Introduction
Intelligence quotient (IQ) is a quantitative estimate of an individual’s general cognitive ability [Reference Wechsler1]. Patients experiencing a first-episode psychosis (FEP) tend to have lower IQs than healthy controls [Reference Kendler, Turkheimer, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2,Reference Khandaker, Barnett, White and Jones3]. It has also been described that these IQ deficits precede the onset of psychosis, probably due to neurodevelopmental impairments [Reference Cosway, Byrne, Clafferty, Hodges, Grant and Abukmeil4,Reference Jones, Rodgers, Murray and Marmot5]. While cognitive abilities aggregate in families, FEP patients tend to perform worse on cognitive tasks than their first-degree relatives, indicating a deviation from familial cognitive aptitude [Reference Barrantes-Vidal, Aguilera, Campanera, Fatjó-Vilas, Guitart and Miret6–Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Mezuk, Sundquist and Sundquist10]. Accordingly, IQ and specific neuropsychological functions have been largely investigated as endophenotypic traits of psychosis that may enhance preventive measures and early intervention [Reference Bertisch, Mesen-Fainardi, Martin, Pérez-Vargas, Vargas-Rodríguez and Delgado11–Reference Zhang, Zhang, Qin and Tan14].
Both IQ and psychosis are highly heritable, with heritability estimates ranging from 40% to 70% [Reference Haworth, Wright, Luciano, Martin, de Geus and van Beijsterveldt15,Reference Willoughby, McGue, Iacono and Lee16] and 60% to 80% [Reference Owen, Sawa and Mortensen17,Reference Hilker, Helenius, Fagerlund, Skytthe, Christensen and Werge18], respectively. The polygenic score (PGS) method is useful for estimating an individual’s genetic make-up for such complex phenotypes [Reference Genç, Schlüter, Fraenz, Arning, Metzen and Nguyen19,Reference Martin, Daly, Robinson, Hyman and Neale20]. On the one hand, it is possible to calculate PGS-IQ based on the results of large-scale genome-wide studies that have characterised the genetic architecture of intelligence [Reference Savage, Jansen, Stringer, Watanabe, Bryois and de Leeuw21]. PGS-IQ is strongly correlated with crystallised intelligence and accounts for up to 5.1% of the variance in general cognitive ability [Reference Loughnan, Palmer, Thompson, Dale, Jernigan and Chieh Fan22]. On the other hand, polygenic risk scores for schizophrenia (PGS-SCZ) can be calculated leveraging the results of genome-wide studies on this disorder [Reference Trubetskoy, Pardiñas, Qi, Panagiotaropoulou, Awasthi and Bigdeli23,24]. PGS-SCZ explain between 2.4% and 7.3% of the variance in SCZ on the liability scale [Reference Trubetskoy, Pardiñas, Qi, Panagiotaropoulou, Awasthi and Bigdeli23,24] and is increased in FEP patients compared to controls [Reference Ferraro, Quattrone, La Barbera, La Cascia, Morgan and Jb25,Reference Harrisberger, Smieskova, Vogler, Egli, Schmidt and Lenz26]. There may be a certain degree of association between these two PGSs, given that numerous genetic variants have been identified as contributing factors to intelligence and SCZ [Reference Hill, Davies, Liewald, McIntosh and Deary27,Reference Murillo-García, Barrio-Martínez, Setién-Suero, Soler, Papiol and Fatjó-Vilas28]. Similarly, PGS discriminating SCZ from bipolar disorder was found to be specifically related to intelligence [Reference Ohi, Nishizawa, Sugiyama, Takai, Kuramitsu and Hasegawa29].
We hypothesised that i) FEP patients would have higher PGS-SCZ and lower PGS-IQ than first-degree relatives and healthy controls, and ii) PGS-SCZ would be negatively associated with IQ and the patient’s IQ deviation from the mean score of their family (named family-IQ), suggesting that genetic predisposition to SCZ is related to worse general cognitive ability. We also expected a positive association of PGS-IQ with IQ.
Our primary aim was to test whether the genetic risk for SCZ, as determined by PGS-SCZ, might be associated with IQ and contributed to patient-specific differences from their family-IQ in a sample of FEP patients, their first-degree relatives, and healthy controls. Second, we also aimed to examine to what extent PGS-IQ predicts intelligence and deviation from family-IQ.
Methods
Sample
Participants were drawn from PAFIP-FAMILIES, a family-based study carried out in Cantabria, Spain, from January 2018 to March 2021, funded by the ISCIII (FIS PI17/00221). All participants were of European ancestry. We recruited first-degree relatives of a cohort of FEP patients previously enrolled in the Cantabria Program for Early Intervention in Psychosis (PAFIP) [Reference Ayesa-Arriola, Ortíz-García de la Foz, Martínez-García, Setién-Suero, Ramírez and Suárez-Pinilla30,Reference Rodríguez-Sánchez, Setién-Suero, Suárez-Pinilla, Mayoral Van Son, Vázquez-Bourgon and Gil López31]. The local institutional review committee (CEIm Cantabria) approved both projects (PAFIP and PAFIP-FAMILIES) under international research ethics standards and all participants gave their written informed consent. The initial sample consisted of 133 FEP patients, 146 parents, 98 siblings, and 202 controls [Reference Murillo-García, Díaz-Pons, Fernández-Cacho, Miguel-Corredera, Martínez-Barrio and Ortiz-García de la Foz32].
FEP patients
The PAFIP program was carried out at the University Hospital Marqués de Valdecilla (Santander, Spain) from 2001 to 2018. FEP patients were referred from the inpatient unit, outreach mental health services, and healthcare centres in the region. Inclusion criteria were as follows: 1) 15–60 years of age; 2) living within the recruitment area; 3) experiencing an FEP; 4) no prior treatment with antipsychotic medication or if previously treated, a total lifetime of antipsychotic treatment of <6 weeks; and 5) criteria for brief psychotic disorder, schizophreniform disorder, SCZ, or not otherwise specified psychosis according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV). The exclusion criteria included meeting the DSM-IV criteria for drug or alcohol dependence, having an intellectual disability, and having a history of neurological disease or head injury.
First-degree relatives
We contacted the parents and siblings of the eligible patients (those with neuropsychological data and DNA samples) and invited them to participate in the study. Inclusion criteria were as follows: 1) age over 15 years, 2) good domain of the Spanish language, and 3) ability to give informed consent in writing. Exclusion criteria included a history of psychiatric diagnosis related to psychotic illness spectrum, organic brain pathology, and intellectual disability or substance use disorders according to DSM-V criteria.
Controls
Controls were retrieved from the PAFIP program, which recruited healthy individuals through advertisements from the local community. They met the same inclusion and exclusion criteria as first-degree relatives. The psychiatric history of controls and relatives was screened by the abbreviated version of the Comprehensive Assessment of Symptoms and History [Reference Andreasen33], a semi-structured psychiatric interview that inquires about the presence of clinical symptoms for mania, depression, and positive, disorganised, and negative dimensions of psychosis.
Phenotypic data
Sociodemographic data
We recorded the sex, age, and completed years of formal education of all participants. Cannabis consumption was recorded for FEP patients, siblings, and controls.
Clinical data
We obtained clinical data from patients at baseline through medical records and interviews. The age at psychosis onset was defined as the age when the emergence of the first continuous psychotic symptom occurred. Duration of untreated illness was defined as the time from the first nonspecific symptom related to psychosis. Duration of untreated psychosis was established as the time from the first continuous psychotic symptom to initiation of antipsychotic drug treatment. Patients were randomly assigned to treatment with olanzapine, risperidone, or haloperidol [Reference Gómez-Revuelta, Pelayo-Terán, Juncal-Ruiz, Vázquez-Bourgon, Suárez-Pinilla and Romero-Jiménez34]. Positive symptoms were assessed by the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms [Reference Andreasen35], and negative symptoms by the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms [Reference Andreasen36]. Functioning was rated by the Global Assessment of Functioning [Reference Hall37]. Diagnoses were confirmed through the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-I) conducted by an experienced psychiatrist within 6 months of the baseline visit.
Estimation of IQ
Expert neuropsychologists administered the WAIS-III Vocabulary subtest [Reference Wechsler1] to estimate the IQ of all participants. This subtest has adequate properties as a proxy measure for crystallised intelligence in the general population and FEP [Reference Lezak, Howieson, Loring and Fischer38]. Crystallised intelligence is defined as knowledge acquired throughout life, including vocabulary, general information, culture, and specific skills [Reference Cattell39]. It represents the stored information and strategies that individuals draw on to solve common problems [Reference Ellingsen and Ackerman40]. Crystallised intelligence is more stable than fluid intelligence [Reference de Oliveira, Nitrini, Yassuda and Brucki41]; thus, the Vocabulary subtest would enable the estimation of cognitive abilities before the onset of psychosis in the FEP sample. This subtest is associated with educational attainment and the linguistic knowledge of one’s native language [Reference de Oliveira, Nitrini, Yassuda and Brucki41]. We have previously used Vocabulary as a proxy measure for premorbid intelligence, showing utility in studying the IQ of FEP patients [Reference Ayesa-Arriola, Setién-Suero, Neergaard, Belzunces, Contreras and van Haren42].
To estimate a proxy of the potential IQ of FEP patients, we calculated a “family-IQ” for each family. This score represents the mean IQ of all family members, including the FEP patient themself. We included patients in the estimation because 42% of our families consisted of only the proband and one other member (see Figure 1). See the details of family-IQ estimated from unaffected relatives only in the Supplementary Material.
Deviation from family-IQ was determined by calculating the difference between the individual and family scores. Positive deviations indicate that an individual’s IQ is above their family-IQ, while negative deviations indicate that it is below their family-IQ.
Genotyping and PGSs estimation
DNA was extracted from venous blood samples at baseline. Samples and data from patients included in this study were provided by the Biobank Valdecilla (PT20/00067), integrated into the Spanish Biobank Network and they were processed following standard operating procedures with the appropriate approval of the Ethical and Scientific Committees. The genotyping was performed at the Centro Nacional de Genotipado (Human Genotyping laboratory, CeGen) using the Global Screening Array v.3.0 panel (Illumina).
The quality control process was performed using PLINK 1.9. Single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) with a minor allele frequency of less than 0.01, missing data exceeding 0.02, or exhibiting deviation from Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium were removed. Participants were excluded if there were discrepancies in sex information or detected heterozygosity. A set of SNPs meeting high-quality criteria (HWE p > 0.001, MAF > 0.01) and subjected to linkage disequilibrium pruning was employed to assess relatedness. We confirmed the participants’ recorded relationships, in which PI-HAT values around 0.50 were considered to indicate first-degree relatives. Ancestry outliers were identified through principal component analysis based on 1000 Genomes Project European reference populations and subsequently removed (see Supplementary Figure S1). The final dataset comprised 525 participants and 492,348 SNPs. Genetic imputation was carried out in the Michigan Imputation Server using Minimac4 and individuals from the Haplotype Reference Consortium (HRC; Version r1.1) as the reference dataset. Genetic variants with MAF > 0.01 were kept. After imputation, 6,910,431 SNPs were available for downstream analyses.
We calculated PGS for each participant using the latest publicly available summary statistics for SCZ [Reference Trubetskoy, Pardiñas, Qi, Panagiotaropoulou, Awasthi and Bigdeli23] and IQ [Reference Savage, Jansen, Stringer, Watanabe, Bryois and de Leeuw21] by the method of polygenic continuous shrinkage (PGS-CS) [Reference Ge, Chen, Ni, Feng and Smoller43]. PGS-CS shrinks the effect sizes towards the population mean, thereby attenuating the influence of variants with unstable or exaggerated effects. This regularisation technique provides more reliable and interpretable PGS estimates, enhancing their predictive power and generalizability across different populations or cohorts. PGS was then calculated in PLINK 1.9 using imputed dosage data in this cohort.
After obtaining the PGS in our sample, we corrected it by their first five ancestry principal components. The aim was to control for their possible influence on our results. We regressed the effect of the principal components on the PGS using a linear model. Finally, we kept the residuals as the corrected PGS and standardised them.
Statistical analysis
We performed statistical analysis in R [44]. To take into account that our sample was related, we carried out linear mixed models (LMMs) using the “lme4” package.
In Equation 1), $ Y $ represents the dependent variable. The subscripts $ i $ and $ j $ on the $ Y $ indicate that each observation $ j $ is nested within cluster $ i $ , in this case, the family. $ {\beta}_0 $ is the overall intercept. $ {\beta}_1X $ represents the vector of fixed effects. $ {\upsilon}_i $ is the random effect of family code. $ \varepsilon $ is the error of the model. We adjusted the p-values by false discovery rate (FDR) and considered those equal to or less than 0.05 as significant.
Between-group comparisons were performed using separate LMMs, one for each dependent variable (IQ, deviation from family-IQ, PGS-SCZ, PGS-IQ, and sociodemographic) according to Equation (1). These models included the grouping variable as a fixed effect (FEP patient, sibling, parent, or control) and the family code as a random effect. We covariated IQ comparisons by sex, age, and years of education. Post hoc comparisons were conducted with Bonferroni correction and effect sizes were estimated using beta standardised coefficients.
Then, we performed the main analyses, consisting of four LMMs according to Equation 1), which were fitted to families without controls. All four models included the same covariates (sex, age, and years of education) and random effect (family code). The first and second models tested the predictive effect of PGS-SCZ on IQ and deviation from family-IQ, respectively. The third and fourth models tested the predictive effect of PGS-IQ on IQ and deviation from family-IQ, respectively.
We tested the potential effect of antipsychotic medication (chlorpromazine-equivalent dose at baseline) on patients’ IQ and found no significant results (p = 0.585). Therefore, the antipsychotic variable was excluded from the main analyses.
Results
Descriptive statistics and between-group comparisons
Of all subjects with PGS estimates, five were removed from the LMM analyses because they could not be nested within families (e.g., a dyad whose family member was removed in QC becomes incomplete). The final sample consisted of 344 relatives and 176 controls. Figure 1 displays the distribution of the 121 families included in the LMMs.
There was a higher proportion of men in the FEP and control groups compared to siblings and parents (p < 0.001). FEP patients were significantly younger than all other groups and had higher rates of cannabis use than controls and siblings (p < 0.050). Siblings were significantly older than controls and had completed more years of education than the other participants had (p < 0.001).
Table 1 shows post hoc comparisons between groups. After correcting for covariates, parents had significantly higher IQs than patients (p = 0.024) and controls (p = 0.018). FEP patients deviated more from family-IQ (p < 0.001) than their relatives. The FEP patients had significantly higher PGS-SCZ than all other groups (p < 0.001), and their parents had significantly higher PGS-SCZ than controls (p = 0.023) (Figure 2). PGS-IQ was not different between groups.
Abbreviations: DUI, duration of untreated illness; DUP, duration of untreated psychosis; GAF, global assessment of functioning; IQ, intelligence quotient; NA, not available; SANS, scale for the assessment of negative symptoms, SAPS, scale for the assessment of positive symptoms.
Notes: All post hoc comparisons were Bonferroni corrected and significant at p < 0.001 except when indicated. *IQ was covariated with age and years of education.
a Controls were used as the reference category in the models (intercept). Therefore, the effect sizes of the other groups represent their differences from the controls.
b Siblings were used as the reference category in the models (intercept).
Predictive effect of the PGSs on IQ and deviation from family-IQ
PGS-SCZ was not associated with IQ (β = −0.08, SE = 0.04, p = 0.53, pFDR = 0.63). However, PGS-SCZ significantly predicted IQ deviation from family-IQ (β = −0.17, SE = 0.05, pFDR = 0.003) (see the results detailed in Table 2).
Overall model 1: Wald = 194.86, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.46.
Overall model 2: Wald = 115.98, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.26.
PGS-IQ significantly predicted the individual IQ (β = 0.13, SE = 0.04, pFDR = 0.003) but showed a trend towards significance in predicting the deviation from family-IQ (β = 0.08, SE = 0.04, pFDR = 0.073) (see the results detailed in Table 3).
Overall model 1: Wald = 204.07, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.46.
Overall model 2: Wald = 104.02, p < 0.001, R 2 = 0.24.
Discussion
Through a family-based design, we add data on the association of the polygenic background of SCZ and IQ with general cognitive performance. We report, as expected, that PGS-SCZ is increased in FEP patients as compared to their relatives and controls. Our data also show that PGS-SCZ significantly predicts the individual’s deviation from the mean IQ of their relatives, whereas PGS-IQ is more predictive of the individual’s IQ.
Between-group differences in IQ, PGS-SCZ, and PGS-IQ
FEP patients had higher PGS-SCZ than other groups, with first-degree relatives having intermediate scores. This supports the efficacy of the PGS method in discerning varying levels of genetic predisposition to psychosis. While previous research indicates that PGS-SCZ can differentiate between FEP patients and controls [Reference Ferraro, Quattrone, La Barbera, La Cascia, Morgan and Jb25,Reference Harrisberger, Smieskova, Vogler, Egli, Schmidt and Lenz26], our findings suggest that it can also detect genetic risk variation within families. Although FEP patients showed PGS-IQ similar to other groups, their IQ scores were lower, suggesting unachieved cognitive potential. In addition, FEP patients showed a negative deviation from their family-IQ of 6.84 points on average. This is consistent with previous research describing a strong correlation between deviation from family cognitive ability and risk of SCZ [Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Mezuk, Sundquist and Sundquist10]. Such deviation is aligned with the well-reported cognitive impairments associated with SCZ [Reference Barrantes-Vidal, Aguilera, Campanera, Fatjó-Vilas, Guitart and Miret6], bringing at the same time new questions about the aetiological mechanisms underlying the intra-family differences. Thus, deviation from familial aptitude emerges as an important marker of neurodevelopmental processes predisposing to psychosis [Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Mezuk, Sundquist and Sundquist10].
We found that unaffected siblings have a lower PGS-SCZ than the proband, implying a slightly reduced genetic predisposition to SCZ. Siblings had similar IQs to controls, and their performance aligned with their family cognitive profile. Previous research consistently shows that siblings tend to perform better than the proband in cognitive domains such as executive functions and memory [Reference Barrantes-Vidal, Aguilera, Campanera, Fatjó-Vilas, Guitart and Miret6,Reference Murillo-García, Díaz-Pons, Fernández-Cacho, Miguel-Corredera, Martínez-Barrio and Ortiz-García de la Foz32,Reference Kuha, Tuulio-Henriksson, Eerola, Perälä, Suvisaari and Partonen45–Reference Scala, Lasalvia, Seidman, Cristofalo, Bonetto and Ruggeri47]. Siblings had higher educational attainment and lower cannabis use rates (Table 1), which may be protective factors that increase cognitive reserve against psychosis [Reference Ayesa-Arriola, de la, Murillo-García, Vázquez-Bourgon, Juncal-Ruiz and Gómez-Revuelta48,Reference Magdaleno Herrero, Murillo García, Yorca Ruiz, Neergaard, Crespo Facorro and Ayesa Arriola49].
Parents in our sample were found to have higher IQs than the other participants, including the healthy controls. This finding contrasts with previous evidence showing IQ deficits among first-degree relatives of FEP patients [Reference Barrantes-Vidal, Aguilera, Campanera, Fatjó-Vilas, Guitart and Miret6,Reference Cella, Hamid, Butt and Wykes7,Reference McIntosh, Harrison, Forrester, Lawrie and Johnstone9,Reference van Os, Marsman, van Dam and Simons50,Reference de Zwarte, Brouwer, Agartz, Alda, Alonso-Lana and Bearden51]. The discrepancy in results may be related to the neuropsychological measure used in our study. We estimated crystallised intelligence, which tends to increase with age [Reference Hartshorne and Germine52] and is strongly influenced by education [Reference Rindermann, Flores-Mendoza and Mansur-Alves53]. As parents in our sample are the oldest, age may have contributed to their IQ advantage.
Relationship between PGS-SCZ and deviation from family-IQ
Our research shows that PGS-SCZ can predict deviation from family-IQ, but it does not have any direct relationship with IQ. These findings converge with some previous studies showing no connection between genetic risk of SCZ and intelligence [Reference Engen, Lyngstad, Ueland, Simonsen, Vaskinn and Smeland54,Reference van, van der, Islam, Gülöksüz, Rutten and Simons55]. However, others have reported a direct correlation between higher PGS-SCZ and low intelligence in individuals at high risk of psychosis [Reference He, Jantac Mam-Lam-Fook, Chaignaud, Danset-Alexandre, Iftimovici and Gradels Hauguel56], with SCZ [Reference Ohi, Nishizawa, Sugiyama, Takai, Kuramitsu and Hasegawa29], and in controls [Reference McIntosh, Gow, Luciano, Davies, Liewald and Harris57,Reference Shafee, Nanda, Padmanabhan, Tandon, Alliey-Rodriguez and Kalapurakkel58]. Conflicting findings in the literature may be due to differences in neuropsychological measures and sample variation. An alternative explanation is that genetic risk for SCZ may influence longitudinal intellectual trajectories rather than cross-sectional IQ scores. Although the literature on FEP is limited, some insights can be drawn from studies of the general population. Germine et al. [Reference Germine, Robinson, Smoller, Calkins, Moore and Hakonarson59] described that PGS-SCZ was associated with reduced speed of emotion identification and verbal reasoning in childhood. McIntosh et al. [Reference McIntosh, Gow, Luciano, Davies, Liewald and Harris57] found that high PGS-SCZ was associated with greater cognitive decline. Therefore, this evidence suggests that genetic liability for SCZ may be related to specific cognitive domains at key life stages. These trajectories need to be explored in the FEP population, as long-term factors such as antipsychotic medication or disease progression may influence their cognitive outcomes.
Concerning intellectual family deviation, our findings indicate that an increase of one standard deviation in PGS-SCZ may lead to roughly 0.17 standard deviations of negative deviation from family-IQ. Following Kendler et al. [Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Mezuk, Sundquist and Sundquist10], we interpret that the genetic liability for SCZ indirectly influences intelligence by disrupting neurodevelopment and preventing the achievement of cognitive potential. In this regard, it could be suggested that increased genetic susceptibility to SCZ in FEP patients may shape developmental trajectories and/or make individuals more sensitive to environmental insults [Reference Guloksuz, Pries, Delespaul, Kenis, Luykx and Lin60,Reference Martin, Robinson, Reutens and Mowry61], leading to the onset of psychosis. This interpretation is based on existing evidence of a common genetic susceptibility between SCZ and neurodevelopmental disorders [Reference Owen and O’Donovan62,Reference Singh, Walters, Johnstone, Curtis, Suvisaari and Torniainen63], which, when combined with environmental risk factors [Reference Guloksuz, Pries, Delespaul, Kenis, Luykx and Lin60,Reference Schmitt, Falkai and Papiol64], can increase the likelihood of impaired cognitive development from an early age.
Relationship between PGS-IQ and IQ
We confirmed a strong association between PGS-IQ and IQ. This association has been previously reported in the general population [Reference Genç, Schlüter, Fraenz, Arning, Metzen and Nguyen19,Reference Loughnan, Palmer, Thompson, Dale, Jernigan and Chieh Fan22], and our study replicates it in the FEP population [Reference Ferraro, Quattrone, La Barbera, La Cascia, Morgan and Jb25,Reference Richards, Pardiñas, Frizzati, Tansey, Lynham and Holmans65]. As expected, polymorphic genetic factors explain a small percentage of the variance in IQ, suggesting that there is a very large amount of variability associated not only with other sources of genomic variability but also with environmental factors.
As PGS-IQ showed a trend towards predicting deviation from family-IQ (p = 0.073), the evidence for this relationship remains unclear. Deviation from family cognition may not solely reflect the risk of SCZ. It is also possible that a lower genetic predisposition to intelligence contributes to this deviation. Further research on IQ in FEP, particularly investigating indirect parental genetic effects, could provide more clarity [Reference Kong, Thorleifsson, Frigge, Vilhjálmsson, Young and Thorgeirsson66,Reference Pingault, Barkhuizen, Wang, Hannigan, Eilertsen and Corfield67]. Research has shown a robust effect of genetic nurture on education, influenced by parental education and socioeconomic status [Reference Wang, Baldwin, Schoeler, Cheesman, Barkhuizen and Dudbridge68,Reference Harden, Turkheimer and Loehlin69]. This pathway could be homologous to IQ, although this needs to be verified in future studies.
Strengths and limitations
The strength of this study lies in the use of neuropsychological and genetic data from FEP patients and their unaffected first-degree relatives. However, some limitations should also be acknowledged. First, the modest sample size of the study, especially when analysing subgroups, and the incomplete families with only sibling pairs limit the study of genetic transmission. In this regard, beyond larger samples future studies would also benefit from including both first-degree relatives of controls and affected and non-affected first-degree relatives of patients. Second, IQ estimation focuses on crystallised intelligence, and the results may not generalise to other types of intelligence such as fluid intelligence. Third, the inclusion of participants of European ancestry may limit the generalisation to diverse populations. Finally, potential biases may also arise from voluntary participation and the exclusion of relatives with a history of psychiatric diagnosis, which may result in a sample with preserved cognitive function. Further studies involving two or more people with psychosis in the same family may be relevant for studying populations at high risk of SCZ.
Conclusions
Based on a family-based design in an FEP population, we confirmed that the polygenic risk for SCZ is increased in the probands, whereas the first-degree relatives score is intermediate between patients and controls. This validates the polygenic background as a discernible marker of genetic risk variation within families. Additionally, our results indicated that the genetic load for SCZ significantly predicts the deviation from the family-IQ, explaining that FEP patients underperformed in the IQ test compared to their relatives. The genetic risk for SCZ may modulate cognition by shaping developmental trajectories and making individuals more sensitive to environmental insults, therefore, preventing individuals from reaching the familial cognitive potential. Further research is needed to determine the potential contribution of genetic liability for intelligence to the unrealised cognitive potential of FEP patients.
Supplementary material
The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1192/j.eurpsy.2024.24.
Acknowledgements
Thanks to all participants of PAFIP-FAMILIES. The authors thank the Biobank Valdecilla (PT20/00067), integrated into the Spanish Biobank Network, for its collaboration.
Competing of interest
The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
Funding support
The PAFIP-FAMILIES project was funded by the ISCIII (FIS PI17/00221). Genotyping was performed at the Human Genotyping laboratory (CeGen), supported by grant PT17/0019, of the PE I+D+i 2013-2016, funded by ISCIII and FEDER. Rosa Ayesa-Arriola and Mar Fatjó-Vilas were funded by Miguel Servet contracts from ISCIII (CP18/00003 and CP20/00072). Nancy Murillo-Garcia was funded by a predoctoral contract from IDIVAL (PREVAL20/05) and a scientific exchange grant from EMBO (SEG9790). Additional sources of funding include ISCIII (PI20/00066, PI14/00639, PI14/00918, MS18-Ayuda) and IDIVAL (INNVAL20/02, INNVAL23/21).
Comments
No Comments have been published for this article.