Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T02:40:48.176Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Predictors of outcome following treatment for chronic fatigue

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Lucy Darbishire*
Affiliation:
Guy's, King's and St Thomas' School of Medicine, London, UK
Paul Seed
Affiliation:
Guy's, King's and St Thomas' School of Medicine, London, UK
Leone Ridsdale
Affiliation:
Guy's, King's and St Thomas' School of Medicine, London, UK
*
Lucy Darbishire, Department of General Practice and Primary Care, Guy's, King's and St Thomas' School of Medicine, 5 Lambeth Walk, London, SE11 6SP, UK. E-mail: lucy.clark@kcl.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

We explored the role of baseline characteristics of 105 patients who presented with fatigue in primary care in determining outcome following either graded exercise or cognitive–behavioural therapy. Meeting the criteria for chronic fatigue syndrome was the most powerful predictor of poor outcome and this negative effect was enhanced by greater functional impairment or greater perceived negative consequences, but was not further enhanced by both.

Type
Short Reports
Copyright
Copyright © 2005 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

We investigated predictors of outcome in a previously reported (Reference McCrone, Ridsdale and DarbishireMcCrone et al, 2004; Reference Ridsdale, Darbishire and SeedRidsdale et al, 2004) group of patients who presented to general practitioners (GPs) with fatigue. We hypothesised that fatigue score following therapeutic intervention with either cognitive–behavioural therapy (CBT) or graded exercise would be predicted by baseline fatigue severity (including chronic fatigue syndrome status and functional impairment), psychological morbidity and illness beliefs.

METHOD

The group was drawn from patients recruited to a multicentre randomised trial comparing CBT with graded exercise for patients with chronic fatigue in primary care (Reference Ridsdale, Darbishire and SeedRidsdale et al, 2004). Inclusion criteria were age 16–75 years; fatigue as a main or important problem lasting for 3 months or more; and a score of at least 4 on the fatigue questionnaire–bimodal scoring (Reference Chalder, Berelowitz and PawlikowskaChalder et al, 1993). Further inclusion and exclusion criteria are detailed in Reference Ridsdale, Darbishire and SeedRidsdale et al, 2004. Of the 123 patients included, 60 were randomised to graded exercise therapy and 63 to CBT. This report describes the 105 patients (85%) who remained in the study at the 8-month follow-up.

Measures

Fatigue was measured with a Likert-scored scale (Reference Chalder, Berelowitz and PawlikowskaChalder et al, 1993). Chronic fatigue syndrome status was determined using criteria defined by Fukuda et al (Reference Fukuda, Straus and Hickie1994): fatigue with a definite onset, of a minimum duration of 6 months, with substantial functional impairment, and four or more additional symptoms from a list of eight. Additional measures included scores on the Hospital Anxiety and Depression scale (Reference Zigmond and SnaithZigmond & Snaith, 1983); degree of functional impairment scored on the Work and Social Adjustment Scale (WSAS; Reference MarksMarks, 1986); illness beliefs, including perceived negative consequences (Reference Weinman, Petrie and Moss-MorrisWeinman et al, 1996); and illness attributions (physical/psychological) (Reference Powell, Dolan and WesselyPowell et al, 1990). For more information on the measures used, see Ridsdale et al (Reference Ridsdale, Darbishire and Seed2004). Patients performed a step test at baseline assessment (stepping on to a bench for 1 min). Patients were also asked to report whether they had previously consulted a doctor for an emotional problem, had been referred to a psychiatrist, or were members of a myalgic encephalomyelitis (ME) support group. Information on prior psychological diagnoses and consultation frequency was extracted from general practice records.

Analysis

Predictors of fatigue outcome were investigated using linear regression analysis. Univariate regression coefficients were calculated for each of the 15 independent variables (10 continuous, 4 binary and 1 categorical). The continuous variables are shown in Table 1. The binary variables were meeting criteria for chronic fatigue syndrome; past history of anxiety or depression; membership of an ME support group; and type of therapy. The categorical variable was duration of fatigue (five ordered categories). Independent variables reaching a significance level of P≤0.05 were entered into a multiple regression model. The results of the regression analyses are shown as the mean increase in final fatigue associated with having v. not having the exposure (binary variables) or with each additional scale point (continuous variables).

Table 1 Initial scores on continuous predictor variables and their correlation with final fatigue

Predictor variable Baseline score Mean (s.d.) Correlation P
Initial fatigue score (range 0-33) 25.17 (5.09) 0.280 0.002
Number of other symptoms (range 0-12) 9.44 (1.99) 0.191 0.026
Functional impairment (WSAS) (range 0-32) 19.28 (8.14) 0.377 <0.001
Number of steps performed (range 10-50) 26.87 (6.82) -0.196 0.023
Depression score (HAD) (range 0-21, score > 10 indicates depression) 8.27 (3.61) 0.265 0.003
Anxiety score (HAD) (range 0-21, score > 10 indicates anxiety) 10.50 (4.53) 0.055 0.288
Belief in a psychological cause of illness (range 1-5; 1 physical, 5 psychological) 2.50 (1.00) -0.147 0.068
Perceived negative consequences (range 1-5; 1 fewer consequences, 5 more consequences) 3.53 (0.67) 0.326 <0.001
Perceived control over fatigue (range 1-5; 1 less control, 5 more control) 3.56 (0.50) -0.137 0.082
Perception of exertion required to perform step test (range 6-20; 7 very very light, 19 very very hard) 14.55 (2.64) 0.061 0.268

RESULTS

The participants’ scores are shown in Table 1. Thirty-one per cent (n=33) met chronic fatigue syndrome criteria, 5% (n=5) were members of an ME support group, and 60% (n=63) had a history of psychological diagnosis. The dependent variable, final fatigue score, was normally distributed (mean 15.03, skewness 0.137), with a significant improvement following therapy of 10.14 (s.d.=8.69) points (95% CI 8.46–11.82, skewness 0.209).

When entered in separate univariate regression analyses, seven of the variables were associated with a higher final fatigue score: greater initial fatigue (b=0.44; 95% CI 0.16–0.79); meeting chronic fatigue syndrome criteria (b=7.74; 95% CI 4.46–11.01); greater functional impairment (b=0.40; 95% CI 0.21–0.59); fewer steps performed (b=-0.25; 95% CI -0.49 to -0.01); higher depression score (b=0.63; 95% CI 0.18–1.08); greater perceived negative consequences (b=4.15; 95% CI 1.80–6.51); and membership of ME support group (b=8.79; 95% CI 1.12–16.46).

In a second model with these seven variables, only chronic fatigue syndrome status (b=4.50; s.e.=2.02; 95% CI 0.49–8.51) contributed significantly (model R 2 0.24, adjusted R 2=0.19, F=4.36; P<0.001). Following this, six bivariate analyses that each included chronic fatigue syndrome status and one of the other six variables showed that only functional impairment (R 2=0.22, adjusted R 2=0.20) and greater perceived negative consequences (R 2=0.21, adjusted R 2=0.19) added significantly to the model when entered alongside chronic fatigue syndrome status. When all three variables were entered simultaneously into the model, it was not enhanced by a greater degree than by adding functional impairment or greater perceived negative consequences alone (R 2=0.22, adjusted R 2=0.20).

DISCUSSION

We expected to find that fatigue severity, illness beliefs, psychological state and physical fitness would affect outcome. In fact, chronic fatigue syndrome status, a known marker of fatigue severity, was the most robust predictor of final fatigue following therapy. Only three variables explained more than 10% of the variance in final fatigue when considered alone: baseline chronic fatigue syndrome status (18%), functional impairment (14%) and perceived negative consequences (11%). Relationships between recovery and each of these variables have been found previously for patients with fatigue in primary and secondary care (Reference Bentall, Powell and NyeBentall et al, 2002; Reference Chalder, Godfrey and RidsdaleChalder et al, 2003). Membership of an ME support group might also be important and has been reported as being so in previous studies (Reference Bentall, Powell and NyeBentall et al, 2002), but the size of the subgroup in our study provided insufficient power to support a relationship.

The results suggest that, individually, functional impairment and greater perceived negative consequences add to the power of chronic fatigue syndrome status to predict final fatigue, but add no more power when combined. This is partly explained by the relatively high correlation observed between them, which at 0.690 is larger than that between any of the other variables (0.656 between functional impairment and baseline fatigue; 0.419 between chronic fatigue syndrome status and final fatigue). Chalder et al (Reference Chalder, Godfrey and Ridsdale2003) also found the latter two variables of predictive value, but did not find that chronic fatigue syndrome status was associated with a poor outcome. The data presented here are the first to suggest that meeting criteria for this syndrome is likely to predict a poor outcome following treatment in primary care.

Levels of fatigue can fluctuate considerably between visits; our study deals with the information that would be available to a GP at a single consultation. Any of the measures used in predicting fatigue could have been repeated at subsequent visits, and any predictive power would have been increased.

Patients with chronic fatigue syndrome are likely to have a poorer prognosis and may require a greater amount or a different type of therapy. In view of this, we believe that it will be useful for GPs to know and apply the criteria for the syndrome when they assess patients with fatigue in primary care. It should help them advise on prognosis and management.

Acknowledgements

We thank the patients, GPs and their teams, therapists and many colleagues. who provided feedback at different stages, including Professors Peter White. and Roger Jones. The study was supported by a grant from the Linbury. Trust.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None Funding detailed in Acknowledgements.

References

Bentall, R. P., Powell, P., Nye, F. J., et al (2002) Predictors of response to treatment for chronic fatigue syndrome. British Journal of Psychiatry, 181, 248252.Google Scholar
Chalder, T., Berelowitz, G., Pawlikowska, T., et al (1993) Development of a fatigue scale. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 37, 147153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chalder, T., Godfrey, E., Ridsdale, L., et al (2003) Predictors of outcome in a fatigued population in primary care following a randomized controlled trial. Psychological Medicine, 33, 283287.Google Scholar
Deale, A., Chalder, T., Marks, I., et al (1997) Cognitive behavior therapy for chronic fatigue syndrome: a randomized controlled trial. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 408414.Google Scholar
Fukuda, K., Straus, S. E., Hickie, I., et al (1994) The chronic fatigue syndrome: a comprehensive approach to its definition and study. International Chronic Fatigue Syndrome Study Group. Annals of Internal Medicine, 121, 953959.Google Scholar
Marks, I. (1986) Behavioural Psychotherapy: Maudsley Book of Clinical Management. Bristol: Wright.Google Scholar
McCrone, P., Ridsdale, L., Darbishire, L., et al (2004) Cost-effectiveness of cognitive behavioural therapy, graded exercise and usual care for patients with chronic fatigue in primary care. Psychological Medicine, 34, 991999.Google Scholar
Powell, R., Dolan, R. & Wessely, S. (1990) Attributions and self-esteem in depression and chronic fatigue syndromes. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 34, 665673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ridsdale, L., Godfrey, E., Chalder, T., et al (2001) Chronic fatigue in general practice: is counselling as good as cognitive behaviour therapy? AUK randomised trial. British Journal of General Practice, 51, 1924.Google Scholar
Ridsdale, L., Darbishire, L. & Seed, P. (2004) Is graded exercise better than cognitive behaviour therapy for fatigue? AUK randomized trial in primary care. Psychological Medicine, 34, 3749.Google Scholar
Weinman, J., Petrie, K. J., Moss-Morris, R., et al (1996) The Illness Perception Questionnaire: a new method for assessing the cognitive representations of illness. Psychology and Health, 11, 114129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zigmond, A. S. & Snaith, R. P. (1983) The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 67, 361370.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Initial scores on continuous predictor variables and their correlation with final fatigue

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.