reference Heath, Whitfield, Martin, Pergadia, Goate, Lind, McEvoy, Schrage, Grant, Chou, Zhu, Henders, Medland, Gordon, Nelson, Agrawal, Nyholt, Bucholz, Madden and Montgomery2011; Medland et al., Reference Medland, Nyholt, Painter, McEvoy, McRae, Zhu, Gordon, Ferreira, Wright, Henders, Campbell, Duffy, Hansell, Macgregor, Slutske, Heath, Montgomery and Martin2009). We attempted to re-contact all research participants between 40 to 80 years old (years of birth: 1946–1986). Those who were known to be deceased, were living overseas, had requested not be included in further studies or had no available valid contact details were excluded, resulting in a final sample size of 13,432. Over 9000 participant records (N = 9685, 72%) were successfully updated. The majority (61.7%) of the participants in the study were women, with a mean age of 58.8 and standard deviation of 7.4 years. All recontacted participants were invited to complete an online survey that aimed to update aspects related to lifestyle and cognitive and behavioral function (full details are given in Lupton et al., Reference Lupton, Robinson, Adam, Rose, Byrne, Salvado, Pachana, Almeida, McAloney, Gordon, Raniga, Fazlollahi, Xia, Ceslis, Sonkusare, Zhang, Kholghi, Karunanithi, Mosley and Breakspear2021). The survey began with an information and consent page followed by a core module that captured the central dataset. Once the core module was complete, the survey included 10 additional modules and the option to consent to linking medical records (described in full in Lupton et al., Reference Lupton, Robinson, Adam, Rose, Byrne, Salvado, Pachana, Almeida, McAloney, Gordon, Raniga, Fazlollahi, Xia, Ceslis, Sonkusare, Zhang, Kholghi, Karunanithi, Mosley and Breakspear2021). No incentive or remuneration was offered to participants to take part in this stage of the study. Participants were offered the option of completing a paper version of the survey, which was mailed to them upon request. Subjects were informed that they might be invited to take part in further research. Overall, 4801 participants completed the core module of the online PISA survey, with a mean age of 60 years and standard deviation of 7 years.

For the purposes of this study, those who took part by completing the core survey module of the PISA questionnaire were considered as participating. All participants provided online informed consent prior to participating in the study, which included permission to link to previously collected data. The PISA study protocol (P2210) was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committees (HREC) of QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute.

Single Nucleotide Polymorphism (SNP)-Based Heritability

Genomewide genotyping of participants within our recruitment pool was performed using a range of genotyping arrays as previously detailed (Cuellar-Partida et al., 2015; Medland et al., Reference Medland, Nyholt, Painter, McEvoy, McRae, Zhu, Gordon, Ferreira, Wright, Henders, Campbell, Duffy, Hansell, Macgregor, Slutske, Heath, Montgomery and Martin2009). Datasets were combined with strict quality control procedures and imputed to the Haplotype Reference Consortium (HRC) Release 1 reference panel (Loh et al., Reference Loh, Danecek, Palamara, Fuchsberger, Reshef, Finucane, Schoenherr, Forer, McCarthy, Abecasis, Durbin and Price2016).

We employed genome-based restricted maximum likelihood (GREML) as implemented in GCTAv.1.91.3 beta (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Lee, Goddard and Visscher2011) to estimate the proportion of variance in the PISA study (on the observed scale) explained by measured genetic differences (SNP-based heritability). This approach leverages a genetic-relatedness matrix (GRM) and restricted maximum likelihood to partition the variance of a phenotype into genetic and environmental components (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Benyamin, McEvoy, Gordon, Henders, Nyholt, Madden, Heath, Martin, Montgomery, Goddard and Visscher2010). This method is frequently used to assess whether genetic covariation explains a significant proportion of the covariation of a trait. For this study, a GRM based on a subset of unrelated individuals of European ancestry was leveraged to identify evidence for a genetic component to taking part in PISA.

Polygenic Risk Scores (PRS)

A PRS is a way to estimate an individual’s genetic susceptibility for a particular trait or disease based on their genetic makeup. It is calculated using the sum of an individual’s genomewide or location-specific genotypes, weighted by effect size estimates for SNPs associated with the trait or disease of interest (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Leung, Lee, Voight and Wang2022).

We computed PRS, using the SBayesR approach, to investigate the extent to which the genetic risk for AD, EA, IQ, and HI predicted participation in PISA. We estimated PRS using GWAS summary statistics for AD (Bellenguez et al., Reference Bellenguez, Küçükali, Jansen, Kleineidam, Moreno-Grau, Amin, Naj, Campos-Martin, Grenier-Boley, Andrade, Holmans, Boland, Damotte, van der Lee, Costa, Kuulasmaa, Yang, de Rojas, Bis and Lambert2022), EA (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Wedow, Okbay, Kong, Maghzian, Zacher, Nguyen-Viet, Bowers, Sidorenko, Karlsson Linnér, Fontana, Kundu, Lee, Li, Li, Royer, Timshel, Walters, Willoughby and Cesarini2018), IQ (Savage et al., Reference Savage, Jansen, Stringer, Watanabe, Bryois, de Leeuw, Nagel, Awasthi, Barr, Coleman, Grasby, Hammerschlag, Kaminski, Karlsson, Krapohl, Lam, Nygaard, Reynolds, Trampush and Posthuma2018) and HI (Hill et al., Reference Hill, Davies, Ritchie, Skene, Bryois, Bell, Di Angelantonio, Roberts, Xueyi, Davies, Liewald, Porteous, Hayward, Butterworth, McIntosh, Gale and Deary2019). For the AD PRS, SNPs within 500 kb distance at either side of the APOE locus were excluded to ensure the exclusion of the APOE-associated signal. The APOE genotype was considered separately from the AD PRS (derived from SNPs rs429358 and rs7412) and was either obtained from genomewide SNP chip data (where either the APOE SNPs were directly genotyped on the array or imputed with a high degree of certainty) or directly genotyped using TaqMan SNP genotyping assays, as described in Lupton et al. (Reference Lupton, Medland, Gordon, Goncalves, MacGregor, Mackey, Young, Duffy, Visscher, Wray, Nyholt, Bain, Ferreira, Henders, Wallace, Montgomery, Wright and Martin2018). Furthermore, as both the GWAS for EA and IQ included potentially overlapping samples with the PISA cohort, we computed PRS using summary data from GWAS meta-analyses that exclude QIMR Berghofer cohorts, including any PISA participants and their family members (referred to as ‘leave one out’ (LOO) QIMRB summary statistics). Prior to estimating SBayesR PRS, we excluded low imputation quality (r 2 < 0.6), allele frequency (MAF < 0.01), nonautosomal, and strand-ambiguous variants. Imputed genotype dosage data were used to calculate PRS by multiplying the variant effect size by the dosage of the effect allele. Finally, the total sum was calculated across all variants. The SBayesR PRS was generated using Plink 2.0 (Purcell et al., Reference Purcell, Neale, Todd-Brown, Thomas, Ferreira, Bender, Maller, Sklar, de Bakker, Daly and Sham2007; Chang et al., Reference Chang, Chow, Tellier, Vattikuti, Purcell and Lee2015).

Statistical Analyses

We examined the association of PRS for AD, EA, IQ and HI PRS and also APOE ε4 carrier status with recruitment bias using a logistic regression model in GCTA (1.91.3 beta), which accounted for sex, age, and the first five genetic ancestry principal components (PCs) as fixed effects. Relatedness among individuals was accounted for as a random effect with a genetic relatedness matrix. The AD PRS excludes the APOE region and is therefore independent of APOE ϵ4. A Nagelkerke’s R 2 was used to estimate the variance explained by the PRS or APOE status.

Results

Demographic Factors

Participants who took part in PISA were, on average, older than those who did not (OR = 1.007; 95% CI [1.006, 1.008] per year of age) p value = 7.33E-30 (Table 1). The participation rate stratified by decade showed that the largest number of participants were in the 50−69 age range, representing 78.6% of the whole sample (Table 2). Female participants were more likely to participate in PISA than males (Table 1) (OR = 1.063; 95% CI [1.04, 1.08]) p value = 1.18E-12.

Table 1. Demographics of participants to take part in PISA

Note: 291 Missing data proportion of APOE.

Table 2. Participation rate by decade

Genetic Factors

The GREML analysis suggested the presence of genetic contribution to the likelihood of participating in PISA. The SNP-based heritability on the observed scale was 0.14 (SE = 0.013, p = <.0001, phenotypic variance of the observed scale ∼0.22).

The results of the association of genetic variables (PRS and APOE), sex and age with participation in the PISA study are shown in Table 3 and Figure 1. Neither AD PRS nor presence of APOE ε4 were associated with participation in PISA. The EA, IQ and HI PRS were positively associated with participating in PISA.

Table 3. The association of genetic variables, sex and age with participation in the PISA study, p < .0083 (Bonferroni corrected threshold)

Note: EA, educational attainment; AD, Alzheimer’s disease; HI, household income.

Figure 1. Forest plots depict odds ratios (OR) and 95% confidence intervals for the association between age, sex, PRS and presence of APOE ɛ4 with participation in the PISA study.

Discussion

We investigated systematic differences between individuals who decided to participate in the PISA study (i.e., complete the core survey module) with those who, despite being contacted and participating in previous studies, did not participate. Investigating variables that associate with study participation is invariably challenging, since data are required to be collected on individuals who are not willing to participate in a study. The PISA study offers a unique opportunity in this research area, as the recruitment pool consists of participants from previous studies, where genetic data has already been collected. PISA consists of middle-aged and older individuals aged ≥40 years with an aim of discovering markers of early AD pathology to identify modifiable risk factors and establish the very earliest phenotypic and neuronal signs of disease onset. Therefore, it is crucial to identify factors that are associated with the likelihood of recruitment into the study and have the potential to introduce bias in association estimates in downstream analyses.

Analysis of the relationship between sex, age and study participation revealed that older female individuals were more likely to participate in PISA (Tables 1 and 2, Figure 1). It is well known that women are more likely than men to participate in clinical research. Research into the factors that influence women’s decision to participate has found that decisions may be impacted not only by individual characteristics, but also by interpersonal relationships and social norms within the community (Baker et al., Reference Baker, Lavender and Tincello2005; Liu & Dipietro Mager, Reference Liu and Dipietro Mager2016). We found in our recruitment pool of middle-aged and older individuals that participants aged 50 and older were more likely to participate than those in their 40s, which may reflect a lack of spare time in the younger age group who are more likely to be of working age and with younger families. It is also possible that older participants perceived more personal risk of cognitive decline and dementia. The recruitment rate reduces once participants reach 70. These finding echoes those in studies of age-related diseases, where the odds of participation are greater in older participants (aged around 55−75 years) and reduced in younger age groups, and once participants reach over 75 years (Beard et al., Reference Beard, Lane, O’Fallon, Riggs and Melton1994; Benfante et al., Reference Benfante, Reed, MacLean and Kagan1989).

Using a PRS approach we tested whether the genetic liabilities to AD and causally associated traits are associated with the decision to participate in the PISA study. We tested for an association of EA, IQ and HI, which are highly related to each other with complex multifaceted relationships. We have previously shown all three to be causally associated with risk of AD at a genetic level using Mendelian randomization analysis, and that this causal association is led by the cognitive component of EA (intelligence). (Thorp et al., Reference Thorp, Mitchell, Gerring, Ong, Gharahkhani, Derks and Lupton2022).

We did not find evidence suggesting that the genetic risk of AD for both AD PRS (with APOE region removed) or APOE genotype are associated with recruitment bias in our sample of middle-aged and older research participants. However, we identified an association of PRS for key causal risk factors for AD, EA, IQ and HI. Previous studies have highlighted the strong relationship between education level and voluntary participation (Enzenbach et al., Reference Enzenbach, Wicklein, Wirkner and Loeffler2019; Lissner et al., Reference Lissner, Skoog, Andersson, Beckman, Sundh, Waern, Edin Zylberstein, Bengtsson and Björkelund2003; van Loon et al., Reference Van Loon, Tijhuis, Picavet, Surtees and Ormel2003). The over-representation of more highly educated individuals in epidemiological studies is likely due to greater health awareness and interest in science (Galea & Tracy, Reference Galea and Tracy2007). In addition, the association of low socioeconomic status with reduced participation in cohort studies and clinical trials has been well documented, and is thought to be due to several factors, including psychosocial and practical barriers to participation.(Howe et al., Reference Howe, Tilling, Galobardes and Lawlor2013; Stuber et al., Reference Stuber, Middel, Mackenbach, Beulens and Lakerveld2020).

A recent investigation of genomic predictors of participation in optional components of the large scale UK Biobank study (up to 451,306 participants) also identified an association of genetic predictors of educational duration, intelligence and lower levels of deprivation (using four measures) with participation, as well as a negative correlation with AD in this well powered sample. Causal associations were confirmed using Mendelian randomization (Tyrrell et al., Reference Tyrrell, Zheng, Beaumont, Hinton, Richardson, Wood, Davey Smith, Frayling and Tilling2021). We did not replicate these previous findings of an association of recruitment with genetic risk for AD, including an association with APOE e4 carrier status (Tyrrell et al. Reference Tyrrell, Zheng, Beaumont, Hinton, Richardson, Wood, Davey Smith, Frayling and Tilling2021). This may be due to decreased power in our dataset, or potentially the influence of an opposing bias where those with a strong family history of AD due to inheritance of APOE 4 are more likely to be compelled to volunteer for a study researching ageing and dementia.

Limitations of the present study must be acknowledged. The participants in the recruitment pool had all previously been successfully recruited into genetic studies, and are therefore more likely to volunteer again. These baseline studies are not representative of the current Australian population, comprising predominantly those of white European ancestry, an over-representation of females, and individuals recruited based on a phenotypes of interest, including twin pairs and their family members, as well as physical and psychiatric conditions. In this study, a lack of response was interpreted as a passive refusal to participate, but a subset of the recruitment pool may have out-of-date contact details. This was mitigated as much as possible by updating contact details using electoral roll information, contacting participants through several modalities including email, letter and phone when available, and using available death records to exclude those who had died. As much as possible we aimed to prevent accessibility issues for any participants without internet access or computing capabilities by making paper copies of the survey available upon request.

Overall, this work shows that in the PISA study key causal risk factors for AD related to cognition are associated with recruitment. Therefore, participants are not accurately representative of the population. Although this does not necessarily invalidate study findings in the investigation of risk factors for age-related cognitive decline and dementia, it must be considered when interpreting findings, specifically for informing analysis strategies, using the appropriate control variables and interpreting the generalizability of findings to the general population (Elwood, Reference Elwood2013; Rothman et al., Reference Rothman, Gallacher and Hatch2013).

Acknowledgments

We would like to acknowledge the contribution of the research participants. The population-based sample for PISA comprises of research participants who have taken part in genetic epidemiology studies led by NM (with colleagues and collaborators) at QIMR Berghofer and elsewhere over the past 40 years. Demographic and contact information as well as genomewide SNP chip data were utilized from these previous studies for participant selection and recruitment. Principal sources of funding for these early studies were from grants to NGM from Australian NHMRC and to Andrew Heath and Pam Madden (Washington University, St Louis) from NIH (mainly NIAAA and NIDA) and we gratefully acknowledge these contributions.

Financial support

The Prospective Imaging Study of Ageing (PISA) is funded by the National Health and Medical Research Council (Grant ID: APP1095227). LMGM is supported by a UQ Research Training Scholarship from The University of Queensland (UQ)

Competing interests

None.

Ethical standard

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Andrews, S. J., Fulton-Howard, B., O’Reilly, P., Marcora, E., Goate, A. M., & Collaborators of the Alzheimer’s Disease Genetics Consortium. (2021). Causal associations between modifiable risk factors and the Alzheimer’s phenome. Annals of Neurology, 89, 5465. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.25918 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, L., Lavender, T., & Tincello, D. (2005). Factors that influence women’s decisions about whether to participate in research: An exploratory study. Birth, 32, 6066. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0730-7659.2005.00346.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beard, C. M., Lane, A. W., O’Fallon, W. M., Riggs, B. L., & Melton, L. J., 3rd. (1994). Comparison of respondents and nonrespondents in an osteoporosis study. Annals of Epidemiology, 4, 398403. https://doi.org/10.1016/1047-2797(94)90075-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bellenguez, C., Küçükali, F., Jansen, I. E., Kleineidam, L., Moreno-Grau, S., Amin, N., Naj, A. C., Campos-Martin, R., Grenier-Boley, B., Andrade, V., Holmans, P. A., Boland, A., Damotte, V., van der Lee, S. J., Costa, M. R., Kuulasmaa, T., Yang, Q., de Rojas, I., Bis, J. C., … Lambert, J.-C. (2022). New insights into the genetic etiology of Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias. Nature Genetics, 54, 412436. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-022-01024-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benfante, R., Reed, D., MacLean, C., & Kagan, A. (1989). Response bias in the Honolulu Heart Program. American Journal of Epidemiology, 130, 10881100. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a115436 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, C. C., Chow, C. C., Tellier, L. C., Vattikuti, S., Purcell, S. M., & Lee, J. J. (2015). Second-generation PLINK: Rising to the challenge of larger and richer datasets. GigaScience, 4, 7. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13742-015-0047-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, K., Leung, Y. Y., Lee, W.-P., Voight, B., & Wang, L.-S. (2022). Polygenic Risk scores in Alzheimer’s disease genetics: Methodology, applications, inclusion, and diversity. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 89, 112. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-220025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elwood, J. M. (2013). Commentary: On representativeness. International Journal of Epidemiology, 42, 10141015. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyt101 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enzenbach, C., Wicklein, B., Wirkner, K., & Loeffler, M. (2019). Evaluating selection bias in a population-based cohort study with low baseline participation: the LIFE-Adult-Study. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 19, 135. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12874-019-0779-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galea, S., & Tracy, M. (2007). Participation rates in epidemiologic studies. Annals of Epidemiology, 17, 643653. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.annepidem.2007.03.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ganguli, M., Lee, C.-W., Hughes, T., Snitz, B. E., Jakubcak, J., Duara, R., & Chang, C.-C. H. (2015). Who wants a free brain scan? Assessing and correcting for recruitment biases in a population-based sMRI pilot study. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 9, 204212. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-014-9297-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ghanbari, R., Lovasi, G. S., & Bader, M. D. M. (2023). Exploring potential for selection bias in using survey data to estimate the association between institutional trust and depression. Annals of Epidemiology, 77, 6166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.annepidem.2022.11.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haring, R., Alte, D., Völzke, H., Sauer, S., Wallaschofski, H., John, U., & Schmidt, C. O. (2009). Extended recruitment efforts minimize attrition but not necessarily bias. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 62, 252260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2008.06.010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heath, A. C., Whitfield, J. B., Martin, N. G., Pergadia, M. L., Goate, A. M., Lind, P. A., McEvoy, B. P., Schrage, A. J., Grant, J. D., Chou, Y.-L., Zhu, R., Henders, A. K., Medland, S. E., Gordon, S. D., Nelson, E. C., Agrawal, A., Nyholt, D. R., Bucholz, K. K., Madden, P. A. F., & Montgomery, G. W. (2011). A quantitative-trait genome-wide association study of alcoholism risk in the community: Findings and implications. Biological Psychiatry, 70, 513518. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.028 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, W. D., Davies, N. M., Ritchie, S. J., Skene, N. G., Bryois, J., Bell, S., Di Angelantonio, E., Roberts, D. J., Xueyi, S., Davies, G., Liewald, D. C. M., Porteous, D. J., Hayward, C., Butterworth, A. S., McIntosh, A. M., Gale, C. R., & Deary, I. J. (2019). Genome-wide analysis identifies molecular systems and 149 genetic loci associated with income. Nature Communications, 10, 5741. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-13585-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howe, L. D., Tilling, K., Galobardes, B., & Lawlor, D. A. (2013). Loss to follow-up in cohort studies: Bias in estimates of socioeconomic inequalities. Epidemiology, 24, 19. https://doi.org/10.1097/EDE.0b013e31827623b1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jack, C. R., Jr, Bennett, D. A., Blennow, K., Carrillo, M. C., Dunn, B., Haeberlein, S. B., Holtzman, D. M., Jagust, W., Jessen, F., Karlawish, J., Liu, E., Molinuevo, J. L., Montine, T., Phelps, C., Rankin, K. P., Rowe, C. C., Scheltens, P., Siemers, E., Snyder, H. M., … Contributors. (2018). NIA-AA Research Framework: Toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 14, 535562. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2018.02.018 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, J. J., Wedow, R., Okbay, A., Kong, E., Maghzian, O., Zacher, M., Nguyen-Viet, T. A., Bowers, P., Sidorenko, J., Karlsson Linnér, R., Fontana, M. A., Kundu, T., Lee, C., Li, H., Li, R., Royer, R., Timshel, P. N., Walters, R. K., Willoughby, E. A., … Cesarini, D. (2018). Gene discovery and polygenic prediction from a genome-wide association study of educational attainment in 1.1 million individuals. Nature Genetics, 50, 11121121. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-018-0147-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, M., Kim, H., Sereika, S. M., Nissley, T. J., & Lingler, J. H. (2022). Willingness to participate in clinical research among individuals with cognitive impairment. Research in Gerontological Nursing, 15, 7684. https://doi.org/10.3928/19404921-20220131-01 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lissner, L., Skoog, I., Andersson, K., Beckman, N., Sundh, V., Waern, M., Edin Zylberstein, D., Bengtsson, C., & Björkelund, C. (2003). Participation bias in longitudinal studies: Experience from the population study of women in Gothenburg, Sweden. Scandinavian Journal of Primary Health Care, 21, 242247. https://doi.org/10.1080/02813430310003309-1693 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, K. A., & Dipietro Mager, N. A. (2016). Women’s involvement in clinical trials: Historical perspective and future implications. Pharmacy Practice, 14. https://doi.org/10.18549/PharmPract.2016.01.708 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loh, P.-R., Danecek, P., Palamara, P. F., Fuchsberger, C., Reshef, A, Y., K Finucane, H., Schoenherr, S., Forer, L., McCarthy, S., Abecasis, G. R., Durbin, R., Price, A. L. (2016). Reference-based phasing using the Haplotype Reference Consortium panel. Nature Genetics, 48, 14431448. https://doi.org/10.1038/ng.3679 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lupton, M. K., Medland, S. E., Gordon, S. D., Goncalves, T., MacGregor, S., Mackey, D. A., Young, T. L., Duffy, D. L., Visscher, P. M., Wray, N. R., Nyholt, D. R., Bain, L., Ferreira, M. A., Henders, A. K., Wallace, L., Montgomery, G. W., Wright, M. J., & Martin, N. G. (2018). Accuracy of Inferred APOE genotypes for a range of genotyping arrays and imputation reference panels. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 64, 4954. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-171104 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lupton, M. K., Robinson, G. A., Adam, R. J., Rose, S., Byrne, G. J., Salvado, O., Pachana, N. A., Almeida, O. P., McAloney, K., Gordon, S. D., Raniga, P., Fazlollahi, A., Xia, Y., Ceslis, A., Sonkusare, S., Zhang, Q., Kholghi, M., Karunanithi, M., Mosley, P. E., … Breakspear, M. (2021). A prospective cohort study of prodromal Alzheimer’s disease: Prospective Imaging Study of Ageing: Genes, Brain and Behaviour (PISA). NeuroImage. Clinical, 29, 102527. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102527 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Medland, S. E., Nyholt, D. R., Painter, J. N., McEvoy, B. P., McRae, A. F., Zhu, G., Gordon, S. D., Ferreira, M. A. R., Wright, M. J., Henders, A. K., Campbell, M. J., Duffy, D. L., Hansell, N. K., Macgregor, S., Slutske, W. S., Heath, A. C., Montgomery, G. W., & Martin, N. G. (2009). Common variants in the trichohyalin gene are associated with straight hair in Europeans. American Journal of Human Genetics, 85, 750755. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajhg.2009.10.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milani, S. A., Cottler, L. B., & Striley, C. W. (2021). Perceptions of research participation among a sample of Florida residents aged 50 and over reporting dementia. Ageing International. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12126-021-09441-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oswald, L. M., Wand, G. S., Zhu, S., & Selby, V. (2013). Volunteerism and self-selection bias in human positron emission tomography neuroimaging research. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 7, 163176. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-012-9210-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patten, S. B. (2000). Selection bias in studies of major depression using clinical subjects. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 53, 351357. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0895-4356(99)00215-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purcell, S., Neale, B., Todd-Brown, K., Thomas, L., Ferreira, M. A. R., Bender, D., Maller, J., Sklar, P., de Bakker, P. I. W., Daly, M. J., & Sham, P. C. (2007). PLINK: A tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. American Journal of Human Genetics, 81, 559575. https://doi.org/10.1086/519795 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rothman, K. J., Gallacher, J. E. J., & Hatch, E. E. (2013). Why representativeness should be avoided. International Journal of Epidemiology, 42, 10121014. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dys223 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savage, J. E., Jansen, P. R., Stringer, S., Watanabe, K., Bryois, J., de Leeuw, C. A., Nagel, M., Awasthi, S., Barr, P. B., Coleman, J. R. I., Grasby, K. L., Hammerschlag, A. R., Kaminski, J. A., Karlsson, R., Krapohl, E., Lam, M., Nygaard, M., Reynolds, C. A., Trampush, J. W., … Posthuma, D. (2018). Genome-wide association meta-analysis in 269,867 individuals identifies new genetic and functional links to intelligence. Nature Genetics, 50, 912919. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-018-0152-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stuber, J. M., Middel, C. N. H., Mackenbach, J. D., Beulens, J. W. J., & Lakerveld, J. (2020). Successfully recruiting adults with a low socioeconomic position into community-based lifestyle programs: A qualitative study on expert opinions. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17, 2764. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17082764 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thorp, J. G., Mitchell, B. L., Gerring, Z. F., Ong, J.-S., Gharahkhani, P., Derks, E. M., & Lupton, M. K. (2022). Genetic evidence that the causal association of educational attainment with reduced risk of Alzheimer’s disease is driven by intelligence. Neurobiology of Aging, 119, 127135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2022.07.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tyrrell, J., Zheng, J., Beaumont, R., Hinton, K., Richardson, T. G., Wood, A. R., Davey Smith, G., Frayling, T. M., & Tilling, K. (2021). Genetic predictors of participation in optional components of UK Biobank. Nature Communications, 12, 113. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-021-21073-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Loon, A. J. M., Tijhuis, M., Picavet, H. S. J., Surtees, P. G., & Ormel, J. (2003). Survey non-response in the Netherlands: Effects on prevalence estimates and associations. Annals of Epidemiology, 13, 105110. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1047-2797(02)00257-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, J., Benyamin, B., McEvoy, B. P., Gordon, S., Henders, A. K., Nyholt, D. R., Madden, P. A., Heath, A. C., Martin, N. G., Montgomery, G. W., Goddard, M. E., & Visscher, P. M. (2010). Common SNPs explain a large proportion of the heritability for human height. Nature Genetics, 42, 565569. https://doi.org/10.1038/ng.608 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, J., Lee, S. H., Goddard, M. E., & Visscher, P. M. (2011). GCTA: A tool for genome-wide complex trait analysis. American Journal of Human Genetics, 88A, 7682. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajhg.2010.11.011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zheng, X., Levine, D., Shen, J., Gogarten, S. M., Laurie, C., & Weir, B. S. (2012). A high-performance computing toolset for relatedness and principal component analysis of SNP data. Bioinformatics, 28, 33263328. https://doi.org/10.1093/bioinformatics/bts606 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographics of participants to take part in PISA

Figure 1

Table 2. Participation rate by decade

Figure 2

Table 3. The association of genetic variables, sex and age with participation in the PISA study, p < .0083 (Bonferroni corrected threshold)

Figure 3

Figure 1. Forest plots depict odds ratios (OR) and 95% confidence intervals for the association between age, sex, PRS and presence of APOE ɛ4 with participation in the PISA study.