Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T20:37:32.070Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Differentiated nomological networks of internalizing, externalizing, and the general factor of psychopathology (‘p factor’) in emerging adolescence in the ABCD study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 January 2021

Sarah J. Brislin*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, 4250 Plymouth Rd, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
Meghan E. Martz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, 4250 Plymouth Rd, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
Sonalee Joshi
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, 4250 Plymouth Rd, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
Elizabeth R. Duval
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, 4250 Plymouth Rd, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
Arianna Gard
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, 4250 Plymouth Rd, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
D. Angus Clark
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, 4250 Plymouth Rd, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
Luke W. Hyde
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, 4250 Plymouth Rd, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
Brian M. Hicks
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, 4250 Plymouth Rd, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
Aman Taxali
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, 4250 Plymouth Rd, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
Mike Angstadt
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, 4250 Plymouth Rd, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
Saige Rutherford
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, 4250 Plymouth Rd, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
Mary M. Heitzeg
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, 4250 Plymouth Rd, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
Chandra Sripada*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, 4250 Plymouth Rd, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Sarah J. Brislin, brislins@med.umich.edu; Chandra Sripada, E-mail: sripada@umich.edu
Author for correspondence: Sarah J. Brislin, brislins@med.umich.edu; Chandra Sripada, E-mail: sripada@umich.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Structural models of psychopathology consistently identify internalizing (INT) and externalizing (EXT) specific factors as well as a superordinate factor that captures their shared variance, the p factor. Questions remain, however, about the meaning of these data-driven dimensions and the interpretability and distinguishability of the larger nomological networks in which they are embedded.

Methods

The sample consisted of 10 645 youth aged 9–10 years participating in the multisite Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study. p, INT, and EXT were modeled using the parent-rated Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL). Patterns of associations were examined with variables drawn from diverse domains including demographics, psychopathology, temperament, family history of substance use and psychopathology, school and family environment, and cognitive ability, using instruments based on youth-, parent-, and teacher-report, and behavioral task performance.

Results

p exhibited a broad pattern of statistically significant associations with risk variables across all domains assessed, including temperament, neurocognition, and social adversity. The specific factors exhibited more domain-specific patterns of associations, with INT exhibiting greater fear/distress and EXT exhibiting greater impulsivity.

Conclusions

In this largest study of hierarchical models of psychopathology to date, we found that p, INT, and EXT exhibit well-differentiated nomological networks that are interpretable in terms of neurocognition, impulsivity, fear/distress, and social adversity. These networks were, in contrast, obscured when relying on the a priori Internalizing and Externalizing dimensions of the CBCL scales. Our findings add to the evidence for the validity of p, INT, and EXT as theoretically and empirically meaningful broad psychopathology liabilities.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Early adolescence is an important developmental period when biopsychosocial transitions (e.g. school entry, puberty) increase risk for youth psychopathology (Crone & Dahl, Reference Crone and Dahl2012; Smetana, Campione-Barr, & Metzger, Reference Smetana, Campione-Barr and Metzger2006). Symptoms that emerge during this time are predictive of severe forms of adult psychopathology (Copeland, Shanahan, Costello, & Angold, Reference Copeland, Shanahan, Costello and Angold2009) and perniciously impact health, wealth, and wellbeing across the life course (Copeland, Wolke, Shanahan, & Costello, Reference Copeland, Wolke, Shanahan and Costello2015; Erskine et al., Reference Erskine, Ferrari, Polanczyk, Moffitt, Murray, Vos and Scott2014). However, efforts to identify at-risk youth and develop effective treatments may be hampered by the current categorical schemes of psychiatric diagnosis, which yield troublingly high levels of comorbidity suggesting disorder boundaries drawn incorrectly (Angold, Costello, & Erkanli, Reference Angold, Costello and Erkanli1999; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, Merikangas and Walters2005; Krueger & Markon, Reference Krueger and Markon2006), rely on arbitrary thresholds for meeting disorder criteria (Lewinsohn, Shankman, Gau, & Klein, Reference Lewinsohn, Shankman, Gau and Klein2004; Rucci et al., Reference Rucci, Gherardi, Tansella, Piccinelli, Berardi, Bisoffi and Pini2003), and which may not line up well with neural mechanisms and neurodevelopmental processes (Casey, Oliveri, & Insel, Reference Casey, Oliveri and Insel2014; Cuthbert, Reference Cuthbert2014).

Alternative, dimensional models of psychopathology have emerged primarily by using factor analytic techniques to identify patterns of covariance across psychiatric symptoms. Initial work defined two correlated dimensions: internalizing (INT), reflecting covariance among depression and anxiety diagnoses, and externalizing (EXT), capturing comorbidity among substance use and delinquent behavior (Achenbach, Reference Achenbach1966; Krueger, Reference Krueger1999; Krueger & Markon, Reference Krueger and Markon2006). Further work, however, also identified a superordinate dimension, the ‘p factor’ (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington, Israel and Poulton2014; Caspi & Moffitt, Reference Caspi and Moffitt2018; Lahey et al., Reference Lahey, Applegate, Hakes, Zald, Hariri and Rathouz2012, Reference Lahey, Rathouz, Keenan, Stepp, Loeber and Hipwell2015; Patalay et al., Reference Patalay, Fonagy, Deighton, Belsky, Vostanis and Wolpert2015; Smith, Atkinson, Davis, Riley, & Oltmanns, Reference Smith, Atkinson, Davis, Riley and Oltmanns2020; Tackett et al., Reference Tackett, Lahey, Van Hulle, Waldman, Krueger and Rathouz2013), that captures their shared variance. This hierarchical structure reliably emerges across different study designs (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington, Israel and Poulton2014, Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Harrington, Hogan, Ramrakha and Moffitt2017; Castellanos-Ryan et al., Reference Castellanos-Ryan, Brière, O'Leary-Barrett, Banaschewski, Bokde, Bromberg and Gallinat2016; Lahey et al., Reference Lahey, Rathouz, Keenan, Stepp, Loeber and Hipwell2015; Murray, Eisner, & Ribeaud, Reference Murray, Eisner and Ribeaud2016; Noordhof, Krueger, Ormel, Oldehinkel, & Hartman, Reference Noordhof, Krueger, Ormel, Oldehinkel and Hartman2015; Patalay et al., Reference Patalay, Fonagy, Deighton, Belsky, Vostanis and Wolpert2015; Pettersson, Lahey, Larsson, Lundstroem, & Lichtenstein, Reference Pettersson, Lahey, Larsson, Lundstroem and Lichtenstein2015; Stochl et al., Reference Stochl, Khandaker, Lewis, Perez, Goodyer, Zammit and Jones2015), across age groups including children (Lahey et al., Reference Lahey, Rathouz, Keenan, Stepp, Loeber and Hipwell2015; Martel et al., Reference Martel, Pan, Hoffmann, Gadelha, do Rosário, Mari and Salum2017; Murray et al., Reference Murray, Eisner and Ribeaud2016; Neumann et al., Reference Neumann, Pappa, Lahey, Verhulst, Medina-Gomez, Jaddoe and Tiemeier2016; Waldman, Poore, van Hulle, Rathouz, & Lahey, Reference Waldman, Poore, van Hulle, Rathouz and Lahey2016), adolescents (Blanco et al., Reference Blanco, Wall, He, Krueger, Olfson, Jin and Merikangas2015; Bloemen et al., Reference Bloemen, Oldehinkel, Laceulle, Ormel, Rommelse and Hartman2018; Carragher et al., Reference Carragher, Teesson, Sunderland, Newton, Krueger, Conrod and Slade2016; Castellanos-Ryan et al., Reference Castellanos-Ryan, Brière, O'Leary-Barrett, Banaschewski, Bokde, Bromberg and Gallinat2016; Laceulle, Vollebergh, & Ormel, Reference Laceulle, Vollebergh and Ormel2015; Lahey et al., Reference Lahey, Rathouz, Van Hulle, Urbano, Krueger, Applegate and Waldman2008, Reference Lahey, Van Hulle, Singh, Waldman and Rathouz2011; Noordhof et al., Reference Noordhof, Krueger, Ormel, Oldehinkel and Hartman2015; Patalay et al., Reference Patalay, Fonagy, Deighton, Belsky, Vostanis and Wolpert2015; Stochl et al., Reference Stochl, Khandaker, Lewis, Perez, Goodyer, Zammit and Jones2015; Tackett et al., Reference Tackett, Lahey, Van Hulle, Waldman, Krueger and Rathouz2013; Waldman et al., Reference Waldman, Poore, van Hulle, Rathouz and Lahey2016), and adults (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington, Israel and Poulton2014; Lahey et al., Reference Lahey, Applegate, Hakes, Zald, Hariri and Rathouz2012; Stochl et al., Reference Stochl, Khandaker, Lewis, Perez, Goodyer, Zammit and Jones2015; Wright & Simms, Reference Wright and Simms2015), and across assessment methods (Blanco et al., Reference Blanco, Wall, He, Krueger, Olfson, Jin and Merikangas2015; Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Achenbach, Althoff, Bagby and Clark2017; Stochl et al., Reference Stochl, Khandaker, Lewis, Perez, Goodyer, Zammit and Jones2015). Recently work also found that subjects' rank-order on p, INT, and EXT dimensions are robust across a wide variety of modeling choices (e.g. bifactor models and higher-order factor models; Clark et al., Reference Clark, Hicks, Angstadt, Rutherford, Taxali, Hyde and Sripada2020). Despite these trends, controversy lingers about the meaning of these data-driven dimensions: Do they represent real liabilities for broad psychopathological risk, or do they instead perhaps reflect ‘artifactual’ causes (e.g. rater response style, general distress at the time of assessment)?

The validity of hypothesized constructs ultimately depends upon locating them in nomological networks, that is, theoretically coherent pattern of linkages among the constructs, other constructs, and observable variables that accounts for their interrelationships (Cronbach & Meehl, Reference Cronbach and Meehl1955). Prior studies made progress on this front, reporting that the p factor is associated with temperament (high negative emotionality, low constraint, high impulsivity; Brandes, Herzhoff, Smack, and Tackett, Reference Brandes, Herzhoff, Smack and Tackett2019; Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington, Israel and Poulton2014; Castellanos-Ryan et al., Reference Castellanos-Ryan, Brière, O'Leary-Barrett, Banaschewski, Bokde, Bromberg and Gallinat2016; Hankin et al., Reference Hankin, Davis, Snyder, Young, Glynn and Sandman2017; Tackett et al., Reference Tackett, Lahey, Van Hulle, Waldman, Krueger and Rathouz2013), poor executive function and lower cognitive ability scores (Bloemen et al., Reference Bloemen, Oldehinkel, Laceulle, Ormel, Rommelse and Hartman2018; Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington, Israel and Poulton2014; Castellanos-Ryan et al., Reference Castellanos-Ryan, Brière, O'Leary-Barrett, Banaschewski, Bokde, Bromberg and Gallinat2016; Hankin et al., Reference Hankin, Davis, Snyder, Young, Glynn and Sandman2017; Lahey et al., Reference Lahey, Rathouz, Keenan, Stepp, Loeber and Hipwell2015; Martel et al., Reference Martel, Pan, Hoffmann, Gadelha, do Rosário, Mari and Salum2017; Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Barch, Tian, Watson, Klein and Kotov2019; Snyder, Friedman, & Hankin, Reference Snyder, Friedman and Hankin2019a), worse mental health outcomes (Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Barch, Tian, Watson, Klein and Kotov2019; Pettersson, Lahey, Larsson, & Lichtenstein, Reference Pettersson, Lahey, Larsson and Lichtenstein2018), common genetic loading across psychiatric disorders (Neumann et al., Reference Neumann, Pappa, Lahey, Verhulst, Medina-Gomez, Jaddoe and Tiemeier2016; Selzam, Coleman, Caspi, Moffitt, & Plomin, Reference Selzam, Coleman, Caspi, Moffitt and Plomin2018), structural and functional neural alterations (e.g. reduced grey matter volume) (Alnæs et al., Reference Alnæs, Kaufmann, Doan, Córdova-Palomera, Wang, Bettella and Westlye2018; Elliott, Romer, Knodt, & Hariri, Reference Elliott, Romer, Knodt and Hariri2018), and socioenvironmental variables including lower birth weight, unsupportive and hostile parenting, and lower socioeconomic status (Carver, Johnson, & Timpano, Reference Carver, Johnson and Timpano2017; Caspi & Moffitt, Reference Caspi and Moffitt2018). In contrast to p, less is known about INT and EXT in the context of these hierarchical models, especially bifactor models. INT has been consistently associated with elevation in fear/distress, while associations with other variables (e.g. positive affect; Hankin et al., Reference Hankin, Davis, Snyder, Young, Glynn and Sandman2017), neurocognition) have been inconsistent. This could be due to the failure to account for shared variance with the p factor. For example, internalizing disorders are associated with reduced cognitive abilities (Levin, Heller, Mohanty, Herrington, & Miller, Reference Levin, Heller, Mohanty, Herrington and Miller2007; Rapport, Denney, Chung, & Hustace, Reference Rapport, Denney, Chung and Hustace2001; Snyder, Reference Snyder2013), while studies that remove the variance associated with p in a bifactor model have found the remaining INT factor is associated with elevated cognitive abilities (Lahey et al., Reference Lahey, Rathouz, Keenan, Stepp, Loeber and Hipwell2015; Masten et al., Reference Masten, Roisman, Long, Burt, Obradović, Riley and Tellegen2005; Patalay et al., Reference Patalay, Fonagy, Deighton, Belsky, Vostanis and Wolpert2015; Tackett et al., Reference Tackett, Lahey, Van Hulle, Waldman, Krueger and Rathouz2013). Research has also been limited on nomological associations of EXT after accounting for p. Previous studies linked the externalizing dimension (Beauchaine & McNulty, Reference Beauchaine and McNulty2013; Hinshaw, Reference Hinshaw2002) to psychological deficits, including reduced neurocognition (Bloemen et al., Reference Bloemen, Oldehinkel, Laceulle, Ormel, Rommelse and Hartman2018), effortful control (Deutz et al., Reference Deutz, Geeraerts, Belsky, Deković, van Baar, Prinzie and Patalay2020; Hankin et al., Reference Hankin, Davis, Snyder, Young, Glynn and Sandman2017), and socioemotional functioning, as well as environmental risk factors, including low SES, reduced environmental enrichment, and harsh parenting (Beauchaine, Shader, & Hinshaw, Reference Beauchaine, Shader, Hinshaw, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2016). However, most previous work was conducted without simultaneously modeling the p factor, and therefore it is unclear if these associations are unique to EXT or driven by p.

In the present study, we build upon and extend previous work by examining the nomological networks of the p and INT and EXT specific factors in baseline data from the ABCD multisite study (Volkow et al., Reference Volkow, Koob, Croyle, Bianchi, Gordon, Koroshetz and Conway2018) of 118 759- to10-year-old emerging adolescents. While recent work by our group and others also has sought to define the structure of psychopathology in this sample (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Hicks, Angstadt, Rutherford, Taxali, Hyde and Sripada2020; Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Barch, Tian, Watson, Klein and Kotov2019; Moore et al., Reference Moore, Kaczkurkin, Durham, Jeong, McDowell, Dupont and Lahey2020), these studies have focused primarily comparing alternative structural models of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Hicks, Angstadt, Rutherford, Taxali, Hyde and Sripada2020; Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Barch, Tian, Watson, Klein and Kotov2019; Moore et al., Reference Moore, Kaczkurkin, Durham, Jeong, McDowell, Dupont and Lahey2020) and validated the derived factors focusing on a small number of criterion variables with the goal of comparing the different factors (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Hicks, Angstadt, Rutherford, Taxali, Hyde and Sripada2020; Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Barch, Tian, Watson, Klein and Kotov2019; Moore et al., Reference Moore, Kaczkurkin, Durham, Jeong, McDowell, Dupont and Lahey2020). The current study will examine the convergent and divergent associations between the general (p) and specific (EXT and INT) factors and a comprehensive, multi-domain pool of criterion measures. We focus on delineating the full unique and overlapping profiles of these latent liabilities, as well as comparing their respective nomological network with those that emerge from the original CBCL Internalizing and Externalizing scales. The ABCD study involves a comprehensive assessment battery (Barch et al., Reference Barch, Albaugh, Avenevoli, Chang, Clark, Glantz and Sher2018), covering demographics, psychopathology, temperament, family history of substance use and psychopathology, socio-environment (school and family environment), and cognitive ability. Multiple assessment modalities are also included, such as youth-, parent-, and teacher-report questionnaires, interviews, and behavioral task performance. Moreover, the ABCD study used a multi-stage probability sampling strategy, and with sample weighting approximates a representative US population sample of 9- and 10-year olds (Garavan et al., Reference Garavan, Bartsch, Conway, Decastro, Goldstein, Heeringa and Zahs2018), thus improving generalizability (Deutz et al., Reference Deutz, Geeraerts, Belsky, Deković, van Baar, Prinzie and Patalay2020; Snyder, Young, & Hankin, Reference Snyder, Young and Hankin2019b). Results from our investigation found that p, INT, and EXT exhibit distinct and well-differentiated nomological networks that are readily interpreted in terms of neurocognitive, temperamental, and social factors. These nomological networks were, in contrast, obscured when relying on the a priori internalizing and externalizing dimensions of the CBCL scales.

Methods

Participants

The study used data collected from the ABCD Study, a large-scale study of youth aged 9–10 years (N = 10 645), recruited from 21 research sites across the USA (Barch et al., Reference Barch, Albaugh, Avenevoli, Chang, Clark, Glantz and Sher2018; Garavan et al., Reference Garavan, Bartsch, Conway, Decastro, Goldstein, Heeringa and Zahs2018; Volkow et al., Reference Volkow, Koob, Croyle, Bianchi, Gordon, Koroshetz and Conway2018). These data were collected from baseline visits between 1 September 2016 and 15 November 2018. The data used in this report came from ABCD Release 2.01, DOI: 10.15154/1504041. The sample was roughly gender-balanced (47.6% female) with a mean age of 9.93 years (s.d. = 0.62 years). Around half (50.7%) of the sample was White, with the remaining participants identifying themselves as Hispanic (19.7%), African-American (14.5%), Other/Multi-racial (9.8%), or Asian (2.1%). Data on ethnicity was missing for 3.2% of the sample. Approximately two-thirds of youth (65.5%) came from households in which the parents were married. Most parents (83.0%) reported at least some college and most households reported an annual income of at least $50 000 (62.6%).

Measures

The Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach et al., Reference Achenbach and Ruffle2000) was used to compute the standard Internalizing Problems and Externalizing Problems composite scales. The Withdrawn, Somatic Complaints, and Anxious/Depressed scales all contribute to higher order Internalizing Problems composite scale, while the Delinquent Behavior and Aggressive Behavior scales contribute to a higher order Externalizing Problems composite scale. The Internalizing and Externalizing Problems scales were t-scored by gender.

p, INT, and EXT

A general p factor and orthogonal EXT and INT factors were modeled using the parent-rated CBCL (age 6–18; Achenbach et al., Reference Achenbach and Ruffle2000). The p, EXT, and INT factor scores used in subsequent analyses were derived by fitting a bifactor model to the 8 CBCL scales (Withdrawn, Somatic Complaints, Anxious/Depressed, Social Problems, Thought Problems, Attention Problems, Delinquent Behavior, and Aggressive Behavior). In this model, there was a general p factor that all scales loaded onto (average scale loading on p = 0.69), and two specific factors: EXT and INT (average scale loading on sub-factors = 0.43). The EXT specific factor included the Delinquent and Aggressive Behaviors scales, while the INT-specific factor included the Withdrawn, Somatic Complaints, and Anxious/Depressed scales. This model fit well was based on conventional fit thresholds (χ2 = 747.73, df = 16, p < 0.001; RMSEA = 0.062; CFI = 0.985; TLI = 0.974; SRMR = 0.015) and was chosen for its good model fit and theoretical interpretability. Across a variety of alternative specifications of hierarchical models of psychopathology in ABCD, the resulting p, INT, and EXT factors that emerge are broadly similar (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Hicks, Angstadt, Rutherford, Taxali, Hyde and Sripada2020), rendering decisions about which specific modeling strategy to adopt less consequential.

Family history variables

Family History of Substance Problems was computed from the ABCD's Family History Assessment (Barch et al., Reference Barch, Albaugh, Avenevoli, Chang, Clark, Glantz and Sher2018). A threshold is established for a family member counting as an affected case based on the number of serious problems that person has had due to alcohol use and substance use. The following coding was used: 0 = neither parent met the threshold; 1 = one or more parents met the threshold.

Family History of Psychopathology Based on previously published protocols (Milne et al., Reference Milne, Moffitt, Crump, Poulton, Rutter, Sears and Caspi2008, Reference Milne, Caspi, Harrington, Poulton, Rutter and Moffitt2009), a family history composite score was constructed from responses for ABCD's Family History Assessment (Barch et al., Reference Barch, Albaugh, Avenevoli, Chang, Clark, Glantz and Sher2018; see Supplement for further description).

Area deprivation index

Area deprivation index (ADI) scores were available for each participant for up to three residences. A weighted average of ADI scores was computed based on months lived at each residence.

The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5) psychopathology

The Kiddie-Structured Assessment for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for DSM-5 (KSADS-5) is a structured, diagnostic interview that was administered to parents via computer in reference to their child (Barch et al., Reference Barch, Albaugh, Avenevoli, Chang, Clark, Glantz and Sher2018; Kobak, Kratochvil, Stanger, & Kaufman, Reference Kobak, Kratochvil, Stanger and Kaufman2013, see online Supplementary Methods for detailed description of coding).

The Prodromal Questionnaire- Brief Version (PQ-B; Loewy, Bearden, Johnson, Raine, and Cannon, Reference Loewy, Bearden, Johnson, Raine and Cannon2005) is a youth-report measure designed to index subclinical prodromal psychosis risk phenotypes.

The Brief Problem Monitoring Form (BPM; Piper, Gray, Raber, and Birkett, Reference Piper, Gray, Raber and Birkett2014) is an abbreviated, 18-item version of the CBCL that was sent to teachers for completion. Completion rate was 35% (N = 4495). This measure produces three t-scores regarding youth psychopathology: Internalizing Problems, Externalizing Problems, and Attention Problems.

Trait measures

The behavioral inhibition system/behavioral activation system (BIS/BAS) measure (Pagliaccio et al., Reference Pagliaccio, Luking, Anokhin, Gotlib, Hayden, Olino and Barch2016) is a 24-item scale designed to assess three facets of behavioral activation, reflecting positive affect: Drive, Fun Seeking, and Reward Responsiveness, and a Behavioral Inhibition scale, indexing sensitivity to punishment.

A 20-item youth short version of the Urgency, Premeditation (lack of), Perseverance (lack of), Sensation Seeking, Positive Urgency, Impulsive Behavior Scale (UPPS-P), developed for the use in the ABCD study (Barch et al., Reference Barch, Albaugh, Avenevoli, Chang, Clark, Glantz and Sher2018) was administered via self-report at baseline to index trait impulsivity yielding five subscales: Negative Urgency, Positive Urgency, Lack of Perseverance, Lack of Planning, and Sensation Seeking.

School environment

Youth reported on School Risk and Protective Factors (SRPF) to assess their connection to the school environment (Zucker et al., Reference Zucker, Gonzalez, Ewing, Paulus, Arroyo, Fuligni and Wills2018). This measure was taken from the PhenX Toolkit, yielding three subscales: School Environment, School Involvement, and School Disengagement scale.

Social functioning

Parents and youth reported on the prosocial behavior of the youth using the Prosocial Behavior Scale, which is a 3-item scale formed from the ‘Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire’ (SDQ; Goodman, Reference Goodman2001).

Family environment

Parent and child both rated the quality of the family environment with the Family Conflict subscale from the PhenX Toolkit Family Environment Scale (Zucker et al., Reference Zucker, Gonzalez, Ewing, Paulus, Arroyo, Fuligni and Wills2018).

Youth reported on their perceived level of parental monitoring using the Parental Monitoring Survey, a scale developed to assess parents' efforts to keep track of their child's whereabouts (Zucker et al., Reference Zucker, Gonzalez, Ewing, Paulus, Arroyo, Fuligni and Wills2018).

Neurocognition

General Neurocognition (GN) scores were computed by fitting a bifactor model to behavioral tasks from the NIH toolbox, the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Task, the WISC-V, and the ‘Little Man’ task (Sripada, Taxali, Angstadt, & Rutherford, Reference Sripada, Taxali, Angstadt and Rutherford2020). Exploratory factor analyses suggested that three broad factors characterized these tasks, corresponding to memory, speed/flexibility, and reasoning; in the bifactor model, these three factors served as the specific factors. The bifactor model fit well by conventional standards: χ2 = 443.16, df = 34, p < 0.001; RMSEA = 0.032; CFI = 0.990; TLI = 0.983; SRMR = 0.017 (West, Taylor, & Wu, Reference West, Taylor and Wu2012). GN factor scores (i.e. scores for the general factor in the bifactor model) were generated using maximum a posteriori scoring (MacCallum, Reference MacCallum2009).

Data analytic strategy

All analyses were performed using R. Data were weighted to correspond to the American Community Survey proportions and analyses accounted for clustering within the collection site and family (Heeringa & Berglund, Reference Heeringa and Berglund2020)Footnote Footnote 1. All SEM analyses were performed with laavan (open-source code at https://github.com/SripadaLab/ABCD_nomological_networks). Factor scores were estimated in factor models as described above, and regressions involving these factors were concurrently performed (in the latent space), controlling for the following covariates: participant sex, race, parent education, parent marital status, household income. Associations between CBCL Internalizing Problems and Externalizing Problems t-scores (gender normed) with the same set of independent variables were examined using multilevel models, with the same covariates excluding sex. When examining associations with demographic variables, all demographic variables were entered into one model. Given the large sample size, we chose to use the conservative alpha level a False Discovery Rate Benjamini-Hochburg corrected p < 0.001 to determine significance.

Results

p. Regarding demographics and family history (Table 1, Fig. 1a) p scores were higher for males relative to females, white relative to African-American, Hispanic, and Asian youth, children in lower income households, children with unmarried parents, and children with a higher family loading of mental health and substance use problems. p factor scores were significantly associated with all KSADS diagnoses, prodromal psychotic symptoms, and teacher ratings of Internalizing, Externalizing, and Attention problems (Table 2, Fig. 1b). Regarding associations with personality traits (Table 2, Fig. 2a) p scores were associated with higher BIS and BAS Drive and Fun Seeking scale scores as well as higher Negative Urgency, Positive Urgency, Lack of Planning and Lack of Perseverance scores. In addition, p was associated with lower scores on GN (Table 2, Fig. 2a). Lastly, p factor scores were associated with the worse school environment, lower school involvement, greater school disengagement, less prosocial behavior, more family conflict, and less parental monitoring (Table 3, Fig. 2b).

Fig. 1. Visualization of associations between p, INT, and EXT and independent variables. (a) Standardized beta weights with demographic variables; (b) Standardized beta weights with KSAD Diagnoses.

Fig. 2. Visualization of associations between p, INT, and EXT and independent variables. (a) Standardized beta weights with teacher-rated psychopathology (BPM), trait measures (BIS/BAS and UPPS), and general neurocognition (GN); (b) Significant associations with school environment (School Risk and Protective Factors), Prosocial Behavior (youth and parent report), Family Conflict (youth and parent report), and Parental Monitoring (youth report).

Table 1. Associations between demographic variables and p, Internalizing (INT), and Externalizing (EXT) factor scores

Note. Significance (*p < 0.001) determined from latent variable model where associations between p, INT, and EXT and all demographic variables were weighted to correspond to American Community Survey proportions and accounted for in the same model. Separate models were used to examine associations between p, INT, and EXT and Family History variables and Area Deprivation Index. Comparison groups for β's are Male (Sex); White (Race/Ethnicity); Married (Parental Marital Status); Less than High School Education (Highest Parental Education); <$100 000 (Household Income); No Parents with Substance Use Problems (Family History of Substance Use Problems); all other variables are continuous. Bolded values |β| ⩾ 0.20, denote at least a medium effect size.

Table 2. Associations with psychopathology, trait measures, and neurocognition

Note. Standardized betas from regression analyses with p, INT, and EXT and include the following covariates: participant sex, race, parent education level, marital status, and household income. For CBCL Scale analyses standardized betas are from separate multilevel models (MLMs) and sex was excluded as a covariate as scales were t-scored by sex. Analyses were weighted to correspond to American Community Survey proportions and clustered by testing site and family ID. ADHD = Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder; ODD = Oppositional Defiant Disorder. * False Discovery Rate corrected p < 0.001; bolded values |β|⩾ 0.20, denote at least a medium effect size.

Table 3. Associations with social environment measures

Note. Standardized betas from regression analyses with p, INT, and EXT and include the following covariates: participant sex, race, parent education level, marital status, and household income. For CBCL Scale analyses standardized betas are from separate multilevel models (MLMs) and sex was excluded as a covariate as scales were t-scored by sex. Analyses were weighted to correspond to American Community Survey proportions and clustered by testing site and family ID. * False Discovery Rate corrected p < 0.001; bolded values |β| ⩾ 0.20, denote at least a medium effect size.

INT. When examining associations between demographic and family variables and INT factor scores (Table 1, Fig. 1a), INT scores were higher for females compared to males and lower for African-American youth compared to white youth. INT factor scores were also higher for youth from households with higher levels of parental education and with a higher family loading of mental health problems. Regarding associations with psychopathology, INT factor scores were higher for individuals with depression, suicide/self-harm, anxiety disorders, and teacher ratings of internalizing problems (Table 2, Fig. 1b). INT scores were significantly lower for individuals with an attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) diagnosis, consistent with a negative association with teacher ratings of EXT and attention problems. INT factor scores were significantly associated with higher BIS and lower scores on all BAS facet scales, as well as lower Positive Urgency, Lack of Planning, and Sensation Seeking scores, reflecting a more emotionally and behaviorally constrained personality (Table 2, Fig. 2a). INT scores were also associated with higher scores on GN. Lastly, higher INT factor scores were associated with more family conflict per parent report (Table 3, Fig. 2b).

EXT. When examining associations between demographic and family history variables and EXT factor scores (Table 1, Fig. 1a), EXT scores were higher in males compared to females, African-American compared to white youth, and youth with unmarried parents. EXT factor scores were also higher for individuals who had at least one parent with a substance use problem. Regarding DSM-5 psychopathology (Table 2, Fig. 1b), youth diagnosed with conduct disorder (CD), oppositional defiant disorder, or bipolar disorder scored higher on the EXT factor than those without diagnoses. These findings are consistent with positive associations between EXT factor scores and teacher ratings of externalizing problems. Showing discriminant associations, youth that was diagnosed with generalized or social anxiety scored lower on the EXT factor than those that did not meet criteria for diagnosis. EXT factor scores were associated with lower BIS scores, higher BAS-Drive and Fun Seeking scores, and higher Negative Urgency, Positive Urgency, Lack of Planning, and Sensation Seeking scores (Table 2, Fig. 2a). Lastly, EXT factor scores were less prosocial behavior and more family conflict.

CBCL Internalizing Problems and Externalizing Problems

In contrast to findings for INT (which accounts for the variance due to p), the CBCL Internalizing Problems scale showed a relatively non-specific pattern of associations, with positive associations with mood and anxiety disorders as expected, but also with CD, ODD, and ADHD (Table 2). In addition, the Internalizing Problems scores were associated with higher levels of trait impulsivity, as evidenced by positive associations with UPPS subscales, counterintuitive to conceptualizations of internalizing proneness as reflecting high levels of constraint. Furthermore, in contrast to EXT, the CBCL Externalizing Problems scale again showed a non-specific pattern of associations with nearly all DSM-5 psychopathology, including, somewhat counterintuitively, internalizing disorders such as depression and anxiety disorders. Scores on the Externalizing Problems scale were also associated with higher scores on the prodromal psychosis scale, teacher-reported internalizing, externalizing, and attention problems, impulsivity, and lower scores on GN.

Discussion

We delineated the nomological networks of three major dimensions that emerge in hierarchical models of psychopathology, the general factor of psychopathology (‘p factor’) and specific INT and EXT factors, in a large, diverse sample of emerging adolescents. We found p, INT, and EXT were each associated with nomological networks readily distinguishable along four axes: neurocognition (p negatively associated, INT positively associated, EXT unrelated), fear/distress emotions (p and INT positively associated), impulsivity (p and EXT positively associated), and social adversity (p and EXT positively associated). Notably, these distinct nomological networks were obscured when looking at the CBCL Internalizing and Externalizing Problems scores without accounting for their substantial overlap (i.e. without accounting for a shared variance from a superordinate p factor). Overall, our results show that three major dimensions that emerge from hierarchical models of psychopathology form clearly distinguishable and readily interpretable nomological networks, adding to the evidence that they help to ‘carve nature at its joints’.

Our results highlight distinct cognitive and temperamental profiles associated with p, INT, and EXT, perhaps offering insights into the psychological and neural mechanisms that drive these broad liabilities. As a summary, Table 4 highlights associations between p/INT/EXT and four dimensions of importance: GN, two emotion/impulse dimensions: a fear/distress dimension (indexed by BIS and KSADS symptom profile), an impulsivity dimension (indexed by UPPS and BAS subscales and KSADS symptom profile), and a social adversity dimension (indexed by School Risk and Protective Factors and Family Conflict scales). p's position in this space reflects more pervasive alterations involving low neurocognition as well as elevations in both fear/distress and impulsivity profiles in the context of high social and environmental adversity. This might reflect a state in which there are both globally elevated emotions – negative and positive – and impulses as well as reduced executive control capacities to modulate these impulses (Carver et al., Reference Carver, Johnson and Timpano2017; Caspi & Moffitt, Reference Caspi and Moffitt2018; Deutz et al., Reference Deutz, Geeraerts, Belsky, Deković, van Baar, Prinzie and Patalay2020). EXT and INT, in contrast, reflect domain-specific alterations: elevated impulsivity profile in EXT and elevated inhibition profile in INT. INT is in addition associated with elevated neurocognition, which in turn might help explain INT's complex pattern of associations. For example, it is possible that higher neurocognition contributes to higher fear/distress symptoms due to greater prospection and associated ruminative worry (Penney, Miedema, & Mazmanian, Reference Penney, Miedema and Mazmanian2015), but at the same time, it is protective for attention problems (i.e. lower BPM Attention and ADHD) due to enhanced cognitive control. Overall, while our pattern of findings is consistent with previous studies (Bloemen et al., Reference Bloemen, Oldehinkel, Laceulle, Ormel, Rommelse and Hartman2018; Brandes et al., Reference Brandes, Herzhoff, Smack and Tackett2019; Carver et al., Reference Carver, Johnson and Timpano2017; Castellanos-Ryan et al., Reference Castellanos-Ryan, Brière, O'Leary-Barrett, Banaschewski, Bokde, Bromberg and Gallinat2016; Deutz et al., Reference Deutz, Geeraerts, Belsky, Deković, van Baar, Prinzie and Patalay2020; Hankin et al., Reference Hankin, Davis, Snyder, Young, Glynn and Sandman2017; Martel et al., Reference Martel, Pan, Hoffmann, Gadelha, do Rosário, Mari and Salum2017; Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Barch, Tian, Watson, Klein and Kotov2019; Moore et al., Reference Moore, Kaczkurkin, Durham, Jeong, McDowell, Dupont and Lahey2020; Tackett et al., Reference Tackett, Lahey, Van Hulle, Waldman, Krueger and Rathouz2013), this study is among the first to position these liabilities for psychopathology along these cognitive, temperament, and environmental continua simultaneously, providing novel insights into their interrelationships.

Table 4. Summary of results

The p factor is conceptualized as a broad liability to all forms of prevalent psychiatric symptomatology. Interestingly, the p factor's nomological network was similarly extensive across domains and included: a higher load of family history of psychopathology (consistent with a high heritability of p; Allegrini et al., Reference Allegrini, Cheesman, Rimfeld, Selzam, Pingault, Eley and Plomin2020; Selzam et al., Reference Selzam, Coleman, Caspi, Moffitt and Plomin2018); reduced family income; worse family, school, and neighborhood environment; reduced cognitive ability; and elevated emotional responses (including fear/distress emotions and impulsivity). In addition, p was associated with a broad array of KSADS diagnoses. Importantly, p's nomological network was established through instruments anchored in youth-, parent-, and teacher-report as well as task-based behavioral performance, suggesting rater-style alone cannot explain the breadth of p-associated risk variables. In contrast to p, nomological associations of the specific INT and EXT were generally more domain-specific. INT and EXT diverged from each other in demographic associations, with males and African-American youth having higher EXT, and females having higher INT. These distinct patterns of nomological associations for p, INT, and EXT raise additional questions about their ultimate drivers (e.g. environment, genes, environment × gene interactions) and perhaps identify targets for prevention efforts that can impact the emergence of broad psychopathology.

It is notable that the nomological networks of p, INT, and EXT were obscured when examining a priori Internalizing and Externalizing Problems scales, which demonstrated non-specific associations when not controlling for common variance due to p. That is, the dimensions derived from the bifactor structural model of psychopathology yielded improved convergent and divergent validity compared to the standard CBCL-based measures of Internalizing and Externalizing. For example, internalizing is broadly conceptualized in terms of elevated inhibition and threat sensitivity, while externalizing is conceptualized in terms of disinhibition and reduced sensitivity to threats (Krueger, McGue, & Iacono, Reference Krueger, McGue and Iacono2001; Nigg, Reference Nigg2017). Yet, CBCL Internalizing scores are positively associated with CD, while CBCL Externalizing scores were counterintuitively positively associated with multiple anxiety disorders. In contrast, the INT and EXT factors that control for the common variance of p exhibit the relationships predicted by prior theory (INT inversely associated with CD; EXT inversely associated anxiety disorders). More broadly, INT and EXT exhibit sharply differentiated, interpretable relationships with a host of variables (teacher-rated externalizing and attention problems, BAS-Drive, GN) that are, in contrast, not visible with the CBCL Internalizing and Externalizing Problems scales. Previous research has raised a concern about the ‘perils of partialing’ (Lynam, Hoyle, & Newman, Reference Lynam, Hoyle and Newman2006), wherein well-defined and interpretable scales can sometimes lose their original interpretation when portions of their variance are partialed away. Results from this study suggest that partialing via a bifactor model is not a ‘peril’ in this case. Instead, here partialing yields more interpretable INT, EXT, and p constructs, which exhibit more differentiated nomological networks that are better aligned with prior theory.

A striking finding in our results is that nomological associations of INT often diverged from those of the p factor. For example, p was related to higher attention problems, higher behavioral activation and impulsivity, and lower GN, while INT was associated with fewer attention problems, lower behavioral activation, and higher GN. This observation is noteworthy because the p factor represents a liability to all prevalent psychiatric symptoms, which includes internalizing spectrum symptoms. Put another way, our results show that two underlying factors contribute to observed fear and distress symptoms measured by the CBCL: a broad liability p factor and a narrow liability INT factor. But importantly, these two liabilities are embedded in highly differentiated, and in several cases, highly divergent, nomological networks. It is notable that the literature on anxiety and depression are similarly mixed, with some studies linking these internalizing disorders to variables associated with p (e.g. lower cognitive abilities; Levin et al., Reference Levin, Heller, Mohanty, Herrington and Miller2007; Rapport et al., Reference Rapport, Denney, Chung and Hustace2001; Snyder, Reference Snyder2013), while other studies link them to variables associated with INT (e.g. higher cognitive abilities; Karpinski, Kolb, Tetreault, and Borowski, Reference Karpinski, Kolb, Tetreault and Borowski2018; Penney et al., Reference Penney, Miedema and Mazmanian2015). Results of the current study serve to resolve some of these tensions by showing that observed internalizing symptoms likely reflect equifinality wherein distinct underlying liabilities (i.e. p-based liability v. INT-based liability) lead to similar clinical presentations (i.e. internalizing symptoms). However, future work replicating these findings to confirm their robustness and using person-centered analyses are needed to fully resolve these findings.

Some limitations of the current work warrant mention. First, our hierarchical model of psychopathology was derived from the parent-report CBCL. While studies show parent-report on youth psychopathology is valid at this age; obtaining reports from multiple informants on psychopathology has been found to improve associations with relevant outcomes (Clark, Durbin, Hicks, Iacono, & McGue, Reference Clark, Durbin, Hicks, Iacono and McGue2017). In future waves of ABCD data collection, the youth themselves will complete the CBCL which will strengthen the model estimates of p, INT, and EXT. Second, our study captures nomological networks among 9- and 10-year-olds. There is some evidence for the rank-order stability of the p factor and specific factors across adolescence (Castellanos-Ryan et al., Reference Castellanos-Ryan, Brière, O'Leary-Barrett, Banaschewski, Bokde, Bromberg and Gallinat2016; McElroy, Belsky, Carragher, Fearon, & Patalay, Reference McElroy, Belsky, Carragher, Fearon and Patalay2018; Snyder, Young, & Hankin, Reference Snyder, Young and Hankin2017), but follow-up investigations in future waves of ABCD data are needed to confirm the stability of the observed networks across time.

In sum, the current study demonstrates distinct, divergent, and interpretable patterns of nomological associations for p, INT, and EXT in pre-adolescents in the racially and economically diverse ABCD sample. These findings set the stage for future studies in the ABCD sample, leveraging longitudinal waves of data to trace the progression of psychopathology through adolescence, and leveraging multi-modal data to extend the nomological networks described here to encompass biological variables including genes and neurocircuitry.

Acknowledgements

Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study (https://abcdstudy.org), held in the NIMH Data Archive (NDA). This is a multisite, longitudinal study designed to recruit more than 10 000 children age 9–10 and follow them over 10 years into early adulthood. The ABCD Study is supported by the National Institutes of Health and additional federal partners under award numbers U01DA041022, U01DA041028, U01DA041048, U01DA041089, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041120, U01DA041134, U01DA041148, U01DA041156, U01DA041174, U24DA041123, and U24DA041147. A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/nih-collaborators. A listing of participating sites and a complete listing of the study investigators can be found at https://abcdstudy.org/principal-investigators.html. ABCD consortium investigators designed and implemented the study and/or provided data but did not necessarily participate in analysis or writing of this report. This manuscript reflects the views of the authors and may not reflect the opinions or views of the NIH or ABCD consortium investigators. The ABCD data repository grows and changes over time. The ABCD release used in this report came from NDA Study 721, 10.15154/1504041, which can be found at https://nda.nih.gov/study.html?id=721. The specific NDA study associated with the data reported in this manuscript is: NDA Study 981, 10.15154/1519364.This work was supported by the following grants from the US National Institutes of Health, the National Institute on Drug Abuse, and the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism: R01MH107741 (CS), U01DA041106 (CS, LH, MH, BH), T32 AA007477 (SB, DC). In addition, CS was supported by a grant from the Dana Foundation David Mahoney Neuroimaging Program.

Conflicts of interest

None.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720005103

Footnotes

The notes appear after the main text.

1 Parallel analyses were run on unweighted data and results were not appreciably different.

References

Achenbach, T. M. (1966). The classification of children's psychiatric symptoms: A factor-analytic study. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 80(7), 137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Achenbach, T. M., & Ruffle, T. M. (2000). The child behavior checklist and related forms for assessing behavioral/emotional problems and competencies. Pediatrics in Review, 21(8), 265271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allegrini, A. G., Cheesman, R., Rimfeld, K., Selzam, S., Pingault, J.-B., Eley, T. C., & Plomin, R. (2020). The p factor: Genetic analyses support a general dimension of psychopathology in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61(1), 3039.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alnæs, D., Kaufmann, T., Doan, N. T., Córdova-Palomera, A., Wang, Y., Bettella, F., … Westlye, L. T. (2018). Association of heritable cognitive ability and psychopathology with white matter properties in children and adolescents. JAMA Psychiatry, 75(3), 287295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angold, A., Costello, E. J., & Erkanli, A. (1999). Comorbidity. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40(1), 5787.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barch, D. M., Albaugh, M. D., Avenevoli, S., Chang, L., Clark, D. B., Glantz, M. D., … Sher, K. J. (2018). Demographic, physical and mental health assessments in the adolescent brain and cognitive development study: Rationale and description. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 5566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beauchaine, T. P., & McNulty, T. (2013). Comorbidities and continuities as ontogenic processes: Toward a developmental spectrum model of externalizing psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 25(42), 15051528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P., Shader, T. M., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2016). An ontogenic processes model of externalizing psychopathology. In Beauchaine, T. P., & Hinshaw, S. P. (Eds.), Oxford Library of psychology. The Oxford handbook of externalizing spectrum disorders (pp. 485501). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Blanco, C., Wall, M. M., He, J.-P., Krueger, R. F., Olfson, M., Jin, C. J., … Merikangas, K. R. (2015). The space of common psychiatric disorders in adolescents: Comorbidity structure and individual latent liabilities. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 54(1), 4552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloemen, A., Oldehinkel, A. J., Laceulle, O. M., Ormel, J., Rommelse, N. N. J., & Hartman, C. A. (2018). The association between executive functioning and psychopathology: General or specific? Psychological Medicine, 48(11), 17871794.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brandes, C. M., Herzhoff, K., Smack, A. J., & Tackett, J. L. (2019). The p factor and the n factor: Associations between the general factors of psychopathology and neuroticism in children. Clinical Psychological Science, 7(6), 12661284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carragher, N., Teesson, M., Sunderland, M., Newton, N. C., Krueger, R. F., Conrod, P. J., … Slade, T. (2016). The structure of adolescent psychopathology: A symptom-level analysis. Psychological Medicine, 46(5), 981994.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carver, C. S., Johnson, S. L., & Timpano, K. R. (2017). Toward a functional view of the p factor in psychopathology. Clinical Psychological Science, 5(5), 880889.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casey, B. J., Oliveri, M. E., & Insel, T. (2014). A neurodevelopmental perspective on the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) framework. Biological Psychiatry, 76(5), 350353. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.01.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., Houts, R. M., Belsky, D. W., Goldman-Mellor, S. J., Harrington, H., Israel, S., … Poulton, R. (2014). The p factor: One general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clinical Psychological Science, 2(2), 119137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caspi, A., Houts, R. M., Belsky, D. W., Harrington, H., Hogan, S., Ramrakha, S., … Moffitt, T. E. (2017). Childhood forecasting of a small segment of the population with large economic burden. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(1), 110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2018). All for one and one for all: Mental disorders in one dimension. American Journal of Psychiatry, 175(9), 831844.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castellanos-Ryan, N., Brière, F. N., O'Leary-Barrett, M., Banaschewski, T., Bokde, A., Bromberg, U., … Gallinat, J. (2016). The structure of psychopathology in adolescence and its common personality and cognitive correlates. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125(8), 1039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, D. A., Durbin, C. E., Hicks, B. M., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. (2017). Personality in the age of industry: Structure, heritability, and correlates of personality in middle childhood from the perspective of parents, teachers, and children. Journal of Research in Personality, 67, 132143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, D. A, Hicks, B. M., Angstadt, M., Rutherford, S., Taxali, A., Hyde, L. W., … Sripada, C. (2020). The General Factor of Psychopathology in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study: A Comparison of Alternative Modeling Approaches. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/b6uy7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Copeland, W. E., Shanahan, L., Costello, E. J., & Angold, A. (2009). Childhood and adolescent psychiatric disorders as predictors of young adult disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66(7), 764772.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Copeland, W. E., Wolke, D., Shanahan, L., & Costello, E. J. (2015). Adult functional outcomes of common childhood psychiatric problems: A prospective, longitudinal study. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(9), 892899.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cronbach, L. J., & Meehl, P. E. (1955). Construct validity in psychological tests. Psychological Bulletin, 52(4), 281302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crone, E. A., & Dahl, R. E. (2012). Understanding adolescence as a period of social–affective engagement and goal flexibility. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13(9), 636650.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuthbert, B. N. (2014). The RDoC framework: Facilitating transition from ICD/DSM to dimensional approaches that integrate neuroscience and psychopathology. World Psychiatry: Official Journal of the World Psychiatric Association (WPA), 13(1), 2835. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20087.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deutz, M. H., Geeraerts, S. B., Belsky, J., Deković, M., van Baar, A. L., Prinzie, P., & Patalay, P. (2020). General psychopathology and dysregulation profile in a longitudinal community sample: Stability, antecedents and outcomes. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 51(1), 114126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliott, M. L., Romer, A., Knodt, A. R., & Hariri, A. R. (2018). A connectome-wide functional signature of transdiagnostic risk for mental illness. Biological Psychiatry, 84(6), 452459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erskine, H. E., Ferrari, A. J., Polanczyk, G. V., Moffitt, T. E., Murray, C. J., Vos, T., … Scott, J. G. (2014). The global burden of conduct disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in 2010. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(4), 328336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garavan, H., Bartsch, H., Conway, K., Decastro, A., Goldstein, R. Z., Heeringa, S., … Zahs, D. (2018). Recruiting the ABCD sample: Design considerations and procedures. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 1622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(11), 13371345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hankin, B. L., Davis, E. P., Snyder, H., Young, J. F., Glynn, L. M., & Sandman, C. A. (2017). Temperament factors and dimensional, latent bifactor models of child psychopathology: Transdiagnostic and specific associations in two youth samples. Psychiatry Research, 252, 139146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heeringa, S. G., & Berglund, P. A. (2020). A guide for population-based analysis of the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study baseline data. BioRxiv. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.02.10.942011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hinshaw, S. P. (2002). Process, mechanism, and explanation related to externalizing behavior in developmental psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30(5), 431446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karpinski, R. I., Kolb, A. M. K., Tetreault, N. A., & Borowski, T. B. (2018). High intelligence: A risk factor for psychological and physiological overexcitabilities. Intelligence, 66, 823.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(6), 593602.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kobak, K. A., Kratochvil, C., Stanger, C., & Kaufman, J. (2013). Computerized screening of comorbidity in adolescents with substance or psychiatric disorders. Anxiety Disorders and Depression. (La Jolaa, CA).Google Scholar
Kotov, R., Krueger, R. F., Watson, D., Achenbach, T. M., Althoff, R. R., Bagby, R. M., … Clark, L. A. (2017). The hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology (HiTOP): A dimensional alternative to traditional nosologies. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126(4), 454477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krueger, R. F. (1999). The structure of common mental disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56(10), 921926.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krueger, R. F., & Markon, K. E. (2006). Reinterpreting comorbidity: A model-based approach to understanding and classifying psychopathology. Annual Review of Child Clinical Psychology, 2, 111133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krueger, R. F., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (2001). The higher-order structure of common DSM mental disorders: Internalization, externalization, and their connections to personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 30(7), 12451259. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00106-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laceulle, O. M., Vollebergh, W. A., & Ormel, J. (2015). The structure of psychopathology in adolescence: Replication of a general psychopathology factor in the TRAILS study. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(6), 850860.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lahey, B. B., Applegate, B., Hakes, J. K., Zald, D. H., Hariri, A. R., & Rathouz, P. J. (2012). Is there a general factor of prevalent psychopathology during adulthood? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(4), 971.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lahey, B. B., Rathouz, P. J., Keenan, K., Stepp, S. D., Loeber, R., & Hipwell, A. E. (2015). Criterion validity of the general factor of psychopathology in a prospective study of girls. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(4), 415422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lahey, B. B., Rathouz, P. J., Van Hulle, C., Urbano, R. C., Krueger, R. F., Applegate, B., … Waldman, I. D. (2008). Testing structural models of DSM-IV symptoms of common forms of child and adolescent psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36(2), 187206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lahey, B. B., Van Hulle, C. A., Singh, A. L., Waldman, I. D., & Rathouz, P. J. (2011). Higher-order genetic and environmental structure of prevalent forms of child and adolescent psychopathology. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68(2), 181189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levin, R. L., Heller, W., Mohanty, A., Herrington, J. D., & Miller, G. A. (2007). Cognitive deficits in depression and functional specificity of regional brain activity. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 31(2), 211233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewinsohn, P. M., Shankman, S. A., Gau, J. M., & Klein, D. N. (2004). The prevalence and co-morbidity of subthreshold psychiatric conditions. Psychological Medicine, 34(4), 613622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loewy, R. L., Bearden, C. E., Johnson, J. K., Raine, A., & Cannon, T. D. (2005). The prodromal questionnaire (PQ): Preliminary validation of a self-report screening measure for prodromal and psychotic syndromes. Schizophrenia Research, 79(1), 117125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lynam, D. R., Hoyle, R. H., & Newman, J. P. (2006). The perils of partialling: Cautionary tales from aggression and psychopathy. Assessment, 13(3), 328341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacCallum, R. C. (2009). Factor analysis. In The SAGE handbook of quantitative methods in psychology (pp. 124147). London: SAGE Publications Ltd. https://doi.org/10.4135/9780857020994.Google Scholar
Martel, M. M., Pan, P. M., Hoffmann, M. S., Gadelha, A., do Rosário, M. C., Mari, J. J., … Salum, G. A. (2017). A general psychopathology factor (p factor) in children: Structural model analysis and external validation through familial risk and child global executive function. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126(1), 137148. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S., Roisman, G. I., Long, J. D., Burt, K. B., Obradović, J., Riley, J. R., … Tellegen, A. (2005). Developmental cascades: Linking academic achievement and externalizing and internalizing symptoms over 20 years. Developmental Psychology, 41(5), 733746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McElroy, E., Belsky, J., Carragher, N., Fearon, P., & Patalay, P. (2018). Developmental stability of general and specific factors of psychopathology from early childhood to adolescence: Dynamic mutualism or p-differentiation? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 59(6), 667675.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michelini, G., Barch, D. M., Tian, Y., Watson, D., Klein, D. N., & Kotov, R. (2019). Delineating and validating higher-order dimensions of psychopathology in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study. Translational Psychiatry, 9(1), 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milne, B. J., Caspi, A., Harrington, H., Poulton, R., Rutter, M., & Moffitt, T. E. (2009). Predictive value of family history on severity of illness: The case for depression, anxiety, alcohol dependence, and drug dependence. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66(7), 738747.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milne, B., Moffitt, T., Crump, R., Poulton, R., Rutter, M., Sears, M., … Caspi, A. (2008). How should we construct psychiatric family history scores? A comparison of alternative approaches from the Dunedin family health history study. Psychological Medicine, 38(12), 17931802.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, T. M., Kaczkurkin, A. N., Durham, E. L., Jeong, H. J., McDowell, M. G., Dupont, R. M., … Lahey, B. B. (2020). Criterion Validity and Relationships between Alternative Hierarchical Dimensional Models of General and Specific Psychopathology. BioRxiv, Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.27.064303.Google ScholarPubMed
Murray, A. L., Eisner, M., & Ribeaud, D. (2016). The development of the general factor of psychopathology ‘p factor’ through childhood and adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44(8), 15731586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neumann, A., Pappa, I., Lahey, B. B., Verhulst, F. C., Medina-Gomez, C., Jaddoe, V. W., … Tiemeier, H. (2016). Single nucleotide polymorphism heritability of a general psychopathology factor in children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 55(12), 10381045.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nigg, J. T. (2017). Annual research review: On the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(4), 361383. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12675.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noordhof, A., Krueger, R. F., Ormel, J., Oldehinkel, A. J., & Hartman, C. A. (2015). Integrating autism-related symptoms into the dimensional internalizing and externalizing model of psychopathology. The TRAILS study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43(3), 577587.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pagliaccio, D., Luking, K. R., Anokhin, A. P., Gotlib, I. H., Hayden, E. P., Olino, T. M., … Barch, D. M. (2016). Revising the BIS/BAS scale to study development: Measurement invariance and normative effects of age and sex from childhood through adulthood. Psychological Assessment, 28(4), 429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patalay, P., Fonagy, P., Deighton, J., Belsky, J., Vostanis, P., & Wolpert, M. (2015). A general psychopathology factor in early adolescence. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 207(1), 1522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penney, A. M., Miedema, V. C., & Mazmanian, D. (2015). Intelligence and emotional disorders: Is the worrying and ruminating mind a more intelligent mind? Personality and Individual Differences, 74, 9093.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pettersson, E., Lahey, B. B., Larsson, H., & Lichtenstein, P. (2018). Criterion validity and utility of the general factor of psychopathology in childhood: Predictive associations with independently measured severe adverse mental health outcomes in adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 57(6), 372383.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pettersson, E., Lahey, B., Larsson, H., Lundstroem, S., & Lichtenstein, P. (2015). The general factor of psychopathology in childhood and maladaptive outcomes in young adulthood: A Swedish population twin study. Behavior Genetics, 45, 679679.Google Scholar
Piper, B. J., Gray, H. M., Raber, J., & Birkett, M. A. (2014). Reliability and validity of brief problem monitor, an abbreviated form of the child behavior checklist. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 68(10), 759767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapport, M. D., Denney, C. B., Chung, K.-M., & Hustace, K. (2001). Internalizing behavior problems and scholastic achievement in children: Cognitive and behavioral pathways as mediators of outcome. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 30(4), 536551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rucci, P., Gherardi, S., Tansella, M., Piccinelli, M., Berardi, D., Bisoffi, G., … Pini, S. (2003). Subthreshold psychiatric disorders in primary care: Prevalence and associated characteristics. Journal of Affective Disorders, 76(1–3), 171181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Selzam, S., Coleman, J. R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., & Plomin, R. (2018). A polygenic p factor for major psychiatric disorders. Translational Psychiatry, 8(1), 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smetana, J. G., Campione-Barr, N., & Metzger, A. (2006). Adolescent development in interpersonal and societal contexts. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 255284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, G. T., Atkinson, E. A., Davis, H. A., Riley, E. N., & Oltmanns, J. R. (2020). The general factor of psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 16, 7598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snyder, H. R. (2013). Major depressive disorder is associated with broad impairments on neuropsychological measures of executive function: A meta-analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin, 139(1), 81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snyder, H. R., Friedman, N. P., & Hankin, B. L. (2019a). Transdiagnostic mechanisms of psychopathology in youth: Executive functions, dependent stress, and rumination. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 43(5), 834851.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snyder, H. R., Young, J. F., & Hankin, B. L. (2017). Strong homotypic continuity in common psychopathology-, internalizing-, and externalizing-specific factors over time in adolescents. Clinical Psychological Science, 5(1), 98110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snyder, H. R., Young, J. F., & Hankin, B. L. (2019b). Chronic stress exposure and generation are related to the p-factor and externalizing specific psychopathology in youth. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 48(2), 306315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sripada, C., Taxali, A., Angstadt, M., & Rutherford, S. (2020) Boost in Test-Retest Reliability in Resting State fMRI with Predictive Modeling. Biorxiv, Retrieved from https://doi.org/10/1101/796714.Google Scholar
Stochl, J., Khandaker, G. M., Lewis, G., Perez, J., Goodyer, I. M., Zammit, S., … Jones, P. B. (2015). Mood, anxiety and psychotic phenomena measure a common psychopathological factor. Psychological Medicine, 45(7), 14831493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tackett, J. L., Lahey, B. B., Van Hulle, C., Waldman, I., Krueger, R. F., & Rathouz, P. J. (2013). Common genetic influences on negative emotionality and a general psychopathology factor in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122(4), 11421153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Volkow, N. D., Koob, G. F., Croyle, R. T., Bianchi, D. W., Gordon, J. A., Koroshetz, W. J., … Conway, K. (2018). The conception of the ABCD study: From substance use to a broad NIH collaboration. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waldman, I. D., Poore, H. E., van Hulle, C., Rathouz, P. J., & Lahey, B. B. (2016). External validity of a hierarchical dimensional model of child and adolescent psychopathology: Tests using confirmatory factor analyses and multivariate behavior genetic analyses. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125(8), 10531066.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
West, S. G., Taylor, A. B., & Wu, W. (2012). Model fit and model selection in structural equation modeling. In Handbook of structural equation modeling (pp. 209231). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Wright, A. G., & Simms, L. J. (2015). A metastructural model of mental disorders and pathological personality traits. Psychological Medicine, 45(11), 23092319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zucker, R. A., Gonzalez, R., Ewing, S. W. F., Paulus, M. P., Arroyo, J., Fuligni, A., … Wills, T. (2018). Assessment of culture and environment in the adolescent brain and cognitive development study: Rationale, description of measures, and early data. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 107120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Visualization of associations between p, INT, and EXT and independent variables. (a) Standardized beta weights with demographic variables; (b) Standardized beta weights with KSAD Diagnoses.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Visualization of associations between p, INT, and EXT and independent variables. (a) Standardized beta weights with teacher-rated psychopathology (BPM), trait measures (BIS/BAS and UPPS), and general neurocognition (GN); (b) Significant associations with school environment (School Risk and Protective Factors), Prosocial Behavior (youth and parent report), Family Conflict (youth and parent report), and Parental Monitoring (youth report).

Figure 2

Table 1. Associations between demographic variables and p, Internalizing (INT), and Externalizing (EXT) factor scores

Figure 3

Table 2. Associations with psychopathology, trait measures, and neurocognition

Figure 4

Table 3. Associations with social environment measures

Figure 5

Table 4. Summary of results

Supplementary material: File

Brislin et al. supplementary material

Brislin et al. supplementary material

Download Brislin et al. supplementary material(File)
File 15.8 KB