Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T06:08:11.036Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Genetic and Environmental Relationship Between Childhood Behavioral Inhibition and Preadolescent Anxiety

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 January 2019

Jessica L. Bourdon*
Affiliation:
Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA
Jeanne E. Savage
Affiliation:
Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
Brad Verhulst
Affiliation:
Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA Department of Psychology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, USA
Dever M. Carney
Affiliation:
Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA
Melissa A. Brotman
Affiliation:
Emotion and Development Branch, National Institute of Mental Health, National Institutes of Health, Department of Health and Human Services, Bethesda, MD, USA
Daniel S. Pine
Affiliation:
Emotion and Development Branch, National Institute of Mental Health, National Institutes of Health, Department of Health and Human Services, Bethesda, MD, USA
Ellen Leibenluft
Affiliation:
Emotion and Development Branch, National Institute of Mental Health, National Institutes of Health, Department of Health and Human Services, Bethesda, MD, USA
Roxann Roberson-Nay
Affiliation:
Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA
John M. Hettema
Affiliation:
Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Jessica L. Bourdon, Email: jlbourdon@vcu.edu

Abstract

This study uses novel approaches to examine genetic and environmental influences shared between childhood behavioral inhibition (BI) and symptoms of preadolescent anxiety disorders. Three hundred and fifty-two twin pairs aged 9–13 and their mothers completed questionnaires about BI and anxiety symptoms. Biometrical twin modeling, including a direction-of-causation design, investigated genetic and environmental risk factors shared between BI and social, generalized, panic and separation anxiety. Social anxiety shared the greatest proportion of genetic (20%) and environmental (16%) variance with BI with tentative evidence for causality. Etiological factors underlying BI explained little of the risk associated with the other anxiety domains. Findings further clarify etiologic pathways between BI and anxiety disorder domains in children.

Type
Articles
Copyright
© The Author(s) 2019 

First characterized by Kagan and colleagues, childhood behavioral inhibition (BI) is a temperamental trait whereby children express shyness, restraint or other negative effect in response to novel people, objects or environments (Fox et al., Reference Fox, Henderson, Marshall, Nichols and Ghera2005; Garcia Coll et al., Reference Garcia Coll, Kagan and Reznick1984; Kagan et al., Reference Kagan, Reznick and Snidman1988). Roughly 20% of children initially scored as having high BI continue to exhibit elements of inhibition as they age into adolescence (Kagan, Reference Kagan, Zentner and Shiner2012), and approximately 40% later develop social anxiety (Clauss et al., Reference Clauss, Avery and Blackford2015; Clauss & Blackford, Reference Clauss and Blackford2012). While BI is a well-documented risk factor for later social anxiety disorder in adolescents and adults (Chronis-Tuscano et al., Reference Chronis-Tuscano, Degnan, Pine, Perez-Edgar, Henderson, Diaz and Fox2009; Clauss & Blackford, Reference Clauss and Blackford2012; Essex et al., Reference Essex, Klein, Slattery, Goldsmith and Kalin2010; Hirshfeld-Becker et al., Reference Hirshfeld-Becker, Biederman, Henin, Farone, Davis, Harrington and Rosenbaum2007, Reference Hirshfeld-Becker, Micco, Henin, Bloomfield, Biederman and Rosenbaum2008; Kagan et al., Reference Kagan, Reznick and Snidman1988; Muris et al., Reference Muris, van Braken, Arntz and Schouten2011; Schwartz et al., Reference Schwartz, Snidman and Kagan1999), it has also been associated with other forms of anxiety-related psychopathology such as specific phobia, panic disorder, separation anxiety, generalized anxiety and avoidant disorder earlier in childhood (Biederman et al., Reference Biederman, Hirshfeld-Becker, Rosenbaum, Herot, Friedman, Snidman and Faraone2001; Dyson et al., Reference Dyson, Klein, Olino, Dougherty and Durbin2011; Frenkel et al., Reference Frenkel, Fox, Pine, Walker, Degnan and Chronis-Tuscano2015; Muris et al., Reference Muris, Merckelbach, Wessel and van de Ven1999; Paulus et al., Reference Paulus, Backes, Sander, Weber and von Gontard2015). Development is complex, with many possible branching points and outcomes, but a hypothetical example could be that a child with elevated BI presents as more generally anxious during childhood. However, when they get to middle school, a time of increased social pressure, social anxiety symptoms become more prominent, and they are diagnosed with social anxiety disorder as an adolescent. Conversely, it is possible for a child to exhibit elevated BI and continue on a linear progression to develop later social anxiety. However, as they age further, they may learn to hide or better manage many of their symptoms and not meet criteria for a diagnosis. Thus, the overall goal of the present study was to clarify the etiological relationship between childhood BI and preadolescent anxiety domains using novel genetic epidemiological approaches.

BI has been well studied from developmental and clinical perspectives over the past three decades, but genetic epidemiological approaches have been underutilized. Past studies of singletons have examined childhood and/or adolescent and adult ages (i.e., Chronis-Tuscano et al., Reference Chronis-Tuscano, Degnan, Pine, Perez-Edgar, Henderson, Diaz and Fox2009; Frenkel et al., Reference Frenkel, Fox, Pine, Walker, Degnan and Chronis-Tuscano2015; Schwartz et al., Reference Schwartz, Snidman and Kagan1999), leaving preadolescence less well understood from a developmental perspective. Prior twin studies have reported moderate heritability through age 2 for BI (42–56%; Emde et al., Reference Emde, Plomin, Robinson, Corley, DeFries, Fulker and Zahn-Waxler1992; Plomin et al., Reference Plomin, Kagan, Emde, Reznick, Braungart, Robinson and DeFries1993; Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Kagan, Reznick and Corley1992) and similar estimates for all childhood anxiety disorders (28–60%; Lahey et al., Reference Lahey, Van Hulle, Singh, Waldman and Rathouz2011; Ogliari et al., Reference Ogliari, Citterio, Zanoni, Fagnani, Patriarca, Cirrincione and Battaglia2006, Reference Ogliari, Spatola, Presenti-Gritti, Medda, Penna, Sazi and Fagnani2010; Scaini et al., Reference Scaini, Belotti and Ogliari2014). However, no twin studies have directly examined the sources of shared genetic and environmental risk factors between BI and anxiety disorders in children. Further, a twin design allows for the examination of possible causal pathways from BI to strongly related anxiety domains even using a cross-sectional design. Pertinently, there is ongoing debate as to whether BI is a distinct construct or simply an earlier version of social anxiety (Clauss & Blackford, Reference Clauss and Blackford2012).

Preadolescence is a key time to study this shared risk, as symptoms of anxiety disorders are common but largely still at preclinical levels (Rapee et al., Reference Rapee, Schniering and Hudson2009; Rapee & Spence, Reference Rapee and Spence2004). Understanding this shared risk would complement prior work by further elaborating the etiology of anxiety disorders from a transdiagnostic, genetic perspective (i.e., Kagan et al., Reference Kagan, Snidman, Zentner and Peterson1999; Rapee & Spence, Reference Rapee and Spence2004) and possibly inform future prevention/intervention efforts. The current study specifically addresses two key knowledge gaps regarding the shared etiologic pathways between childhood BI and related anxiety domains by dissecting their developmental and phenotypic relationships at the genetic epidemiological level. The first aim of this study was to clarify the exact relationship between BI and key anxiety domains. For example, previously reported associations between BI and generalized anxiety, separation anxiety or panic symptoms (Biederman et al., Reference Biederman, Hirshfeld-Becker, Rosenbaum, Herot, Friedman, Snidman and Faraone2001; Dyson et al., Reference Dyson, Klein, Olino, Dougherty and Durbin2011; Frenkel et al., Reference Frenkel, Fox, Pine, Walker, Degnan and Chronis-Tuscano2015; Muris et al., Reference Muris, Merckelbach, Wessel and van de Ven1999; Paulus et al., Reference Paulus, Backes, Sander, Weber and von Gontard2015) may be indirect effects of correlations between these symptoms and social anxiety rather than a direct result of shared genetic and environmental effects. The second aim was to disaggregate the overlapping sources of liability between the anxiety domains for which BI shares the most genetic and environmental variance. Specifically, it will be tested whether shared genetic and environmental factors are simply correlational, or if evidence for causal pathways can be identified.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Families consisting of at least one parent and two twins between the ages of 9 and 13 were recruited from the Mid-Atlantic Twin Registry (Lilley & Silberg, Reference Lilley and Silberg2013). They were enrolled as part of the Virginia Commonwealth University Juvenile Anxiety Study between January 2013 and March 2016, which met all ethical and Institutional Review Board requirements for conduct and consent (Carney et al., Reference Carney, Moroney, Machlin, Hahn, Savage, Lee and Hettema2016). During an in-person laboratory visit, child participants were administered an array of experimental tasks and survey instruments that probed constructs putatively related to the development of anxiety and other internalizing disorders. Their parents completed survey measures about their children’s and their own behavior, emotions and life experiences. A portion of families were invited back for a second laboratory visit 2–4 weeks later to collect reliability data on all measures. Primary analyses in the current study were restricted to the first of two laboratory visits, with parent measures further restricted to maternal reports (89%). The final analytical sample included 352 families and 704 children (M age = 11.22; SD age = 1.41; female = 53%; monozygotic [MZ] = 114 pairs; dizygotic [DZ] = 238 pairs; 88% of full sample). This sample did not differ from the full sample on included demographic or clinical variables. To reduce genetic variance for future association studies, the sample recruited only Caucasian participants (see Carney et al., Reference Carney, Moroney, Machlin, Hahn, Savage, Lee and Hettema2016 for a more detailed discussion).

Measures

Two measures were included in the current study: a retrospective version of the Behavioral Inhibition Questionnaire (BIQ; Bishop et al., Reference Bishop, Spence and McDonald2003) and the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders — Parent and Child Versions (SCARED; Birmaher et al., Reference Birmaher, Khetarpal, Brent, Cully, Balach, Kaufman and Neer1997). A series of preliminary analyses confirmed their validity, reliability and appropriate usage in the primary analyses as discussed below.

BIQ

The BIQ is a comprehensive, 30-item questionnaire completed by parents to report on their child’s recent behaviors related to BI. It has previously demonstrated reliability and validity (Bishop et al., Reference Bishop, Spence and McDonald2003; Broeren & Muris, Reference Broeren and Muris2010) and adequate correlation with the gold standard behavioral assessment of BI (r = .46 among maternal raters, Bishop et al., Reference Bishop, Spence and McDonald2003; 74% agreement rate, Hudson & Dodd, Reference Hudson and Dodd2012). The BIQ has been successfully used in multiple studies of inhibition and anxiety in childhood, including assessing their neurophysiological and cognitive underpinnings (Clauss et al., Reference Clauss, Benningfield, Rao and Blackford2016; Fu et al., Reference Fu, Taber-Thomas and Perez-Edgar2015; Morales et al., Reference Morales, Taber-Thomas and Perez-Edgar2017; Taber-Thomas et al., Reference Taber-Thomas, Morales, Hillary and Perez-Edgar2016), predicting anxiety symptoms (Edwards et al., Reference Edwards, Rapee and Kennedy2010) and evaluating early interventions (Kennedy et al., Reference Kennedy, Rapee and Edwards2009).

For the current study, we included a retrospectively worded version of the BIQ in which the parents of preadolescents reported on behaviors related to BI when their child was between the ages of 2 and 6 (i.e., early childhood). We confirmed the reliability, validity and factor structure of this slightly modified version of the instrument in the current sample. Overall fit for the factor structure was comparable to that found in previous examinations of the BIQ (see Supplemental Table S1; Bishop et al., Reference Bishop, Spence and McDonald2003; Broeren & Muris, Reference Broeren and Muris2010; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Klein, Olino, Dyson, Doughtery and Durbin2011; Vreeke et al., Reference Vreeke, Muris, Mayer, Huijding, Bos, van der Veen and Verjeij2012); it also demonstrated appropriate reliability in a subset of N = 228 in this study (Cronbach’s alpha = .96; intraclass correlation for test–retest reliability = .87; 95% CI = .84, .89) and convergent and divergent validity. For consistency with other studies using the BIQ (Clauss et al., Reference Clauss, Benningfield, Rao and Blackford2016; Fu et al., Reference Fu, Taber-Thomas and Perez-Edgar2015), the total sum score was used in all analyses.

SCARED

The SCARED assesses anxiety disorder symptoms in children over the past 3 months via five subscales (social, generalized, separation, panic and school avoidance), where the first four are related to diagnostic syndromes and were used in these analyses. It has shown acceptable internal consistency (α = .90−.94) and test–retest reliability (r = .70−.90) in both prior studies and the current one (Birmaher et al., Reference Birmaher, Khetarpal, Brent, Cully, Balach, Kaufman and Neer1997; Carney et al., Reference Carney, Moroney, Machlin, Hahn, Savage, Lee and Hettema2016). We collected both parent and child responses to the SCARED since both types of report for any form of psychopathology are limited and potentially biased (De Los Reyes & Kazdin, Reference De Los Reyes and Kazdin2005). As expected, parent-rated BI more strongly correlated with parent-rated than child-rated recent anxiety (see Supplemental Table S2), yet preadolescent children potentially offered additional insight into their own symptomology. Information was combined across raters by including the average of the parent and child SCARED sum scores for each subscale in the analyses with parent-rated BI (see “Analyses” and “Results” below for analytic rationale).

Analyses

Primary statistical analyses were performed with the R software package (version 1.0.136; R Development Core Team, 2015) and twin analyses in OpenMx (OpenMx Development Team, 2011).

Descriptive statistics

The mean, standard deviation and range were calculated for each phenotype. Pearson correlations estimated the within-individual (i.e., phenotypic) associations between anxiety clusters and BI. MZ and DZ twin correlations were calculated individually for each phenotype as well as the cross-twin, cross-trait correlations between phenotypes. This information roughly predicts how the variance of the measures will decompose at the univariate and multivariate levels (Neale & Cardon, Reference Neale and Cardon1992).

Biometrical modeling overview

Biometrical structural equation modeling (SEM; Neale & Cardon, Reference Neale and Cardon1992) was performed to fit selected twin models to the phenotypes of BI and each anxiety subscale. In this process, the variance was decomposed into additive genetic (A), shared (familial) environmental (C) and unique environmental (E) factors contributing to the phenotype(s) in each model. The A covariance reflects the additive effect of individual alleles at genetic loci influencing a trait. On average, MZ twins shared 100% of their segregating genes, whereas DZ twins shared 50%, so the A factor contributed twice as much to the MZ twin correlation. The C covariance reflects environmental influences that make family members more alike compared with random pairs of individuals, and contributes equally to the MZ and DZ correlations. The E variances are uncorrelated between twins and reflect the aspects of the environment that are specific to each individual; it also includes measurement error. Beginning with full twin SEM in which all three (ACE) components were included, a series of nested submodels were tested in which paths of interest (A, C or both) were constrained to zero to examine their contributions to the phenotype. Model fit was compared using the likelihood ratio test (Wilks, Reference Wilks1938) and Akaike information criteria statistic (Akaike, Reference Akaike1987). These statistics tested whether there was a significant decrease in model fit after parameters in the full model were constrained to zero, indicative of the importance of the constrained parameter(s).

Due to the fact that both children and parents rated the SCARED but only parents rated the BIQ, a multiple rater model was tested to examine and reduce potential rater bias for the SCARED prior to the primary analyses. Specifically, parent and child SCARED scores were included as indicators for a latent ‘anxiety’ variable in the univariate SEM. This was done for each anxiety symptom cluster (social, generalized, separation and panic), and two models per cluster were tested: one where parent and child scores were allowed to freely load on the latent variable, and another where they were constrained to be equal. The best-fitting model was used in all further analyses to index each anxiety symptom cluster, respectively. Additionally, sex was treated as a covariate in all biometrical models due to its known association with various anxiety domains.

Primary analyses

For the first aim, biometrical SEM was used to fit a multivariate Cholesky decomposition of BI and all anxiety clusters to assess which etiological factors were shared between the phenotypes. The order of the clusters in the model was based on the strength of the phenotypic associations between each cluster and BI. At the multivariate level, each source of variance (A, C and E) was estimated as well as its contributions to the covariance between phenotypes. A series of nested submodels tested whether the shared covariance paths could be eliminated from the model, again comparing likelihood ratio test and Akaike information criteria fit statistics.

As part of the second aim, a direction-of-causation model was utilized to further explicate the relationship between BI and anxiety constructs with which it shared substantial variance (as revealed from the multivariate biometrical model in the first aim). Traditionally, the direction-of-causation models requires longitudinal or experimental data to show causality. In a cross-sectional, biometrical context, the direction-of-causation models leverage known differences between MZ and DZ twins’ shared genetics and environments. Specifically, these models utilize cross-twin, cross-trait correlations across three unique sources of variance (A, C, E or dominance [D]) (Gillespie & Martin, Reference Gillespie, Martin, Everitt and Howell2005; Heath et al., Reference Heath, Kesler, Neale, Hewitt, Eaves and Kendler1993; Verhulst & Estabrook, Reference Verhulst and Estabrook2012). To the extent that the sources of variance differ across the phenotypes (or that the magnitudes of the same sources of variance differ substantially), inferences can be made about whether a beta regression coefficient (as opposed to a correlation) best explains the relationship between two phenotypes. This was tested via five scenarios that were nested within a full Cholesky decomposition model (see Figure 1; Verhulst & Estabrook, Reference Verhulst and Estabrook2012): (a) phenotype 1 causes phenotype 2, (b) phenotype 2 causes phenotype 1, (c) reciprocal causation between phenotypes, (d) an external factor causes both (correlated liabilities), and (e) no association between the two phenotypes. Additionally, the proportion of variance accounted for in the ‘outcome’ phenotype in each direction-of-causation model was estimated and compared with the proportion of variance accounted for by the default correlated liability model (here, the bivariate Cholesky decomposition). The direction-of-causation model was then compared with a correlated liability model (i.e., a bivariate Cholesky decomposition with shared sources of variance) using likelihood ratio test and Akaike information criteria fit statistics.

Fig. 1. Full direction-of-causation model tested within a nested Cholesky decomposition model (top). Final model found in the present study (bottom).

Results

Correlations and Heritability Estimates

For all anxiety clusters, parent and child SCARED scores could be constrained to be equal in their multiple indicator models (see Supplemental Table S3). In other words, the average of the parent and child SCARED sum scores provides a reasonable representation of these anxiety domains for analyses with parent-rated BI and were used in all analyses. Within-person correlations among BI and each anxiety domain were consistent with the past findings (see Table 1; Dyson et al., Reference Dyson, Klein, Olino, Dougherty and Durbin2011; Muris et al., Reference Muris, Merckelbach, Wessel and van de Ven1999; Paulus et al., Reference Paulus, Backes, Sander, Weber and von Gontard2015). Consistent with prior reports, childhood BI had the strongest association with preadolescent social anxiety symptoms (r = .57; p < .001) and smaller but significant correlations with generalized anxiety, separation anxiety and panic symptoms(r = .11−.18; p < .05).

Table 1. Within-person correlations between BI and anxiety symptom clusters

Note: *p < .05; **p < .001.

All twin correlations for BI and each anxiety domain were significant except for the DZ correlations for BI and social anxiety symptoms (see Table 2 for these data and other descriptive statistics on these measures). For some scales, the DZ correlations were less than half of the MZ correlations, indicating possible influence of non-additive genetic factors such as genetic dominance or epistasis (denoted by D). ADE models that estimate non-additive genetic influences were tested but did not significantly improve model fit over ACE models for any of the phenotypes tested. This was unsurprising, as the power to detect non-additive genetic variance factors has been typically poor in practical twin sample sizes (Martin et al., Reference Martin, Eaves, Kearsey and Davies1978). The magnitudes of the remaining correlations indicated the possibility of both genetic and common environmental contributions to the variance of each phenotype. The cross-twin, cross-trait genetic correlations followed the same patterns of weaker cross-twin DZ correlations across phenotypes (see Supplemental Table S4).

Table 2. Descriptive and twin statistics for behavioral inhibition and anxiety symptom clusters

Note: Bold type indicates the model used in primary analyses. *p < .05; **p < .001.

Heritability estimates were calculated for each phenotype within the multivariate Cholesky decomposition (see below) to assess consistency with past literature. The estimated heritability of retrospective childhood BI was higher than that of concurrent BI in toddlers (see Table 3; h 2 = .76; confidence interval = .65−.84; Emde et al., Reference Emde, Plomin, Robinson, Corley, DeFries, Fulker and Zahn-Waxler1992; Plomin et al., Reference Plomin, Kagan, Emde, Reznick, Braungart, Robinson and DeFries1993; Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Kagan, Reznick and Corley1992). For the anxiety clusters, heritability estimates were slightly different than previously reported (Lahey et al., Reference Lahey, Van Hulle, Singh, Waldman and Rathouz2011; Ogliari et al., Reference Ogliari, Citterio, Zanoni, Fagnani, Patriarca, Cirrincione and Battaglia2006, Reference Ogliari, Spatola, Presenti-Gritti, Medda, Penna, Sazi and Fagnani2010; Scaini et al., Reference Scaini, Belotti and Ogliari2014) except for social anxiety, which was consistent with the past studies (h 2 = .52). Specifically, heritability was slightly lower for generalized anxiety (h 2 = .49) and slightly higher for both separation anxiety (h 2 = .62) and panic (h 2 = .68).

Table 3. Variance and covariance components for the best-fitting multivariate model between BI and anxiety symptom clusters (95% confidence intervals)

Note: Variance components above are equal to the squares of the standardized path estimates seen in Figure 2. Within each phenotype, these should add up to 1.00, but may not due to rounding error.

Note on variance component naming: A11/E11 = behavioral inhibition symptoms; A22/E22 = social anxiety symptoms; A21/E21 = shared between BI and social anxiety symptoms; A33/E33 = generalized anxiety symptoms; A31/E31 = shared between BI and generalized anxiety symptoms; A44/E55 = separation anxiety symptoms; A41/E41 = shared between BI and separation anxiety symptoms; A55/E55 = panic symptoms; A51/E51 = shared between BI and panic symptoms.

All other paths represent shared variance between anxiety symptom clusters.

Fig. 2. Standardized path estimates from the final multivariate model depicting the shared paths between BI and anxiety symptom clusters and unique paths only (shared inter-anxiety paths not included for simplicity but can be found in the supplementary material).

Relationship Between BI and All Anxiety Clusters

The phenotypic ordering in the multivariate model was based on decreasing magnitude of correlations found between BI and each anxiety domain (Table 1): BI, social anxiety, generalized anxiety, separation anxiety and panic (see Table 3, Figure 2 and Supplemental Table S5).

Consistent with past findings for each individual phenotype, only additive genetic (A) and unique environment (E) significantly contributed to this multivariate relationship (Lahey et al., Reference Lahey, Van Hulle, Singh, Waldman and Rathouz2011; Ogliari et al., Reference Ogliari, Citterio, Zanoni, Fagnani, Patriarca, Cirrincione and Battaglia2006, Reference Ogliari, Spatola, Presenti-Gritti, Medda, Penna, Sazi and Fagnani2010; Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Kagan, Reznick and Corley1992; Scaini et al., Reference Scaini, Belotti and Ogliari2014). At least some proportion of the genetic or environmental risk of each anxiety cluster was shared with BI (see Figure 2). Specifically, social anxiety shared the most of its variance with BI (A = .20; E = .16), followed in smaller magnitudes by generalized anxiety (E = .07), separation (E = .05) and panic (E = .01).

There was also significant shared variance between the anxiety clusters (see Table 3), although not as much as previously reported in this age group (Ogliari et al., Reference Ogliari, Spatola, Presenti-Gritti, Medda, Penna, Sazi and Fagnani2010). Generalized and separation anxiety shared the most genetic variance (A = .14), while social and generalized anxiety shared the most environmental variance (E = .04). A Cholesky decomposition of the anxiety measures alone revealed that, except for social anxiety, these lower between-domain covariance estimates were not due to residual covariance accounted for by including BI in the model (see Supplemental Table S6).

Relationship Between BI and social Anxiety

Due to the modest but significant amounts of additive genetic and unique environmental variance shared between BI and social anxiety but not with other anxiety clusters, the direction-of-causation model was only tested between these two phenotypes. This model with social anxiety symptoms regressed onto BI fit significantly better than the other direction-of-causation models tested (BI regressed onto social anxiety, joint causation, no association), notably ruling out these other three possibilities as explanations for the relationship between BI and social anxiety. When compared with the correlated liabilities model, which represents an external unmeasured factor causing both phenotypes, the model with BI causing social anxiety was not significantly worse; it estimated a small but notable regression coefficient (β = .047; see Table 4, Figure 1 and Supplemental Table S7). Both models had comparable parameter estimates and fit indices, making it important to assess the amount of social anxiety’s variance accounted for by BI in each model to further explain their relationship from a genetically informed perspective.

Table 4. Variance components and proportion of social anxiety’s variance accounted for by BI in the best-fitting correlated liabilities and direction-of-causation models

Note: Variance components above are equal to the squares of the standardized path estimates. Within each phenotype, these should add up to 1.00, but may not due to rounding error.

Note on path naming: A21/E21 = shared between BI and social anxiety symptoms; β1 = causal path from BI to social anxiety symptoms; R 2 = amount of social anxiety’s variance that is accounted for by BI.

BI in both models accounted for a similar proportion of social anxiety’s variance. Specifically, 37% of social anxiety’s variance (24% of the A and 13% of the E variance) was accounted for by BI in the correlated liabilities model compared with 38% (30% of the A and 8% of the E variance) in the direction-of-causation model. Taken together with their comparable fit to these data, this implies that a direction-of-causation model with a single beta coefficient from BI to social anxiety symptoms modestly informs their relationship beyond the correlated risk structure.

Discussion

Using novel approaches that complement existing developmental research, the current study examined the shared genetic and environmental influences between BI and anxiety symptomology. The first aim of the study sought to clarify the etiology between BI and related anxiety domains, and the second examined the potential causality between BI and the anxiety domain(s) with most substantial genetic and environmental overlap with BI from the first aim. Findings explicate prior reported phenotypic associations of BI with various anxiety symptoms in childhood and its role as a particular risk factor for later social anxiety. It was found that retrospectively reported childhood BI shared both genetic and environmental variance with preadolescent social anxiety symptoms, but there was little to no genetic overlap between BI and other domains of anxiety symptomatology. Tentative evidence was also found for causality between BI and social anxiety.

These findings clarify that associations between childhood BI and preadolescent anxiety symptoms other than social anxiety are primarily a function of shared environmental influences and not simply indirect effects from the correlations between social anxiety and other anxiety clusters. The residual variance of social anxiety increased when BI was taken out of the model, but the genetic and environmental components for the rest of the anxiety clusters and their interrelationships remained unchanged. This implies that associations found in the model that included BI are fairly robust. If the relationship between BI and the other clusters was due to correlations with social anxiety, then their variance components should have differed in the model without BI. Given the generally more stable effects of genetic-versus-environmental influences across development, this finding helps clarify the more robust, longitudinal relationship seen between early BI and later social anxiety disorder compared with other anxiety disorders.

Further, there is tentative evidence that the relationship between BI and social anxiety is causal. A significant proportion of the variance of preadolescent social anxiety symptoms was explained by childhood BI, specifically about a quarter of the additive genetic variance and a tenth of the unique environmental variance. While it had been previously shown that about 40% of children with BI later develop social anxiety (Clauss et al., Reference Clauss, Avery and Blackford2015; Clauss & Blackford, Reference Clauss and Blackford2012), the current findings suggest a particularly strong, etiologically relevant link. Our twin direction-of-causation model provided a potential approach for distinguishing correlation and direct causation between etiological sources of covariance between BI and social anxiety. The model fits were not significantly worse, and variance estimates were on par with those of the correlated model, implying that either is an appropriate way to represent the relationship between BI and social anxiety disorder. The direction-of-causation model provides an etiologically informed twin version of a linear regression, bridging the gap between genetically informed and behavioral-based studies.

These findings contribute to two key threads in the BI and social anxiety literature. There has been a long-standing debate regarding the developmental distinction between BI and social anxiety disorder (Clauss & Blackford, Reference Clauss and Blackford2012). The current study adds a genetically informed perspective to this discussion by demonstrating that both shared genetic and environmental factors partially, yet substantially, underpin the relationship between BI and social anxiety. This expands upon past longitudinal studies and expert reviews (Fox et al., Reference Fox, Henderson, Marshall, Nichols and Ghera2005; Kagan et al., Reference Kagan, Snidman, Zentner and Peterson1999; Rapee & Spence, Reference Rapee and Spence2004). Importantly, these findings also suggest that there are etiologic distinctions, supporting the hypothesis that these phenotypes are not two measures of the same underlying construct.

In addition, these results add to the growing evidence that childhood BI is a putative endophenotype for later social anxiety. Endophenotypes are ‘intermediate’ phenotypes that lie in the etiological pathway between genetic variation and a disorder (Cannon & Keller, Reference Cannon and Keller2006; Gottesman & Gould, Reference Gottesman and Gould2003). BI has previously been shown to meet a subset of the basic requirements as an endophenotype for social anxiety, including association with the disorder and being itself heritable. The present study adds further support to these two criteria and provides the first firm evidence for a third requisite criterion: co-segregation of the two phenotypes within families (instantiated here as cross-phenotype correlations within twin pairs). Furthermore, these results suggest that this familial coaggregation is primarily due to additive genetic factors. This is the first study conducted on a substantially sized, genetically informative sample that includes measures of both phenotypes since Kagan’s longitudinal studies of BI (Kagan et al., Reference Kagan, Reznick and Snidman1988).

There are additional potential strengths and insights provided by this study design. We modified the BIQ to a retrospective parent report in an attempt to, at least partially, capture the mother’s recollection of her child’s BI at an earlier age. That version was shown to be similarly reliable and valid as prior versions of the BIQ (however also see limitations below). Additionally, treating the included phenotypes as quantitative traits provided unique advantages. Statistical power to detect effects with this method was generally increased compared with binary categories. The use of quantitative measures of psychopathology is consistent with recent efforts by the National Institute of Mental Health to shift research toward dimensional constructs that potentially cut across diagnostic boundaries (Cuthbert & Insel, Reference Cuthbert and Insel2013; Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn and Wang2010). Finally, our finding suggests that retrospective parent report of BI assesses a strongly heritable form of inhibited temperament in this age group. This adds genetic evidence to existing rationale for measuring BI in early childhood and provides further support that parent report is a viable approach to assessing the trait when direct laboratory assessment is unavailable (Bishop et al., Reference Bishop, Spence and McDonald2003; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Rhee, Corley, Friedman, Hewitt and Robinson2012).

The results of this study should be interpreted in the context of several limitations. First, using a retrospective report of BI could have limited the reliability and validity of the measured trait due to recall biases such as parents of high-anxiety preadolescents reporting higher levels of early BI (McPhail & Haines, Reference McPhail and Haines2010). However, there is little evidence that such potential bias in retrospective reports outweighs their utility (Hardt & Rutter, Reference Hardt and Rutter2004). This limitation, together with moderate stability of BI over development (Fox et al., Reference Fox, Henderson, Marshall, Nichols and Ghera2005), suggests that our assessment likely provides a compromise between past and current inhibition severity. Second, the use of the BIQ to index BI only moderately aligned with BI obtained by behavioral observation methods. As already mentioned, however, the BIQ is a reliable measure of inhibited temperament that has often been used in recent studies of BI (i.e., Bishop et al., Reference Bishop, Spence and McDonald2003; Broeren & Muris, Reference Broeren and Muris2010; Clauss et al., Reference Clauss, Benningfield, Rao and Blackford2016; Edwards et al., Reference Edwards, Rapee and Kennedy2010; Fu et al., Reference Fu, Taber-Thomas and Perez-Edgar2015; Hudson & Dodd, Reference Hudson and Dodd2012; Kennedy et al., Reference Kennedy, Rapee and Edwards2009; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Klein, Olino, Dyson, Doughtery and Durbin2011; Morales et al., Reference Morales, Taber-Thomas and Perez-Edgar2017; Taber-Thomas et al., Reference Taber-Thomas, Morales, Hillary and Perez-Edgar2016; Vreeke et al., Reference Vreeke, Muris, Mayer, Huijding, Bos, van der Veen and Verjeij2012). Third, our findings may not be generalizable to a wider population since the sample was made up of entirely Caucasian participants selected to limit genetic variance for the larger aims of the study (Carney et al., Reference Carney, Moroney, Machlin, Hahn, Savage, Lee and Hettema2016). Fourth, twin direction-of-causation models ideally should examine phenotypes with different sources of variance (Gillespie & Martin, Reference Gillespie, Martin, Everitt and Howell2005; Heath et al., Reference Heath, Kesler, Neale, Hewitt, Eaves and Kendler1993; Verhulst & Estabrook, Reference Verhulst and Estabrook2012). The sources of variance between BI and social anxiety symptoms were the same (additive genetics and unique environment) but of sufficiently different magnitudes to make the model appropriate for use in the current study. Finally, the DZ correlations for BI and social anxiety symptoms were low and not significantly different from zero (see Table 2). This has also been reported in past studies in which BI was assessed via behavioral observation (Plomin et al., Reference Plomin, Kagan, Emde, Reznick, Braungart, Robinson and DeFries1993; Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Kagan, Reznick and Corley1992; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Rhee, Corley, Friedman, Hewitt and Robinson2012), suggesting this may likely be inherent in various measures of BI. There are a few possible explanations for this, which we address in the supplemental text.

Conclusions

This was the first study to examine the relationship between childhood BI and a broad array of preadolescent anxiety symptom clusters from a genetic epidemiological perspective. Of the anxiety symptoms examined, only social anxiety shared a significant proportion of genetic and environmental factors with childhood BI. It also found tentative evidence for a potentially causal pathway that partially explains the relationship between these two phenotypes, expanding extant knowledge about the progression of BI and the etiology of social anxiety. Current findings support and extend past research indicating that childhood BI is most robustly a risk factor for later social anxiety, despite associations with other anxiety domains. They also suggest that childhood BI and later social anxiety disorder are related but distinct phenotypes, with early BI functioning as a potential developmental endophenotype for later social anxiety. While current treatment implications are unclear, such research adds etiological insight into anxiety risk prediction that may be useful for clinicians to consider. Future studies in this line of research could expand the clinical impact of this work by informing clinicians’ treatment decisions. For example, the specific mechanisms of the relationship between BI and social anxiety need further exploration, including the developmental progression to social anxiety disorder in adolescence. Such knowledge can help to guide early intervention and prevention efforts aimed at social anxiety outcomes in middle to late childhood.

Supplementary Material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2018.73.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all children and parents of the Virginia Commonwealth University Juvenile Anxiety Study for their participation.

Financial Support

The Virginia Commonwealth University Juvenile Anxiety Study was funded by the National Institutes of Health’s (NIH) National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) grants NIH-R01MH098055 (PI: J.M.H.) and NIMH-IRP-ziamh002781 (PI: D.S.P.). J.L.B. was supported by the NIMH grant T32MH020030 (PI: M. Neale).

Conflict of Interest

The authors have no conflicts of interest to report.

References

Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52, 317332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biederman, J., Hirshfeld-Becker, D. R., Rosenbaum, J. F., Herot, C., Friedman, D., Snidman, N., … Faraone, S. V. (2001). Further evidence of association between behavioral inhibition and social anxiety in children. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 16731679.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birmaher, B., Khetarpal, S., Brent, D., Cully, M., Balach, L., Kaufman, J., & Neer, S. M. (1997). The screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders (SCARED): Scale construction and psychometric characteristics. Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 545553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, G., Spence, S. H., & McDonald, C. (2003). Can parents and teachers provide a reliable and valid report of behavioral inhibition? Child Development, 74, 18991917.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broeren, S., & Muris, P. (2010). A psychometric evaluation of the behavioral inhibition questionnaire in a non-clinical sample of Dutch children and adolescents. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 41, 214229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cannon, T. D., & Keller, M. C. (2006). Endophenotypes in the genetic analyses of mental disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 2, 267290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carney, D. M., Moroney, E., Machlin, L., Hahn, S., Savage, J. E., Lee, M., … Hettema, J. M. (2016). The twin study of negative valence emotional constructs. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 19, 456464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chronis-Tuscano, A., Degnan, K. A., Pine, D. S., Perez-Edgar, K., Henderson, H. A., Diaz, Y., … Fox, N. A. (2009). Stable early maternal report of behavioral inhibition predicts lifetime social anxiety disorder in adolescence. Journal of American Academy of Child Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 928935.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clauss, J. A., Avery, S. N., & Blackford, J. U. (2015). The nature of individual differences in inhibited temperament and risk for psychiatric disease: A review and meta-analysis. Progress in Neurobiology, 127–128, 2345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clauss, J. A., Benningfield, M. M., Rao, U., & Blackford, J. U. (2016). Altered prefrontal cortex function marks heightened anxiety risk in children. Journal of the American Academy of Child Adolescent Psychiatry, 55, 809816.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clauss, J. A., & Blackford, J. U. (2012). Behavioral inhibition and risk for developing social anxiety disorder: A meta-analytic study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51, 10661075.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuthbert, B. N., & Insel, T. R. (2013). Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: The seven pillars of RDoC. BMC Medicine, 11, 126133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Los Reyes, A., & Kazdin, A. E. (2005). Informant discrepancies in the assessment of childhood psychopathology: A critical review, theoretical framework, and recommendations for further study. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 483509.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dyson, M. W., Klein, D. N., Olino, T. M., Dougherty, L. R., & Durbin, C. E. (2011). Social and nonsocial behavioral inhibition in preschool-age children: Differential associations with parent-reports of temperament and anxiety. Child Psychiatry Human Development, 42, 390405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edwards, S. L., Rapee, R. M., & Kennedy, S. (2010). Prediction of anxiety symptoms in preschool-aged children: Examinations of maternal and paternal perspectives. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 313321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emde, R. N., Plomin, R., Robinson, J., Corley, R., DeFries, J., Fulker, D. W., … Zahn-Waxler, C. (1992). Temperament, emotion, and cognition at fourteen months: The MacArthur Longitudinal Twin Study. Child Development, 63, 14371455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Essex, M. J., Klein, M. H., Slattery, M. J., Goldsmith, H. H., & Kalin, N. H. (2010). Early risk factors and developmental pathways to chronic high inhibition and social anxiety disorder in adolescence. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 4046.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, N. A., Henderson, H. A., Marshall, P. J., Nichols, K. E., & Ghera, M. M. (2005). Behavioral inhibition: Linking biology and behavior within a developmental framework. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 235262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frenkel, T. I., Fox, N. A., Pine, D. S., Walker, O. L., Degnan, K. A., & Chronis-Tuscano, A. (2015). Early childhood behavioral inhibition, adult psychopathology and the buffering effects of adolescent social networks: A twenty-year prospective study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56, 10651073.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fu, X., Taber-Thomas, B. C., & Perez-Edgar, K. (2015). Frontolimbic functioning during threat-related attention: Relations to early behavioral inhibition and anxiety in children. Biological Psychology, 122, 98109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garcia Coll, C., Kagan, J., & Reznick, J. S. (1984). Behavioral inhibition in young children. Child Development, 55, 10051019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillespie, N. A., & Martin, N. G. (2005). Direction of causation models. In Everitt, B. S. & Howell, D. C. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of statistics in behavioral science (pp. 13631370). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Gottesman, I. I., & Gould, T. D. (2003). The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: Etymology and strategic intentions. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 636645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardt, J., & Rutter, M. (2004). Validity of adult retrospective reports of adverse childhood experiences: Review of the evidence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 260273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heath, A. C., Kesler, R. C., Neale, M. C., Hewitt, J. K., Eaves, L. J., & Kendler, K. S. (1993). Testing hypotheses about direction of causation using cross-sectional family data. Behavior Genetics, 23, 2950.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirshfeld-Becker, D. R., Biederman, J., Henin, A., Farone, S. V., Davis, S., Harrington, K., & Rosenbaum, J. F. (2007). Behavioral inhibition in preschool children at risk is a specific predictor of middle childhood social anxiety: A five-year follow-up. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 28, 225233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirshfeld-Becker, D. R., Micco, J., Henin, A., Bloomfield, A., Biederman, J., & Rosenbaum, J. (2008). Behavioral inhibition. Depression and Anxiety, 25, 357367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hudson, J. L., & Dodd, H. F. (2012). Informing early intervention: Preschool predictors of anxiety disorders in middle childhood. PLoS ONE, 8, e42359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., … Wang, P. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): Toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 748751.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagan, J., Reznick, S., & Snidman, N. (1988). Biological bases of childhood shyness. Science, 240, 167171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kagan, J. (2012). The biography of behavioral inhibition. In Zentner, M. & Shiner, R. L. (Eds.), Handbook of Temperament (pp. 6982). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Kagan, J., Snidman, N., Zentner, M., & Peterson, E. (1999). Infant temperament and anxious symptoms in school age children. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 209224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kennedy, S. J., Rapee, R. M., & Edwards, S. L. (2009). A selective intervention program for inhibited preschool-aged children of parents with an anxiety disorder: Effects on current anxiety disorders and temperament. Journal of the American Academy of Child Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 602609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, J., Klein, D. N., Olino, T. M., Dyson, M. W., Doughtery, L. R., & Durbin, C. E. (2011). Psychometric properties of the Behavioral Inhibition Questionnaire in preschool children. Journal of Personality Assessment, 93, 545555.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lahey, B. B., Van Hulle, C. A., Singh, A. L., Waldman, I. D., & Rathouz, P. J. (2011). Higher-order genetic and environmental structure of prevalent forms of child and adolescent psychopathology. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68, 181189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lilley, E. C., & Silberg, J. L. (2013). The mid-atlantic twin registry, revisited. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16, 424428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, N. G., Eaves, L. J., Kearsey, M. J., & Davies, P. (1978). The power of the classical twin study. Heredity, 40, 97116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McPhail, S., & Haines, T. (2010). Response shift, recall bias, and their effect on measuring change in health-related quality of life amongst older hospital patients. Health and Quality Outcomes, 8, 6574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morales, S., Taber-Thomas, B. C., & Perez-Edgar, K. E. (2017). Patterns of attention to threat across tasks in behaviorally inhibited children at risk for anxiety. Developmental Science, 20, e12391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muris, P., Merckelbach, I., Wessel, M., & van de Ven, M. (1999). Psychopathological correlates of self-reported behavioural inhibition in normal children. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 575584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muris, P., van Braken, A. M. L., Arntz, A., & Schouten, E. (2011). Behavioral inhibition as a risk factor for the development of childhood anxiety disorders: A longitudinal study. Journal of Child Family Studies, 20, 157170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neale, M. C., & Cardon, L. R. (1992). Methodology for genetic studies of twin and families. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ogliari, A., Citterio, A., Zanoni, A., Fagnani, C., Patriarca, V., Cirrincione, R., … Battaglia, M. (2006). Genetic and environmental influences on anxiety dimensions in Italian twins evaluated using the SCARED questionnaire. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 20, 760777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ogliari, A., Spatola, C. A., Presenti-Gritti, P., Medda, E., Penna, L., Sazi, M. A., … Fagnani, C. (2010). The role of genes and environment in shaping co-occurrence of DSM-IV defined anxiety dimensions among Italian twins aged 8–17. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24, 433439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
OpenMx Development Team. (2011). OpenMx official documentation. Retrieved from http://openmx.psyc.virginia.edu/docs/OpenMx/latest/Google Scholar
Paulus, F. W., Backes, A., Sander, C. S., Weber, M., & von Gontard, A. (2015). Anxiety disorders and behavioral inhibition in preschool children: A population-based study. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 46, 150157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plomin, R., Kagan, J., Emde, R. N., Reznick, J. S., Braungart, J. M., Robinson, J., … DeFries, J. C. (1993). Genetic change and continuity from fourteen to twenty months: The MacArthur Longitudinal Twin Study. Child Development, 64, 13541376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Development Core Team. (2015). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Rapee, R. M., Schniering, C. A., & Hudson, J. L. (2009). Anxiety disorders during childhood and adolescence: Origins and treatment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 5, 311341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapee, R. M., & Spence, S. H. (2004). The etiology of social phobia: Empirical evidence and an initial model. Clinical Psychology Review, 24, 737767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, J. L., Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Corley, R. (1992). The heritability of inhibited and uninhibited behavior: A twin study. Developmental Psychology, 28, 10301037.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scaini, S., Belotti, R., & Ogliari, A. (2014). Genetic and environmental contributions to social anxiety across different ages: A meta-analytic approach to twin data. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 28, 650656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, C. E., Snidman, N., & Kagan, J. (1999). Adolescent social anxiety as an outcome of inhibited temperament in childhood. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 10081015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, A. K., Rhee, S. H., Corley, R. P., Friedman, N. P., Hewitt, J. K., & Robinson, J. L. (2012). The magnitude of genetic and environmental influences on parental and observational measures of behavioral inhibition and shyness in toddlerhood. Behavioral Genetics, 42, 764777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taber-Thomas, B. C., Morales, S., Hillary, F. G., & Perez-Edgar, K. E. (2016). Altered topography of intrinsic functional connectivity in childhood risk for social anxiety. Depression and Anxiety, 33, 9951004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verhulst, B., & Estabrook, R. (2012). Using genetic information to test causal relationships in cross-sectional data. Journal of Theoretical Politics, 24, 238344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vreeke, L. J., Muris, P., Mayer, B., Huijding, J., Bos, A. E. R., van der Veen, M., … Verjeij, F. (2012). The assessment of an inhibited, anxiety-prone temperament in a Dutch multi-ethnic population of preschool children. European Child Adolescent Psychiatry, 21, 623633.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilks, S. S. (1938). The large-sample distribution of the likelihood ratio for testing composite hypotheses. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 9, 670–662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Full direction-of-causation model tested within a nested Cholesky decomposition model (top). Final model found in the present study (bottom).

Figure 1

Table 1. Within-person correlations between BI and anxiety symptom clusters

Figure 2

Table 2. Descriptive and twin statistics for behavioral inhibition and anxiety symptom clusters

Figure 3

Table 3. Variance and covariance components for the best-fitting multivariate model between BI and anxiety symptom clusters (95% confidence intervals)

Figure 4

Fig. 2. Standardized path estimates from the final multivariate model depicting the shared paths between BI and anxiety symptom clusters and unique paths only (shared inter-anxiety paths not included for simplicity but can be found in the supplementary material).

Figure 5

Table 4. Variance components and proportion of social anxiety’s variance accounted for by BI in the best-fitting correlated liabilities and direction-of-causation models

Supplementary material: File

Bourdon et al. supplementary material

Bourdon et al. supplementary material 1

Download Bourdon et al. supplementary material(File)
File 48.6 KB