Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-11T03:28:42.572Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Behavioral estimates of absolute threshold in rat

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 June 2009

Carmen Muñoz Tedó
Affiliation:
Departamento de Psicobiologia, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid 28023, Spain
Pilar Herreros De Tejada
Affiliation:
Departamento de Psicobiologia, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid 28023, Spain
Daniel G. Green
Affiliation:
Department of Ophthalmology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor

Abstract

Dark-adapted thresholds of albino and pigmented rats were estimated using behavioral methods. Albino and pigmented rats who had been water deprived learned to bar press for water reinforcement when a light stimulus was presented. Absolute threshold was defined to be the light intensity at which bar pressing behavior was significantly modified by the presence of the light stimulus. Albino rats had an average threshold of −5.23 log cd/m2 and the pigmented rats had a threshold of −5.0 log cd/m2. These values are close to −5.3 log cd/m2, the psychophysical threshold of human observers in the same apparatus. Consistent with our earlier electrophysiology, these behavioral experiments provide no evidence for an albino/pigmented sensitivity difference. Comparisons are made between behavioral and electrophysiological determinations of absolute threshold in albino and pigmented rats. Thresholds determined behaviorally agree remarkably well with those derived from visual evoked potentials.

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1994

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Balkema, G. (1988). Elevated dark-adapted thresholds in albino rodents. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science 29, 544.Google ScholarPubMed
Balkema, G. & Drager, U. (1991). Impaired visual thresholds in hypo-pigmented animals. Visual Neuroscience 6, 577585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balkema, G.W., Pinto, L.H., Drager, U.C. & Vanable, J.W. (1981). Characterization of abnormalities in the visual system of the mutant mouse pearl. Journal of Neuroscience 1, 1320.Google Scholar
Berkley, M.A. & Watkins, D.W. (1973). Grating resolution and refraction in the cat estimated from evoked cerebral potentials. Vision Research 13, 403415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blough, D.S. & Yager, D. (1972). pp. 732763. Visual psychophysics in animals. In Handbook of Sensory Physiology, Vol. VII/4: Visual Psychophysics, ed. Jameson, D. & Hurvich, L.M.Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Birch, D. & Jacobs, G.H. (1975). Behavioral measurements of rat spectral sensitivity. Vision Research 15, 687691.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, F.W. & Kulikowski, J.J. (1972). The visual evoked potential as a function of contrast sensitivity of a grating pattern. Journal of Physiology 222, 345356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, F.W., Maffei, L. & Piccolfno, M. (1973). The contrast sensitivity of the cat. Journal of Physiology 229, 719731.Google Scholar
Dodt, E. & Echte, K. (1961). Dark and light adaptation in pigmented and white rat as measured by electroretinogram threshold. Journal of Neurophysiology 24, 427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, D.G. (1971). Light adaptation in the rat retina: Evidence for two receptor mechanisms. Science 174, 598600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, D.G., Herreros De Tejada, P. & Glover, M. (1991). Are albino rats night blind? Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science 32, 23662371.Google Scholar
Green, D.G., Herreros De Tejada, P. & Glover, M.J. (1994). Electrophysiological estimates of visual sensitivity in albino and pigmented mice. Visual Neuroscience 11, 919925.Google Scholar
Hayes, J.M. & Balkema, G.W. (1993 a). Elevated dark adapted thresholds in hypopigmented mice measured with a water maze screening apparatus. Behavior Genetics 23, 395403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, J.M. & Balkema, G.W. (1993 b). Visual thresholds in mice: Comparison of retinal damage and hypopigmentation, Visual Neuroscience 10, 931938.Google Scholar
Herreros De Tejada, P., Green, D.G. & Munoz, Tedo C. (1992). Visual thresholds in albino and pigmented rats. Visual Neuroscience 9, 409414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurylo, D.D., Hansen, R.M. & Fulton, A.B. (1991). Dark adapted threshold in young RCS rats. Vision Research 31, 637642.Google Scholar
Muntz, W.R.A. (1967). A behavioral study on photopic and scotopic vision in the hooded rat. Vision Research 7, 371376.Google Scholar
Ratto, G.M., Robinson, D.W., Yan, B. & McNaughton, P.A. (1991). Development of the light response in neonatal mammalian rods. Nature 351, 654657.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sieoel, S. (1956). Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Silveira, L.C., Heywood, C.A. & Cowey, A. (1987). Contrast sensitivity and visual acuity of the pigmented rat determined electrophysiologically. Vision Research 27, 17191731.Google Scholar
Silver, P.H. (1967). Spectral sensitivity of the white rat by a training method. Vision Research 7, 377383.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trejo, L.J. & Cicerone, C.M. (1987). Changes in visual sensitivity with age in rats with hereditary retinal degeneration. Vision Research 27, 915918.Google Scholar