Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T13:37:09.319Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Seeing gray through the ON and OFF pathways

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 June 2009

E. J. Chichilnisky
Affiliation:
Department of Neurobiology, Stanford University, Stanford
B. A. Wandell
Affiliation:
Neuroscience Program and Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford

Abstract

Color appearance judgments revealed fundamental differences in visual processing of incremental and decremental lights. First, the balance of cone activation required for a light to appear achromatic was different for increments and decrements (Judd, 1940; Helson & Michels, 1948). Second, adaptation-the visual system's adjustment to background light – affected achromatic decrements more than increments. Third, the regulation of adaptation for incremental and decremental stimuli depended differently on background signals from the three cone types. We interpret these asymmetries as differences in mechanisms of adaptation in the ON and OFF pathways, and suggest that they evolved to accommodate the range and physical sources of color signals in the two pathways.

Type
Short Communication
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1996

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ahn, S.J. & Macleod, D.I.A. (1993). Link-specific adaptation in the luminance and chromatic channels. Vision Research 33, 22712286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowen, R.W., Pokorny, J. & Smith, V.C. (1989). Sawtooth contrast sensitivity: Decrements have the edge. Vision Research 29, 15011509.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boynton, R.M. & Whitten, D.N. (1970). Visual adaptation in monkey cones: Recordings of late receptor potentials. Science 170, 14231426.Google Scholar
Brainard, D.H. (1989). Calibration of a computer controlled color monitor. Color Research and Application 14, 2334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brainard, D.H. & Wandell, B.A. (1992). Asymmetric color-matching: How color appearance depends on the illuminant. Journal of I he Optical Society of America A 9, 14331448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burnham, R.W., Evans, R.M. & Newhall, S.M. (1952). Influence on color perception of adaptation to illumination. Journal of the Optical Society of America 42, 597605.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chichilnisky, E.J. & Wandell, B.A. (1995). Photoreceptor sensitivity changes explain color appearance shifts induced by large uniform backgrounds in dichoptic matching. Vision Research 35, 239254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Du Buf, J.M.H. (1992). Brightness versus apparent contrast: I. Incremental and decremental disks with varying diameter. Spatial Vision 6, 159182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Efron, B. (1982). The Jackknife, the Bootstrap and Other Resampling Plans. Philadelphia, Pennsylvania: Society for Industrial and Applied Mathematics.Google Scholar
Helson, H. & Michels, W.C. (1948). The effect of chromatic adaptation on achromaticity. Journal of the Optical Society of America 38, 10251032.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunt, R.W.G. (1995). The Reproduction of Color. Tolworth, England: Fountain Press.Google Scholar
Hurvich, L.M. & Jameson, D. (1958). Further development of a quantified opponent-color theory. In Visual Problems of Colour II, pp. 691723. London: HMSO.Google Scholar
Ishihara, S.(1977). Tests for Colour-Blindness. Tokyo: Kanehara Shuppen Company Ltd.Google Scholar
Judd, D.B. (1940). Hue saturation and lightness of surface colors with chromatic illumination. Journal of the Optical Society of America 30, 2132.Google Scholar
Krauskopf, J.(1980). Discrimination and detection of changes in luminance. Vision Research 20, 671677.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mausfeld, R. & Niederee, R.(1993). An inquiry into relational concepts of colour, based on incremental principles of colour coding for minimal relational stimuli. Perception 22, 427462.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mollon, J.D. & Polden, P.G. (1977). An anomaly in the response of the eye to light to short wavelengths. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B (London) 278, 207240.Google ScholarPubMed
Pugh, E.N. (1976). The nature of the π1 colour mechanism of W.S. Stiles. Journal of Physiology (London) 257, 713747.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pugh, E.N. & Larimer, J. (1980). Test of the identity of the site of blue/yellow hue cancellation and the site of chromatic antagonism in the π1, pathway. Vision Research 20, 779788.Google Scholar
Pugh, E.N. & Mollon, J.D. (1979). A theory of the π1, and π3 color mechanisms of Stiles. Vision Research 19, 293312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schiller, P.H., Sandell, J.H. & Maunsell, J.H.R. (1986). Functions of the ON and OFF channels of the visual system. Nature 322, 824825.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schnapf, J.L., Nunn, B.J., Meister, M. & Baylor, D.A. (1990). Visual transduction in cones of the monkey Macaca fascicularis. Journal of Physiology (London) 427, 681713.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shevell, S.K. (1978). The dual role of chromatic backgrounds in color perception. Vision Research 18, 16491661.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, V. & Pokorny, J. (1975). Spectral sensitivity of the foveal cone photopigments between 400 and 500 nm. Vision Research 15, 161171.Google Scholar
Sternheim, C.E., Stromeyer, C.F. III & Khoo, M.C.K. (1979). Visibility of chromatic flicker upon spectrally mixed adapting fields. Vision Research 19, 175183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stiles, W.S. (1959). Color vision: The approach through increment threshold sensitivity. Proceedings National Academy of Sciences of the U.S.A. 45, 100114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stromeyer, C.F. III & Sternheim, C.E. (1981). Visibility of red and green spatial patterns upon spectrally mixed adapting fields. Vision Research 21, 397408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Von Kries, J. (1905). Influence of adaptation on the effects produced by luminous stimuli. In Sources of Color Science, ed. Macadam, D.L., pp. 120127. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Walraven, J.(1976). Discounting the background – the missing link in the explanation of chromatic induction. Vision Research 16, 289295.Google Scholar
Walraven, J.(1977). Colour signals from incremental and decremen-tal light stimuli. Vision Research 17, 7176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walters, H.V. (1942). Some experiments on the trichromatic theory of vision. Proceedings of the Royal Society B (London) 131, 2750.Google Scholar
Wandell, B.A. (1995). Foundations of Vision. Sunderland, Massachusetts: Sinauer.Google Scholar
Wandell, B.A. & Pugh, E.N. (1980 a). Detection of long-duration, long-wavelength incremental flashes by a chromatically coded pathway. Vision Research 20, 625636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wandell, B.A. & Pugh, E.N. (1980 b). A field-additive pathway detects brief-duration, long-wavelength incremental flashes. Vision Research 20, 613624.Google Scholar
Wassef, E.G.T. (1958). Investigation into the theory of prediction of the appearance of colors and its bearing on the theory of color vision. Optica Acta 5, 101108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werner, J.S. & Walraven, J.(1982). Effect of chromatic adaptation on the achromatic locus: The role of contrast, luminance and background color. Vision Research 22, 929944.Google Scholar
White, T.W., Irvin, G.E. & Williams, M.C. (1980). Asymmetry in the brightness and darkness Broca-Sulzer effects. Vision Research 20, 723726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whittle, P.(1986). Increments and decrements: Luminance discrimination. Vision Research 26, 16771692.Google Scholar
Wright, W.D. (1947). Researches on Normal and Defective Colour Vision. St. Louis, Missouri: C.V. Mosby.Google Scholar
Wyszecki, G. & Stiles, W.S. (1982). Color Science–Concepts and Methods, Quantitative Data and Formulae. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar