Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T08:23:36.180Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Twins Reared Apart and Twins in Families: The Findings Behind the Fascination

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 July 2018

Nancy L. Segal*
Affiliation:
Psychology Department, California State University, Fullerton, CA, USA
Yesika S. Montoya
Affiliation:
School of Social Work, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
Erika N. Becker
Affiliation:
Psychology Department, California State University, Fullerton, CA, USA
*
address for correspondence: Nancy L. Segal, Psychology Department, California State University, Fullerton, CA 92834, USA. E-mail: nsegal@fullerton.edu

Abstract

Dr. Irving I. Gottesman was very creative in applying twin research designs to test hypotheses and predictions about human behavior. Two designs stand out among others: twins reared apart from birth and the twin-family study. I (NS) was inspired to incorporate both of these approaches into my own research programs at California State University, Fullerton. Gottesman's involvement and contributions to reared-apart twin research are described, followed by selected findings from the Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart (MISTRA), the Fullerton Study of Chinese Twins Reared Apart (CTA), a case report on separated monozygotic (MZ) female twins from South Korea, and doubly exchanged MZ male twin pairs from Colombia, South America. Recent findings from an ongoing twin-family study of social closeness, and future directions, are also presented.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2018 

Twins reared apart from birth are a rare unplanned experiment, allowing unique insights into genetic and environmental factors underlying human behavior. Such studies are an informative variation of the classic monozygotic (MZ)dizygotic (DZ) twin comparison. That is because similarities between reared-together twins may reflect both shared genes and shared environments, whereas similarities between reared-apart twins offer direct estimates of genetic influence (Segal, Reference Segal2012). The origins, methods, findings, and implications from four major reared-apart twin studies are available in comprehensive volumes published when these studies concluded (Juel-Nielsen, Reference Juel-Nielsen1965; Reference Juel-Nielsen1980; Newman et al., Reference Newman, Freeman and Holzinger1937; Segal, Reference Segal2012; Shields, Reference Shields1962). Results from other reared-apart twin projects have also been reported in the scientific literature (see, e.g., Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Thornton and Pedersen2000; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Gao, Lv, Yu, Wang, Liao and Wang2015). Gottesman was intensely interested in the research possibilities offered by reared-apart twins and made significant contributions in this regard.

Accidentally exchanged twins are a rare subgroup of separated twins, with only nine documented cases world-wide. The complete chronology of these pairs is summarized in Table 1. Of particular interest is the most recent case involving a double exchange between two pairs of MZ male twins from Colombia, South America. This event generated a series of genetically and environmentally informative kinships, some familiar, but some unique. Familiar sibling sets are two reared-apart MZ (MZA) twin pairs raised in dramatically different environments. The unique sibling pairs include: (a) two virtual twin pairs (same-age unrelated siblings) who believed they were DZ twins and were raised as such; and (b) two pairs of ‘replicas’ (unrelated siblings who were not reared together, but who genetically replicated the unrelated reared-together pairs); comparing replicas with virtual twins yields a measure of shared environmental influence. Gottesman was fascinated by this situation and was instrumental in making some Spanish protocols available as I (NS) prepared to visit Colombia to study these twins. A book detailing the life histories, physical development, and behavioral traits of these twins, Accidental Brothers, was dedicated to Irv (Segal & Montoya, Reference Segal and Montoya2018).

TABLE 1 Chronology of Switched-at-Birth Twin Pairs

Sourced from Segal & Montoya (Reference Segal and Montoya2018).

a Double exchange: Switching of an identical twin from one pair with a fraternal twin from another pair.

b Double exchange: Switching of an identical twin from one pair with an identical twin from another pair.

Twins Reared Apart

In his 1980 introduction to Juel-Nielsen's Danish reared-apart twin study, Gottesman wrote, ‘Amidst the furor created (Hearnshaw, Reference Hearnshaw1979) by [Sir Cyril Burt's] fabrication of data, co-workers, and pairs of twins three genuinely classic studies of such rare pairs comprising much more than mere disembodied test scores for IQ have been ignored’ (p. 7). His recognition of the investigatory power of reared-apart twins is further evident in his subsequent remarks, also in the introduction: ‘Identical twins brought up apart are scarce, indeed, and their potential for stimulating research ideas about the origins of normal and abnormal characteristics is great’ (p. 9). He shared these views with his close colleague James Shields, who conducted the second reared-apart twin study in Great Britain in 1962. ‘I doubt if MZAs will ever be more numerous and representative enough to provide the main evidence about environment, or about genetics, but they furnish critical examples of persons of identical genotype reared in different homes. They can give unique real-life illustrations of some of the many possible pathways from genes to human behavior — and so will always be of human and scientific interest’ (Shields, Reference Shields1978, p. 92).

Gottesman was a faculty member at the University of Minnesota in 1979 when Dr. Thomas J. Bouchard, Jr. invited newly reunited MZA twins, Jim Lewis and Jim Springer, to his laboratory for several days of intensive psychological and medical assessment. The idea of a reared-apart twin study had been in the air prior to the discovery of the ‘Jim twins’, but Bouchard was not optimistic about identifying a sufficiently large sample of separated sets. However, the media attention surrounding the Jim twins attracted other participants, and when the study concluded in 1999, a total of 137 pairs (81 MZA and 56 DZA) had been assessed (Segal, Reference Segal2012).

Gottesman was closely involved with this study despite his absence from Minnesota during the years 1980–2001. He co-authored papers on personality (Markon et al., Reference Markon, Krueger, Bouchard and Gottesman2002), intelligence (Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Bouchard, Krueger, McGue and Gottesman2004), and twin methodology (Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Turkheimer, Gottesman and Bouchard2009), but his signature contributions are two papers on the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) scales (DiLalla et al., Reference DiLalla, Carey, Gottesman and Bouchard1996; Reference DiLalla, Gottesman, Carey and Bouchard1999). The senior author on both MMPI papers was David DiLalla, Gottesman's former doctoral student and a contributor to this symposium; co-author Gregory Carey was also one of Gottesman's former students.

Like Gottesman, I (NS) was closely involved with the Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart (MISTRA) as a postdoctoral fellow (1982–1985) and research associate (1985–1991), as well as in the years that followed. In 2006, I launched the first prospective study of reared-apart twins adopted from China and completed several case reports of twins raised in different families.

The Fullerton Study of Chinese Twins Reared Apart came about in response to a Canadian mother who had adopted one MZ female twin from China and wrote to me in 2001 for information about raising a ‘singleton twin’. In fact, China's one-child policy, enacted in 1979, led to the abandonment of hundreds of thousands of female infants; that policy and the cultural preference for male children are indirectly responsible for separating twins, mostly female (Segal, Reference Segal2005; Segal et al., Reference Segal, Stohs and Evans2011). I have assessed 23 separated sets (21 from China and 2 from Vietnam) and completed follow-up visits for approximately half the sample. The twins’ mean age at their first assessment was 9.60 years (SD = 6.48), and ranged between 3.19 and 24.98 years. Twins completed a general intelligence test, and their parents completed forms concerning their child's behavioral problems, medical life histories, family background, and other areas. Companion studies of adopted Chinese twins raised together (CTTs) and virtual twins (VTs; same-age unrelated children of all backgrounds raised together from birth) are also in progress.

Two papers from this ongoing research program concern twin relationships and developmental risks. A qualitative analysis of the Chinese twins’ responses to their first meeting with, and first departure from, their co-twin found that twins older than 18 months of age displayed strong attraction toward one another, whereas younger twins showed more neutral reactions. Older twins also showed more intense reactions and greater upset than younger twins at the time of departure (Segal & Marelich, Reference Segal and Marelich2011). This exploratory story included only the first 10 pairs; a more in-depth analysis of the twins' social relationships is planned for the future. A second study combined CTTs and VTs in a study of developmental risks. Surprisingly, age at adoption added less than other predictors to adoptees’ externalizing and internalizing behaviors. Family factors, such as parental education, contributed significantly to the behavioral outcomes of the CTTs. Comparing the resemblance of CTTs and VTs showed that genetic influence affected initial adaptation to adoption, refusal/avoidance, cying/clinging, and developmental delays (r is = 0.71–0.91), with shared environmental effects evident for the latter two measures (Segal, Tan et al., Reference Segal, Tan and Graham2015).

I (NS) have completed four case reports of reared-apart twins, three MZAf pairs born in South Korea (Segal & Cortez, Reference Segal and Cortez2014; Segal & Hur, Reference Segal and Hur2008; Segal, Hur et al., Reference Segal, Hur and Graham2015) and one DZAf pair born in England (Segal, Cortez et al., Reference Segal, Cortez, Zettel-Watson, Cherry, Mechanic, Munson and Reed2015). Co-twins in all three MZAf pairs were raised in different countries (two in the United States and South Korea, and one in the United States and France). Despite the extremely different environments in which some of these co-twins were raised, some striking similarities in their behavioral and medical characteristics were observed. At the same time, differences in their mental abilities and personality traits were expressed that were possibly tied to events in their respective life histories. The co-twins in the DZAf pair both remained in England until one of the twins moved to the United States in her twenties. These twins, who were reunited at 78 years of age, have been entered into the Guinness World Records as the world's longest separated set (GWR, 2016). As expected, given their 50% average genetic relatedness, these twins were less alike behaviorally than the member of the three MZAf pairs.

In 2012, I authored Born Together-Reared Apart: The Landmark Minnesota Twin Study, a book that surveyed the history and findings of the MISTRA (Segal, Reference Segal2012). Gottesman was one of two prepublication reviewers, and on one Saturday morning, he spent several hours on the telephone with me commenting on various sections. I am confident that the book was significantly improved because of his perceptive eye.

Twins Switched at Birth

Twins switched at birth are identified very rarely, reflected in the fact that only nine cases have been documented worldwide. However, I strongly suspect that some twins are growing up either as singletons in the wrong family, or as VTs believing that are DZ. In fact, two independent medical companies estimated that between 20,000–23,000 misplacements of newborns occur each year in United States hospitals, but are quickly corrected (DNA Diagnostics Center, 2010; Rusting, Reference Rusting2001). However, given the very large number of potential errors, it seems unlikely that all such mistakes are rectified.

MZ twins would have an easier time learning if they had been exchanged because of their identical appearance — in fact, instances of mistaken identity were responsible for revealing the truth in all nine cases. In 2011, I published a book covering the life events and behavioral similarities and differences of a switched-at-birth MZAf pair from Gran Canaria, Spain (Segal, Reference Segal2011). These twins learned about the switch at age 28 when a family friend was certain that a clothing store customer was a woman she had known for some time.

Two of the nine cases involved a double exchange, that is, the switching of a newborn twin in one pair with a newborn twin in another pair. The first case, which occurred in Puerto Rico, involved an MZAf pair and an allegedly DZAf pair. In this case, the correct children were returned to their biological families when they were 18 months old. This was traumatic for the mother and father who were interviewed, given the strong parent–child attachments that had developed. However, a more striking case from Colombia, South America came to light in 2014 when it was discovered that a newborn MZAm twin in one pair had been exchanged with a newborn MZAm twin in another pair. Again, the truth was revealed because of mistaken identity when the twins turned 25. The following section expands upon selected aspects of these twins’ physical and behavioral development. All four twins are shown in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1 The four Colombian twins at a family gathering in the country, March 2015. The members of one MZA twin pair are the first and second from the left; the members of the other MZA twin pair are third and fourth from the left. Photo credit: Dr. Nancy L. Segal.

Zygosity

The monozygosity of both twin pairs was confirmed by concordance across 21short tandem repeat markers (STRs). However, it was of interest to apply Lykken's (Reference Lykken1978) zygosity assessment formula, based upon fingerprint ridge count and body size; using these numbers, the probability of misclassification is less than 0.1%. Kevin Haroian, who worked closely with Lykken, performed these calculations on data gathered from the twins in 2015 and 2016; the 2015 measures included height, weight, ponderal index and total fingerprint ridge count, while the 2016 measures additionally included the cephalic index and updated weight. The zygosity probabilities for the reared-apart twins and reared-together unrelated brothers are summarized in Tables 2 and 3.

TABLE 2 Colombian Twins’ Body Size Measures

cm = centimeters, kg = kilograms, lb = pounds, Head L = head length, Head W = head width, BMI = body mass index, 1 = co-twins, 2 = co-twins.

TABLE 3 Probability of Colombian Brothers’ MZ Twinship Based on Fingerprint Ridge Count and Ponderal and Cephalic Indices in 2015 and 2016

A-D = absolute difference, TRC = total fingerprint ridge count, PI = ponderal index, CI = cephalic index, MZA = MZ reared-apart pair, VT = virtual twins (unrelated pair raised together).

a CI recorded in 2016; PI based on 2015 weight.

b CI recorded in 2016; PI based on 2016 weight.

Several findings are worth noting: First, Table 2 shows that one twin (Country-1) gained over 20 pounds (nearly 10 kg) since the first visit (approximately 15 months earlier), increasing his BMI from 23.04 to 26.49 and widening the difference between himself and his co-twin. (His weight gain was due to intense working out at a gym and a dietary regimen of high protein drinks.) Most importantly, Table 3 shows that increasing the number of genetically informative traits in Lykken's formula generally increases the probability (and presumed accuracy) of twin-type classification. However, it also shows that the one twin's weight gain caused a substantial drop in the probability that he and his co-twin are MZ twins (87 to 47%). Thus, the timing of such assessments can be critical — it is likely that the twins would have been diagnosed as DZ by the rules specified in Nichols and Bibro's (Reference Nichols and Bilbro1966) physical resemblance questionnaire, a weight difference of 15 pounds or more is one index of dizygosity at the second level of certainty. Given the foregoing, the best method for zygosity diagnosis remains DNA analysis.

Cognitive Development

The twins completed both the WAIS-IV IQ test and the Raven Advanced Progressive Matrices (APM), Set-II (Raven, Reference Raven1965). In the interest of confidentiality, general intelligence test results were summarized in general terms by Segal and Montoya (Reference Segal and Montoya2018). In brief, the city-raised twins outscored their country-raised co-twins, (who had not advanced beyond the fifth grade), with both similarities and differences noted in the profiles of the reared-apart twins and unrelated pairs. One set of replicas performed quite differently, while the other set showed unexpected resemblance in their profile pattern, although not in their level. These findings are of interest given the different life histories of the various pair members, but this single case does not challenge the 50–75% genetic influence on general intelligence, based upon years of twin and family research.

The Raven APM, Set II is a non-verbal multiple-choice test, devised in 1947 for officer selection. It assesses the ability to draw comparisons between figures and to develop a logical method of reasoning. The 36 items can be completed either with a time limit (capacity for observation and clear thinking) or without a time limit (efficiency of intellectual work). The twins completed only Set-II as a power test, without a time constraint, in order to reveal differences in what they knew and could do. Efforts were made to note the item on which they were working at 42 min, given that approximately 40 min yields a satisfactory score distribution in group administrations. Instructions were reviewed carefully prior to testing.

The better educated city-raised twins outscored their country-raised co-twins. The co-twins in one MZA pair were at items 34 and 24, respectively, after 42 min had elapsed, whereas the co-twins in the other pair completed the test in less than 42 min. Members of the unrelated pairs (VTs) scored closer to one another than to their reared-apart co-twins. Interestingly, the degree of difference between the two MZA pairs and the two VT pairs was about the same. The biggest difference among the six pairings was between one of the replicated pairs, followed by the two MZA twin pairs. These findings are shown graphically in Figure 2.

FIGURE 2 Colombian twins’ performance on the Raven APM, Set II. Illustration credit: Kelly Donovan, BA.

These results suggest that the twins’ different educational backgrounds played a role in their performance, with the lack of education overwhelming the genetic potentials of the country-raised twins, and despite the general recognition of the Raven APM as a valid, non-verbal measure of mostly fluid intelligence (Kvist & Gustafsson, Reference Kvist and Gustafsson2008). General intelligence test scores for some twins in the MISTRA varied as a function of educational experience, but the effects were not systematic (Segal, Reference Segal2012). In addition, Newman et al. (Reference Newman, Freeman and Holzinger1937) found that educationally advantaged twins generally outperformed their less advantaged co-twins, but the intraclass correlations for both mental ability tests in that study remained high. The lower scores of the country-raised Colombian twins may partly reflect their lack of education, although as yet unknown factors (e.g., unfamiliarity with the test situation) may have played a role; the higher scoring country-raised twin had, in fact, obtained a GED in his early twenties. Interestingly, both large Raven and WAIS-IV discrepancies were noted for one reared-apart pair, whereas only a large Raven discrepancy was noted for the other pair.

Twin-Family Study of Social Closeness

On a different twin-related track, Gottesman applied the twin-family design to assess the risk of schizophrenia to the children of discordant MZ and DZ twin pairs (Gottesman & Bertelsen, Reference Gottesman and Bertelsen1989). As I noted in the introduction to this special issue (and as Christensen and Bertelesen describe in their article), a key finding was that the offspring of discordant MZ twins were at equal risk for developing the disorder, whereas the offspring born only to affected DZ co-twins were at risk. The twin-family study (variously known as the MZ Half-Sibling Design, Nuclear Family Twin Design, Children-of-Twins Approach, and Twin-Pedigree Study) has been used to assess factors affecting a range of physical and behavioral traits, such as cognition (Van Leeuwen et al., Reference Van Leeuwen, Van Den Berg and Boomsma2008), age at menarche (Mendle et al., Reference Mendle, Turkheimer, D'Onofrio, Lynch, Emery, Slutske and Martin2006), externalizing disorders (Hicks et al., Reference Hicks, Krueger, Iacono, McGue and Patrick2004), and self-esteem (Bleidorn et al., Reference Bleidorn, Hufer, Kandler, Hopwood and Riemann2018). My laboratory continues to use this model to compare the social closeness of MZ and DZ twin aunts and uncles expressed toward their nieces and nephews (Segal et al., Reference Segal, Seghers, Marelich, Mechanic and Castillo2007; Segal & Marelich, Reference Segal and Marelich2011). Updated findings and future directions for this project have also been presented (Segal et al., Reference Segal, Mollova, Marelich and Preston2016).

Participants from both intact and non-intact twin pairs (n = 470 individuals: 44 MZm, 318 MZf, 15 DZm, 93 DZf; 6 male and 28 female twins were from opposite-sex twin pairs) completed a Social Closeness Survey, mostly online, although a minority of participants from the annual Twins Days Festival in Twinsburg, Ohio completed a pencil and paper version. Two major hypotheses, guided by evolutionary reasoning, were assessed: (1) MZ twin aunts/uncles will indicate greater social closeness toward their nieces and nephews than DZ twin aunts/uncles, and (2) twins with female co-twins will express greater social closeness toward their nieces and nephews than twins with male co-twins. Hypothesis 1 is derived from Hamilton's (Reference Hamilton1964) assertion that natural selection favors alleles predisposing individuals to act in ways that favor transmission of those alleles. Alleles predisposing individuals to behave altruistically toward others likely to carry copies of those alleles would be an indirect way of transmitting one's genes to future generations, thereby enhancing one's reproductive success (inclusive fitness). Hypothesis 2 drew upon the concept of paternity uncertainty, namely that because of hidden ovulation, internal fertilization and continuous female sexual receptivity, males cannot be certain that children delivered by their partner are truly theirs. Thus, greater social closeness should be expressed by uncles and aunts whose nieces and nephews were born to twin sisters, rather than to twin brothers.

Both hypotheses were supported by the data. The main effect for zygosity was significant, F(1, 466) = 10.85, p = .001, with lower scores reflecting increased social closeness. Closeness toward nieces and nephews was greater for MZ twins (M = 22.26, SD = 6.00) than for DZ twins (M = 25.81, SD = 6.02).In addition, the main effect for co-twin sex was significant, F(1, 466) = 17.26, p < .001. As expected, closeness was greater for twins with female co-twins (M = 22.37, SD = 5.09) than for twins with male co-twins (M = 26.45, SD = 5.47). Not surprisingly, social closeness correlated significantly with perceived similarity: r = 0.58, p < .001. This study is ongoing and can be completed online at https://fullerton.qualtrics.com/jfe/form/SV_1XJDEwEaPIYEiyN.

Acknowledgment

Irving I. Gottesman may be gone, but he is not forgotten. His memory remains in the minds of everyone who knew him, and his work will be cited often in the years to come.

I wish to thank the members of this panel for their participation and their generosity. I also wish to thank Irv's University of Minnesota colleague, Dr. Bruce Overmeier, for launching an ‘In Honor Of’ page for Irv, sponsored by the Federation of Associations in Behavioral and Brain Sciences (FABBS). Colleagues, friends, and people who may have known Irv just by his work are asked to consider making a donation of any size — all donations are welcome. Please visit http://fabbs.org/our_scientists/irving-i-gottesman-phd/

References

Bleidorn, W., Hufer, A., Kandler, C., Hopwood, C. J., & Riemann, R. (2018). A nuclear twin family study of self-esteem. European Journal of Personality. Advance online publication.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DiLalla, D. L., Carey, G., Gottesman, I. I., & Bouchard, T. J. (1996). Heritability of MMPI personality indicators of psychopathology in twins reared apart. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 491499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DiLalla, D. L., Gottesman, I. I., Carey, G., & Bouchard, T. J. Jr. (1999). Heritability of MMPI Harris-Lingoes and subtle-obvious subscales in twins reared apart. Assessment, 6, 353366.Google Scholar
DNA Diagnostics Center. (2010, November 1). Identification techniques for preventing infant mix-ups. Retrieved from http://www.dnacenter.com/science-technology/articles/infant-mix-up.Google Scholar
Gottesman, I. I., & Bertelsen, A. (1989). Confirming unexpressed genotypes for schizophrenia: Risks in the offspring of Fischer's Danish identical and fraternal discordant twins. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 867872.Google Scholar
Guinness World Records (GWR). (2016). Humans: Twins. (pp. 6263). London, UK: Author.Google Scholar
Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behavior. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hearnshaw, L. S. (1979). Cyril Burt, Psychologist. New York, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Hicks, B. M., Krueger, R. F., Iacono, W. G., McGue, M., & Patrick, C. J. (2004). Family transmission and heritability of externalizing disorders: A twin-family study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61, 922928.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, W., Bouchard, T. J., Krueger, R. F., McGue, M., & Gottesman, I. I. (2004). Just one g: Consistent results from three test batteries. Intelligence, 32, 95107.Google Scholar
Johnson, W., Turkheimer, E., Gottesman, I. I., & Bouchard, T. J. (2009). Beyond heritability: Twin studies in behavioral research. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 217220.Google Scholar
Juel-Nielsen, N. (1965). Individual and environment: Monozygotic twins reared apart. New York, NY: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Juel-Nielsen, N. (1980). Individual and environment: Monozygotic twins reared apart (revised edition). New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Thornton, L. M., & Pedersen, N. L. (2000). Tobacco consumption in Swedish twins reared apart and reared together. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 886892.Google Scholar
Kvist, A. V., & Gustafsson, J. E. (2008). The relation between fluid intelligence and the general factor as a function of cultural background: A test of Cattell's investment theory. Intelligence, 36, 422436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lykken, D. T. (1978). The diagnosis of zygosity in twins. Behavior Genetics, 8, 437473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markon, K. E., Krueger, R. F., Bouchard, T. J. Jr., & Gottesman, I. I. (2002). Normal and abnormal personality traits: Evidence for genetic and environmental relationships in the Minnesota study of twins reared apart. Journal of Personality, 70, 661693.Google Scholar
Mendle, J., Turkheimer, E., D'Onofrio, B. M., Lynch, S. K., Emery, R. E., Slutske, W. S., & Martin, N. G. (2006). Family structure and age at menarche: A children-of-twins approach. Developmental Psychology, 42, 533542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newman, H. N., Freeman, F. N., & Holzinger, K. J. (1937). Twins: A study of heredity and environment. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Nichols, R.C., & Bilbro, W. C. Jr. (1966). The diagnosis of twin zygosity. Acta Geneticae et Statistica Medicae, 16, 265275.Google ScholarPubMed
Raven, J. C. (1965). Advanced progressive matrices, Set-II. Eynsham, Oxford, UK: Oxford Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Rusting, R. R. (2001). Baby switching: An under-reported problem that needs to be recognized. Journal of Healthcare Protection Management, 17, 89100.Google Scholar
Segal, N. L. (2005). Indivisible by two: Lives of extraordinary twins. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Segal, N. L. (2011). Someone else's twin: The true story of babies switched at birth. Amherst, NY: Prometheus.Google Scholar
Segal, N. L. (2012). Born together — Reared apart: The landmark Minnesota twin study. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Segal, N. L., & Cortez, F. A. (2014). Born in Korea — adopted apart: Behavioral development of monozygotic twins raised in the United States and France. Personality and Individual Differences, 70, 97104.Google Scholar
Segal, N. L., Cortez, F. A., Zettel-Watson, L., Cherry, B. J., Mechanic, M., Munson, J. E., . . . Reed, B. (2015). Genetic and experiential influences on behavior: Twins reunited at seventy-eight years. Personality & Individual Differences, 73, 110117.Google Scholar
Segal, N. L., & Hur, Y.-M. (2008). Reared apart Korean female twins: Genetic and cultural influences on life histories, physical and health-related measures, and behavioral traits. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 32, 542548.Google Scholar
Segal, N. L., Hur, Y.-M., & Graham, J. L. (2015). Korean twins reared apart: Genetic and cultural influences on behavior and health. Adoption Quarterly, 18, 291310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Segal, N. L., & Marelich, W. D. (2011). Social closeness and gift giving by MZ and DZ twin parents toward nieces and nephews: An update. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 101105.Google Scholar
Segal, N. L., Mollova, S., Marelich, W. D., & Preston, K. (2016, June–July). Does genetic relatedness affect social closeness toward nieces and nephews? A twin-family study. Paper presented at the Human Behavior and Evolution Society Conference, Vancouver, Canada.Google Scholar
Segal, N. L., & Montoya, Y. S. (2018). Accidental brothers: The story of twins exchanged at birth and the power of nature and nurture. New York, NY: St. Martin's Press.Google Scholar
Segal, N. L., Seghers, J. P., Marelich, W. D., Mechanic, M., & Castillo, R.(2007). Social closeness of monozygotic and dizygotic twin parents toward their nieces and nephews. European Journal of Personality, 21, 487506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Segal, N. L., Stohs, J. H., & Evans, K. (2011). Chinese twin children reared apart and reunited: First prospective study of co-twin reunions. Adoption Quarterly, 14, 6178.Google Scholar
Segal, N. L., Tan, T. X., & Graham, J. L. (2015). Twins and virtual twins: Do genetic (as well as experiential) factors affect developmental risks? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 136, 5569.Google Scholar
Shields, J. (1962). Monozygotic twins: Brought up apart and together. London, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shields, J. (1978). MZA twins: Their use and abuse. From Newman to Burt — A decline? Progress in Clinical and Biological Research, 24, 7993.Google Scholar
Van Leeuwen, M., Van Den Berg, S. M., & Boomsma, D. I. (2008). A twin-family study of general IQ. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 7688.Google Scholar
Zhou, B., Gao, W., Lv, J., Yu, C., Wang, S., Liao, C., . . . Wang, H. (2015). Genetic and environmental influences on obesity-related phenotypes in Chinese twins reared apart and together. Behavior Genetics, 45, 427437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

TABLE 1 Chronology of Switched-at-Birth Twin Pairs

Figure 1

FIGURE 1 The four Colombian twins at a family gathering in the country, March 2015. The members of one MZA twin pair are the first and second from the left; the members of the other MZA twin pair are third and fourth from the left. Photo credit: Dr. Nancy L. Segal.

Figure 2

TABLE 2 Colombian Twins’ Body Size Measures

Figure 3

TABLE 3 Probability of Colombian Brothers’ MZ Twinship Based on Fingerprint Ridge Count and Ponderal and Cephalic Indices in 2015 and 2016

Figure 4

FIGURE 2 Colombian twins’ performance on the Raven APM, Set II. Illustration credit: Kelly Donovan, BA.