Introduction
The period spanning childhood, adolescence, and young adulthood is crucial since the intrinsic architecture of the brain is shaped by a wide repertoire of factors, including those that are strongly dependent on the caregiver (e.g. parents, family, guardian), experiences within educational and community settings, as well as overarching societal and cultural norms. A plethora of studies have suggested psychiatric disorders to be linked to the experiences encountered during neurodevelopment (Kim-Cohen et al., Reference Kim-Cohen, Caspi, Moffitt, Harrington, Milne and Poulton2003; Rothbart, Reference Rothbart2011; Shevlin, McElroy, & Murphy, Reference Shevlin, McElroy and Murphy2017). There are several factors like unstable caregiving, socioeconomic situations, social disparity, stress, and frequent exposure to adversities that can lead to maladapted development (Gee, Reference Gee2021; Holz et al., Reference Holz, Berhe, Sacu, Schwarz, Tesarz, Heim and Tost2022; McLaughlin, Weissman, & Bitrán, Reference McLaughlin, Weissman and Bitrán2019; Rakesh & Whittle, Reference Rakesh and Whittle2021; Rebello, Moura, Pinaya, Rohde, & Sato, Reference Rebello, Moura, Pinaya, Rohde and Sato2018). During the developmental phase, synaptic pruning and white matter myelination play an important role in configuring the neural architecture (McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Weissman and Bitrán2019). These processes adapt the neural system such that the system is maximally efficient for the environment in which it is developed. These processes are required for the healthy development of the brain, and a faultily programmed process can alter the functionality and lead to neurodevelopmental disorders (Cardozo et al., Reference Cardozo, de Lima, Maciel, Silva, Dobransky and Ribeiro2019; Feinberg, Reference Feinberg1982; Germann, Brederoo, & Sommer, Reference Germann, Brederoo and Sommer2021). Studies attempt to understand these processes from a cohort of categorized healthy and diseased populations. This distinction (healthy vs. diseased) obscures the state of transition from healthy to disease, and the understanding of the influences that tip the balance from typical to atypical development remains limited. When population-based big neurodevelopmental data is collected without any preconceived distribution of subjects, it can be expected that brain signatures of both typical and atypical population are present in the data in the latent form. Using advance analysis techniques, these patterns can be extracted and the factors that lead to atypicality can be comprehended. In this work, we attempted to understand the point of bifurcation in the neurodevelopmental trajectory, by categorizing the neural pattern of typical and atypical development coexisting within the cohort of healthy individuals. We eventually investigated the factors that have maximal influence in altering the typical pattern of neurodevelopment, and can drive the normal pattern of development towards atypicality. At the same time, we also explored how the factor shapes the brain organization in both typical and atypical populations, and if there was any change in the pattern over the course of development (period spanning childhood, adolescence, and young adulthood).
In this aspect, resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rsfMRI) has the potential to reveal underlying neural organization (Biswal, Yetkin, Haughton, & Hyde, Reference Biswal, Yetkin, Haughton and Hyde1995; Buckner, Krienen, & Yeo, Reference Buckner, Krienen and Yeo2013; Fox & Raichle, Reference Fox and Raichle2007) and provide information regarding brain functions and cognitive abilities (Finn et al., Reference Finn, Shen, Scheinost, Rosenberg, Huang, Chun and Constable2015; Kashyap, Bhattacharjee, Yeo, & Chen, Reference Kashyap, Bhattacharjee, Yeo and Chen2019a; Kashyap et al., Reference Kashyap, Kong, Bhattacharjee, Li, Zhou and Yeo2019b, Reference Kashyap, Eng, Bhattacharjee, Gupta, Ho, Ho and Chen2021; Kong et al., Reference Kong, Li, Orban, Sabuncu, Liu, Schaefer and Eickhoff2018; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Nichols, Vidaurre, Winkler, Behrens, Glasser and Miller2015). The information latent in the rsfMRI brain signatures can reflect our lifestyle habits, mental health conditions, and the environment we live in (Finn et al., Reference Finn, Shen, Scheinost, Rosenberg, Huang, Chun and Constable2015; Ikeda, Kawano, Watanabe, Yamashita, & Kawahara, Reference Ikeda, Kawano, Watanabe, Yamashita and Kawahara2022; Kashyap et al., Reference Kashyap, Bhattacharjee, Yeo and Chen2019a, Reference Kashyap, Kong, Bhattacharjee, Li, Zhou and Yeo2019b, Reference Kashyap, Eng, Bhattacharjee, Gupta, Ho, Ho and Chen2021; Lake et al., Reference Lake, Finn, Noble, Vanderwal, Shen, Rosenberg and Constable2019; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Nichols, Vidaurre, Winkler, Behrens, Glasser and Miller2015). Strategies to identify subtypes from rsfMRI by dissecting heterogeneity in the rsfMRI pattern from a large cohort and then associating the subtypes with behaviors have gained momentum (Drysdale et al., Reference Drysdale, Grosenick, Downar, Dunlop, Mansouri, Meng and Etkin2017; Feczko & Fair, Reference Feczko and Fair2020; Mattoni, Smith, & Olino, Reference Mattoni, Smith and Olino2023). Such brain-behavior associations with subtype identification have enabled the identification of the factors that have a significant effect on mental health (Dias et al., Reference Dias, Iyer, Carpenter, Cary, Wilson, Mitchell and Fair2015; Drysdale et al., Reference Drysdale, Grosenick, Downar, Dunlop, Mansouri, Meng and Etkin2017; Feczko & Fair, Reference Feczko and Fair2020; Mattoni et al., Reference Mattoni, Smith and Olino2023; Zhu et al., Reference Zhu, Becquey, Chen, Lejuez, Li and Bi2022). For example, in a dataset of healthy subjects (n = 500) from the human connectome project (HCP, age range of 36–100 years), Smith et al. (Reference Smith, Nichols, Vidaurre, Winkler, Behrens, Glasser and Miller2015) identified a positive and negative mode of variation in rsfMRI that was associated with positive (e.g. high performance on memory and cognitive test, life satisfaction) and negative (like substance use, anger, and rule-breaking behavior) spectrum of behaviors. In our previous work (Kashyap et al., Reference Kashyap, Bhattacharjee, Yeo and Chen2019a), we investigated rsfMRI subtypes from 788 HCP subjects to identify behaviors that maximize the rsfMRI variance. We found that the deviations in rsfMRI pattern were associated with higher usage of marijuana, illicit drugs, alcohol, tobacco, and predisposition toward antisocial personality. These findings have enriched our understanding of the factors that can bifurcate the mental health trajectory in healthy adults. However, approximately 63% of mental illnesses begin prior to age 25, and 37% of them start before the age of 14 (McGrath et al., Reference McGrath, Al-Hamzawi, Alonso, Altwaijri, Andrade, Bromet and Zaslavsky2023; Solmi et al., Reference Solmi, Radua, Olivola, Croce, Soardo, Salazar de Pablo and Fusar-Poli2022). These statistics clearly indicate that there is a need to understand the factors that dichotomize the neurodevelopmental trajectory and contribute to atypical brain development. In this aspect, leveraging the potential of previous computational techniques (applied to aging datasets) on large neurodevelopmental datasets is useful, and several studies have applied similar strategies to associate variations in the pattern of rsfMRI with behavior (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Tam, Kebets, Orban, Ooi, Asplund and Yeo2022; Evans et al., Reference Evans, Kochalka, Ngoon, Wu, Qin, Battista and Menon2015; Kebets et al., Reference Kebets, Piguet, Chen, Ooi, Kirschner, Siffredi and Bernhardt2023; Lake et al., Reference Lake, Finn, Noble, Vanderwal, Shen, Rosenberg and Constable2019; Qu et al., Reference Qu, Chen, Tam, Ooi, Dhamala, Cocuzza and Holmes2023; Sripada et al., Reference Sripada, Rutherford, Angstadt, Thompson, Luciana, Weigard and Heitzeg2020; Uddin et al., Reference Uddin, Supekar, Lynch, Khouzam, Phillips, Feinstein and Menon2013). The subtle advantage of extending such techniques to early phases of development is also to map a continuum of mental health across the lifespan.
In this exploratory study, we have used our previous hypothesis-free approach (Kashyap et al., Reference Kashyap, Bhattacharjee, Yeo and Chen2019a) to investigate factors that have maximal influence on variation of rsfMRI during neurodevelopment. We used rsfMRI of 987 healthy subjects, within specified age bands – children (6–11 years), adolescents (12–17 years), and young adults (18–23 years), from the Consortium on Vulnerability to Externalizing Disorders and Addictions (c-VEDA): an accelerated longitudinal cohort of children and adolescents in India (Fernandes et al., Reference Fernandes, Spiers, Vaidya, Zhang, Sharma, Holla and Chakrabarti2021; Holla et al., Reference Holla, Taylor, Glen, Lee, Vaidya, Mehta and Rao2020; Sharma et al., Reference Sharma, Ravi, Kumar, Thennarasu, Heron, Hickman and Mehta2023, Reference Sharma, Vaidya, Iyengar, Zhang, Holla, Purushottam and Hickman2020; Vaidya et al., Reference Vaidya, Holla, Heron, Sharma, Zhang, Fernandes and Das2023; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Vaidya, Iyengar, Sharma, Holla, Ahuja and Chakrabarti2020). We estimated features from rsfMRIs, and classified subjects using a dissimilarity maximization algorithm that is based on the similarity/dissimilarity of their rsfMRI pattern (Kashyap et al., Reference Kashyap, Bhattacharjee, Yeo and Chen2019a; Kong et al., Reference Kong, Li, Orban, Sabuncu, Liu, Schaefer and Eickhoff2018). The rsfMRI features were extracted using dynamic mode decomposition (DMD) technique (Brunton, Johnson, Ojemann, & Kutz, Reference Brunton, Johnson, Ojemann and Kutz2016; Rowley, Mezić, Bagheri, Schlatter, & Henningson, Reference Rowley, Mezić, Bagheri, Schlatter and Henningson2009; Schmid, Reference Schmid2010). Since rsfMRI contains information about the brain's static (spatial) and dynamic (time-evolving) properties, the DMD algorithm's capacity to retain the spatial- and frequency-based data characteristics has proven advantageous. Previous studies have applied this technique to rsfMRI and found spatio-temporal patterns (dynamic modes, DMs) to have enhanced associations with behaviors (Casorso et al., Reference Casorso, Kong, Chi, Van De Ville, Yeo and Liégeois2019; Ikeda et al., Reference Ikeda, Kawano, Watanabe, Yamashita and Kawahara2022). Here, we have used these DMs to classify subjects into two groups – one with high similarity and another with high dissimilarity between rsfMRI-features (Kashyap et al., Reference Kashyap, Bhattacharjee, Yeo and Chen2019a; Kong et al., Reference Kong, Li, Orban, Sabuncu, Liu, Schaefer and Eickhoff2018). We did not formulate any directional hypothesis regarding the behavioral manifestation of rsfMRI features. So, we compared a wide range of cohort characteristics that includes scores of psychopathology, socio-economic status, social cognition, environment of home, community and school, behavioral tasks (e.g. working memory, visual attention), demographic- (age and sex), and anthropometric-parameters (e.g. height, weight) between the two groups. Subsequently, we explored how the behavioral measures that distinguished the two groups were related to differences in neural organization by correlating the behavioral scores with the DM of the brain areas. We then investigated how the correlational pattern evolved in the three age groups (childhood, adolescence, and young adulthood). Altogether, we aimed to understand the factors that differentiate typical and atypical development by capitalizing on the heterogeneity of rsfMRIs and investigating how the neural pattern is shaped in both developmental groups.
Methods
Study protocol
The cVEDA study is a cohort of about 9000 individuals (aged 6–23 years) covering a diverse population (e.g. regions with socio-political conflicts, migratory workers with high substance use, slum, high familial risk, urban and rural) from five geo-spatial regions of India. The Institutional Ethics Review Boards of National Institute of Mental Health and Neurosciences (NIMHANS) Bangalore, India (Item No. VII, SI. No. 7.08, Behavioral Sciences) and all other collaborating institutions approved the data collection protocol setup in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (1964 and later versions). A subset of 1140 subjects underwent an intensive assessment in which multiple modalities of data pertaining to (i) neuroimaging (structural-, functional, and diffusion-MRI), (ii) behavioral–phenotypic characterization (with special emphasis on externalizing behaviors), (iii) environmental exposures (psychosocial stressors, societal discrimination, nutrition and asset security, environmental toxins), and (iv) genomics (blood/buccal swab and urine) were collected. Details of the data collection procedure are available elsewhere (Fernandes et al., Reference Fernandes, Spiers, Vaidya, Zhang, Sharma, Holla and Chakrabarti2021; Sharma et al., Reference Sharma, Ravi, Kumar, Thennarasu, Heron, Hickman and Mehta2023, Reference Sharma, Vaidya, Iyengar, Zhang, Holla, Purushottam and Hickman2020; Vaidya et al., Reference Vaidya, Holla, Heron, Sharma, Zhang, Fernandes and Das2023; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Vaidya, Iyengar, Sharma, Holla, Ahuja and Chakrabarti2020). The rsfMRI's used in this study were obtained from five different 3 T MRI scanners (for details, please refer Vaidya et al., Reference Vaidya, Holla, Heron, Sharma, Zhang, Fernandes and Das2023) with scanning duration kept at 6 min across the sites. The cVEDA team followed a standard protocol (with structural scans based on a protocol defined by the ADNI consortium http://www.loni.ucla.edu/ADNI/Cores/index.shtml) to ensure the comparability of image-acquisition techniques and the ability to pool the multi-site MRI data. The MRI scanners engaged in the data collection for cVEDA were from Siemens and Philips. Emphasis was placed to maintain the key parameters that influence image contrast and signal to noise ratio uniform across the scanners. For rsfMRI aquisition, a gradient echoplanar imaging (EPI) sequence was utilized. To facilitate the signal equilibration, three initial dummy scans were conducted and excluded from subsequent analysis. The uniform imaging parameters across sites were as follows: voxel size set at 3.4 × 3.4 mm2, slice thickness at 2.4 mm, interslice gap of 1 mm, descending slice acquisition order, repetition time (TR) of 2200 ms, echo time (TE) of 30 ms, and a flip angle set at 75 degrees. The imaging matrix was standardized at 64 × 64 mm covering 40 axial slices to ensure full brain coverage. Full technical specifications are available at http://cveda-project.org/standard-operating-procedures/.
RsfMRI preprocessing
The pre-processed rsfMRI data were obtained from cVEDA's previous study (Vaidya et al., Reference Vaidya, Holla, Heron, Sharma, Zhang, Fernandes and Das2023). They have performed rigid body registration of each functional volume to the middle volume (FSL MCFLIRT) and applied slice-time correction (Jenkinson, Beckmann, Behrens, Woolrich, & Smith, Reference Jenkinson, Beckmann, Behrens, Woolrich and Smith2012). Non-brain tissues were removed using FSLBET, and images were co-registered to high-resolution T1 image (FSL FLIRT using the BBR algorithm) (Jenkinson, Bannister, Brady, & Smith, Reference Jenkinson, Bannister, Brady and Smith2002). As a part of motion correcting transformations, BOLD-to-T1w transformation and T1w-to-MNI template (MNI) warp were applied in a single step using Advanced Normalization Toolbox (ANTs v2.1.0) (Avants, Tustison, & Song, Reference Avants, Tustison and Song2009). Frame-wise displacement was calculated for each functional run, and ICA-based Automatic Removal Of Motion Artifacts (AROMA) was used to generate non-aggressively denoised data (Pruim et al., Reference Pruim, Mennes, van Rooij, Llera, Buitelaar and Beckmann2015). A high-pass filter with a cutoff period of 125 s (<0.008 Hz) was used to remove the slow drifts and conserve high-frequency bands in the signal. Lastly, the denoized data were resampled to 2 mm isotropic and smoothed using a 4 mm non-linear filter using FSL SUSAN (Jenkinson et al., Reference Jenkinson, Bannister, Brady and Smith2002). The rsfMRI time series (165 volumes) were then extracted from 116 regions using the automated anatomical labeling (AAL) atlas that consists of 90 cortical (45 for each hemisphere) and 26 cerebellar regions (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., Reference Tzourio-Mazoyer, Landeau, Papathanassiou, Crivello, Etard, Delcroix and Joliot2002). Therefore, the rsfMRI of a subject was 116 × 165 matrix.
Primary behavioral measures
The cVEDA study collected several measures for the three age bands- children, adolescents, and young adults. In this study, we considered those parameters for which data was available across the three age bands, as some questionnaires were applicable to only specific age bands (Sharma et al., Reference Sharma, Vaidya, Iyengar, Zhang, Holla, Purushottam and Hickman2020; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Vaidya, Iyengar, Sharma, Holla, Ahuja and Chakrabarti2020). The parameters included data on (A) socio-economic status, (B) psychopathological condition, (C) environmental exposure at (i) home and neighborhood – using questionnaires from adverse childhood experiences (ACE), which includes the exposure to abuse, neglect, adversities from family and community (Felitti et al., Reference Felitti, Anda, Nordenberg, Williamson, Spitz, Edwards and Marks2019), and (ii) school – using school climate questionnaires (SCQ) (Bochaver, Korneev, & Khlomov, Reference Bochaver, Korneev and Khlomov2022); and (D) executive abilities tasks that measured – (i) risk taking propensity – using the balloon analogue risk task (BART) (Lejuez et al., Reference Lejuez, Read, Kahler, Richards, Ramsey, Stuart and Brown2002), (ii) response-inhibition using stop signal task (SST) (Logan & Cowan, Reference Logan and Cowan1984), (iii) visual attention using trail making test (TMT) (Piper et al., Reference Piper, Li, Eiwaz, Kobel, Benice, Chu and Raber2012), (iv) cognitive flexibility – using card sorting test (Berg, Reference Berg1948), (v) visuospatial-attention and working memory – using CORSI block tapping task (Corsi, Reference Corsi1972; Kessels, van den Berg, Ruis, & Brands, Reference Kessels, van den Berg, Ruis and Brands2008), (vi) short-term memory – using the digit span test (DST) (Croschere, Dupey, Hilliard, Koehn, & Mayra, Reference Croschere, Dupey, Hilliard, Koehn and Mayra2012), (vii) theory-of-mind and social perception – using Social Cognition Rating Tools in the Indian Setting (SOCRATIS) (Mehta et al., Reference Mehta, Thirthalli, Naveen Kumar, Mahadevaiah, Rao, Subbakrishna and Keshavan2011). The list of behavioral measures with a brief explanation is available in Table 1 (also see supplement). The anthropometric parameters include height (in cm), weight (in kg), body mass index, leg length (in cm), and circumference of head and mid-arm (in cm). The demographic measures included the age and sex of the subjects. Altogether, 43 cohort characteristics (measuring socio-economic status-1, psychopathology-1, environmental experiences-11, cognition & task performance-22, anthropometry-6, and demography-2) that were available across a total of 987 subjects were analyzed.
Extraction of rsfMRI features/dynamic modes (DMs)
The DMD algorithm was originally developed to understand fluid dynamics, and the details are described in the methodological papers (Brunton et al., Reference Brunton, Johnson, Ojemann and Kutz2016; Kutz, Brunton, Brunton, & Proctor, Reference Kutz, Fu and Brunton2016; Rowley et al., Reference Rowley, Mezić, Bagheri, Schlatter and Henningson2009; Schmid, Reference Schmid2010). It has also been applied to rsfMRI to extract features for biomarker development (Casorso et al., Reference Casorso, Kong, Chi, Van De Ville, Yeo and Liégeois2019; Ikeda et al., Reference Ikeda, Kawano, Watanabe, Yamashita and Kawahara2022). DMD is a dimensionality reduction approach that builds on the power of singular value decomposition to provide the spatio-temporal features of the multidimensional data (see supplement). The low-rank eigen-decomposition technique in DMD computed eigenvectors and corresponding eigenvalues from rsfMRI data (Casorso et al., Reference Casorso, Kong, Chi, Van De Ville, Yeo and Liégeois2019; Ikeda et al., Reference Ikeda, Kawano, Watanabe, Yamashita and Kawahara2022). Eigenvectors (i.e. spatial characteristics) represent dynamic modes (DMs), which are coherent spatial structures, and the corresponding eigenvalues (i.e. temporal characteristics) represent the frequency. Studies have shown that DM obtained with all the frequency bands combined has higher associations with the behaviors (Ikeda et al., Reference Ikeda, Kawano, Watanabe, Yamashita and Kawahara2022), and the complementary information about individual differences leads to improved classification accuracy (Huang et al., Reference Huang, Zhu, Hao, Shi, Gao, Xu and Zhang2019). Therefore, the DM (116 × 1 matrix) was obtained for each individual across the complete range of frequency.
Determining subjects with atypical DM pattern
Initially, all the subjects (n = 987) were considered as one group and the DM of every subject was obtained. The subjects were then classified based on the similarity/dissimilarity in the pattern of the DM (Kashyap et al., Reference Kashyap, Bhattacharjee, Yeo and Chen2019a; Kong et al., Reference Kong, Li, Orban, Sabuncu, Liu, Schaefer and Eickhoff2018). To determine this, we correlated DMs across subjects using Pearson's correlation and obtained a 987 × 987 DM-correlation matrix. To select a subset of subjects with dissimilar patterns, we randomly picked an entry in the DM-correlation matrix (representing a pair of subjects) whose absolute correlation was less than a threshold of 0.80 (see supplement for other thresholds). We continued adding new random subjects, such that each newly added subject was minimally correlated (absolute r < 0.80) with the current set of subjects. The procedure terminated when no more subjects could be added. The procedure was repeated 5000 times, resulting in 5000 sets with varied numbers of subjects per set. Of these 5000 sets, we chose the set containing subjects with the smallest maximum absolute correlation. This subset of subjects formed the dissimilar-rsfMRI-pattern group, and the remaining subjects constructed the similar-rsfMRI-pattern group.
Brain-behavior associations
The present study employs a data-driven, bottom-up approach, where we begin by investigating variations in brain feature patterns and subsequently explore how these differences might be manifested in behavior. Therefore, after classifying the subjects with similar and dissimilar patterns of rsfMRI we identified traits that distinguished the groups by performing two-tailed t test with Bonferroni correction across the 43 parameters (measuring socioeconomic status, psychopathology, environmental exposure at home/school/society, cognition and task performance, demography, and anthropometry).
We then analyzed the potential influence of the differentiating factors on the resting brain features in both similar- and dissimilar-rsfMRI-groups. To this, we correlated (r) the DMs of the brain areas (that are different between the two groups) with the scores of the behavioral measure that differentiated the two groups. To trace how these relationships evolved with age, we conducted separate correlations for children (6–11 years), adolescents (12–17 years), and young adults (18–23 years). The procedure was repeated for both sexes as well.
Exploratory and additional behavioral measures
With a similar approach, in our previous work on HCP adults, we found the subjects in the dissimilar group to have higher usage of marijuana, illicit drugs, alcohol, and tobacco, with problems of antisocial personality (Kashyap et al., Reference Kashyap, Bhattacharjee, Yeo and Chen2019a). Interestingly, the c-VEDA team have also provided similar estimates related to clinical assessment (using the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview, and Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire), externalizing behavior and psychopathology including substance use behavioral addictions (a total of 41 measures, see supplement) (Sharma et al., Reference Sharma, Vaidya, Iyengar, Zhang, Holla, Purushottam and Hickman2020; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Vaidya, Iyengar, Sharma, Holla, Ahuja and Chakrabarti2020). Naturally, it became interesting to explore whether these behaviors are also significant in neurodevelopment. To this, for each behavioral measure we removed the subjects with missing data from both the groups (similar- and dissimilar-rsfMRI) and performed two-tail t test with Bonferroni's correction.
Data and code availability
The public dataset cVEDA is available at https://cveda-project.org/. The code of DMD can be downloaded at https://faculty.washington.edu/kutz/page26/. The code for classification is also available at https://github.com/suklamaa/Maximizing_Dissimilarity_in_fMRI.
Results
DM spatial maps
The spatial map of averaged DM across subjects (n = 987) is shown in Fig. 1a. We extracted subjects with a dissimilar pattern of rsfMRI from the DM correlation matrix. A total of 178 subjects formed the dissimilar-rsfMRI-pattern group (max r = 0.79), and the remaining 809 subjects formed the similar-rsfMRI-pattern group (max r = 0.99). For the dissimilar-rsfMRI-pattern group, the number of subjects in the three age bands were- children = 50, adolescents = 59, and young adults = 69. Similarly, for the similar-rsfMRI-pattern group, number of children, adolescents, and young adults were 173, 342, and 294, respectively. The two groups differed in the distribution of the DM (p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected) across a set of 18 brain areas located bilaterally in the (i) frontal regions that comprise frontal supra orbital (FSO), frontal mid orbital (FMO), and frontal inferior orbital (FIO); (ii) parietal regions that include mid cingulum (MC), cuneus (Cun), and precuneus (PreCun); and (iii) temporal regions with three areas temporal pole superior (TPSup), temporal pole mid (TPMid), and inferior temporal lobe (ITL). The location of these areas in the brain is shown in red, pink, and blue colored dots, each representing the areas specific to frontal, parietal, and temporal regions (Fig. 1). For the similar-rsfMRI-pattern group, the DMs (mean ± s.d.) across the subjects for the 18 bilateral areas were (i) left-FSO (−0.04 ± 0.00), right-FSO (−0.04 ± 0.00); (ii) left-FMO (−0.06 ± 0.00), right-FMO (−0.06 ± 0.00); (iii) left-FIO (−0.06 ± 0.00), right-FIO (−0.06 ± 0.00); (iv) left-MC (−0.10 ± 0.00), right-MC (−0.10 ± 0.00); (v) left-Cun (−0.11 ± 0.01), right-Cun (−0.12 ± 0.01); (vi) left-PreCun (−0.11 ± 0.01), right-PreCun (−0.11 ± 0.00); (vii) left-TPSup (−0.06 ± 0.00), right-TPSup (−0.06 ± 0.00); (viii) left-TPMid (−0.05 ± 0.01), right-TPMid (−0.05 ± 0.01); and (ix) left-ITL (−0.05 ± 0.00), right-ITL (−0.05 ± 0.01). Similarly, for the dissimilar-rsfMRI-pattern group, the DMs (mean ± s.d.) for 18 bilateral areas were (i) left-FSO (−0.05 ± 0.01), right-FSO (−0.04 ± 0.01); (ii) left-FMO (−0.06 ± 0.01), right-FMO (−0.06 ± 0.01); (iii) left-FIO (−0.07 ± 0.00), right-FIO (−0.06 ± 0.00); (iv) left-MC (−0.09 ± 0.00), right-MC (−0.09 ± 0.00); (v) left-Cun (−0.11 ± 0.01), right-Cun (−0.11 ± 0.01); (vi) left-PreCun (−0.11 ± 0.01), right-PreCun (−0.10 ± 0.00); (vii) left-TPSup (−0.06 ± 0.00), right-TPSup (−0.06 ± 0.00); (viii) left-TPMid (−0.05 ± 0.00), right-TPMid (−0.05 ± 0.01); and (ix) left-ITL (−0.06 ± 0.00), right-ITL (−0.06 ± 0.01). This has been shown in Fig. 1b (i and ii) for the areas in the left and right hemispheres, respectively. These areas are a part of the default mode network of the brain (Buckner, Andrews-Hanna, & Schacter, Reference Buckner, Andrews-Hanna and Schacter2008).
Behavioral association
The scores of 43 cohort characteristics were compared between the dissimilar- and similar-rsfMRI pattern groups (see supplement). We found two behavioral measures (adversity frequency and abuse) to survive the significance level with Bonferroni's correction (p < 0.05). This suggested that frequent adversities encountered during the developmental phase significantly influence the resting state pattern as evaluated from the rsfMRI (Fig. 1a). The similar-rsfMRI pattern group representing the typical neurodevelopment comprised subjects that had less (1.07 ± 1.23) frequent encounters with adversities compared to the dissimilar-rsfMRI pattern group (2.26 ± 2.35). The effect size, as measured by Cohen's d, was d = 0.80, indicating a large effect of frequent exposure to adversities on the differences in the rsfMRI pattern between the two groups during neurodevelopment. Interestingly, only the scores of abuse (that constitutes adversity) differentiated the two groups, with individuals in the similar-rsfMRI pattern group facing less abuse (−0.13 ± 1.07) compared to the dissimilar-rsfMRI pattern group (1.58 ± 1.40). The Cohen's d of 1.18 suggested the large impact abuse holds on the development of the functional architecture of the brain during growth. Statistical analysis of the additional behaviors related to externalizing and substance use was not significant (see Table 2S in supplement).
The similar-rsfMRI pattern group had 194 children, 323 adolescents, and 292 young adults. For the similar-rsfMRI pattern group, the correlation of the DMs with the frequency of adversities was significant (p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected) for two frontal regions (FMO, and FIO), two parietal regions (Cun and PreCun), and one temporal region (ITL) across both hemispheres. For the children, the correlation values for the brain areas were (i) left-FMO = 0.27, right-FMO r = 0.25; (ii) left-FIO r = 0.22, right-FIO = 0.23; (iii) left-Cun r = 0.23, right-Cun r = 0.25; (iv) left- and right-PreCun r = 0.21; and (v) left- and right-ITL r = 0.23. For the adolescents, the correlation values for the brain areas were (i) left-FMO r = 0.28, right-FMO r = 0.27; (ii) left-FIO r = 0.24, right-FIO r = 0.21; (iii) left-Cun r = 0.27, right-Cun r = 0.26; (iv) left-PreCun r = 0.22, right-PreCun = 0.24; and (v) left-ITL r = 0.28, right-ITL r = 0.26. For the young adults, the correlation values for the brain areas were (i) left-FMO r = 0.22, right-FMO r = 0.27; (ii) left-FIO r = 0.21, right-FIO r = 0.20; (iii) left- and right-Cun r = 0.22; (iv) left- and right-PreCun r = 0.21; and (v) left-ITL r = 0.24, right-ITL = 0.26. Interestingly, the correlational pattern remained consistent across the three age bands (Fig. 1b). For the ease of visualization, the spatial location of the areas is mapped in Fig. 2a. The correlational pattern was also observed between the scores of abuse and the DMs. The correlation pattern remained similar as observed for frequency of adversity (so the values are not repeated) owing to high (r = 0.60) and significant (p < 0.00001) correlation between scores of abuse and frequency of adversities.
Correspondingly, the dissimilar-rsfMRI pattern group consisted of 30 children, 77 adolescents, and 71 young adults. Significant correlations (p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected) of DM with adversity were observed in (i) parietal regions (MC, Cun, and PrCun) for children, (ii) frontal regions (FSO, FMO, and FIO) for adolescents, and (iii) parieto-temporal regions (MC, Cun, and PrCun; TPSup, TPMid, and ITL) for the young adults (Fig. 1c). For the children, the correlation values for the brain areas in parietal regions were (i) left-MC r = 0.32, right-MC r = 0.34; (ii) left-Cun r = 0.51, right-Cun r = 0.52; and (iii) left-PreCun r = 0.33, right-PreCun = 0.36. For the adolescents, the correlation values for the brain areas in frontal regions were (i) left-FSO r = 0.36, right-FSO r = 0.34; (ii) left-FMO r = 0.45, right-FMO r = 0.47; and (iii) left-FIO r = 0.34, right-FIO r = 0.37. For the young adults, the correlation values for the brain areas in parieto-temporal regions were (i) left-MC r = 0.37, right-MC r = 0.33; (ii) left-Cun r = 0.36, right-Cun r = 0.34; (iii) left-PreCun r = 0.35, right-PreCun r = 0.37; (iv) left-TPSup r = 0.38, right-TPSup = 0.34; (v) left-TPSup = 0.35, right-TPSup r = 0.36; and (vi) left-ITL r = 0.36, right-ITL r = 0.38. The correlational pattern was also observed between the scores of abuse and the DMs. The correlation pattern trended similarly as observed for frequency of adversity (so the values are not repeated) owing to high (r = 0.58) and significant (p < 0.00001) correlation between scores of abuse and frequency of adversities. Moreover, for the two groups, there were no sex-specific signature differences in the correlational pattern of DMs and frequency of adversities.
In contrast to the similar-rsfMRI pattern group, the dissimilar-rsfMRI pattern group displayed shifts in correlational patterns across the three age bands. As illustrated in Fig. 3b, these shifts emphasize the age-related changes in neural organization resulting from ongoing exposure to adversities in different developmental windows. Specifically, the involvement of parietal region was most evident in children, the frontal region in adolescents, and both parieto-temporal regions in young adults.
Discussion
The purpose of this study was to identify the atypical signature of brain development latent within the general population, investigate the cohort characteristics associated with the atypicality, and understand how the neural system is shaped by aberrant characteristics. To this, we adopted our previous approach (Kashyap et al., Reference Kashyap, Bhattacharjee, Yeo and Chen2019a) to classify the rsfMRI features (obtained from the DMD technique) of 987 subjects from the cVEDA neurodevelopmental cohort (6–23 years). Two groups with similar- and dissimilar-rsfMRI patterns (n = 809 and 178) emerged. The similar-rsfMRI-pattern group with a more homogenous resting state brain pattern represented typical development, and the other represented an atypical pattern of neurodevelopment. The pattern showed significant differences in the 18 bilateral areas from the frontal (FSO, FMO, and FIO), parietal (MC, Cun, and PreCun), and temporal (TPSup, TPMid, and ITL) regions representing the default mode network (DMN) (Andrews-Hanna, Reidler, Sepulcre, Poulin, & Buckner, Reference Andrews-Hanna, Reidler, Sepulcre, Poulin and Buckner2010; Buckner et al., Reference Buckner, Andrews-Hanna and Schacter2008). Frequent encounters with life adversities distinguished the two groups, with atypicality being associated with higher frequency (seen in the dissimilar-rsfMRI pattern group). Within the parameters that constitute adversity, abuses faced during the neurodevelopmental period were of primary concern. The study is in support of the ongoing effort aimed to embrace neural heterogeneity in the population (Drysdale et al., Reference Drysdale, Grosenick, Downar, Dunlop, Mansouri, Meng and Etkin2017; Mattoni et al., Reference Mattoni, Smith and Olino2023; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Nichols, Vidaurre, Winkler, Behrens, Glasser and Miller2015; Zhu et al., Reference Zhu, Becquey, Chen, Lejuez, Li and Bi2022). These studies have suggested that the hypothesis-free bottom-up approach (as adopted in our study) – wherein biological subgroups with more homogenous brain patterns across individuals are first identified, and then behavioral differences between them are examined – can provide new insights into mental health-related research and clinical practice (Fair, Dosenbach, Moore, Satterthwaite, & Milham, Reference Fair, Dosenbach, Moore, Satterthwaite and Milham2021; Feczko & Fair, Reference Feczko and Fair2020; Mattoni et al., Reference Mattoni, Smith and Olino2023). Adding on, we further evaluated how the neural system has been restructured by the frequent adversities encountered by individuals of the two groups (Similar and dissimilar-rsfMRI pattern group). For this, the DMs of the DMN areas were correlated with the adversity frequency scores. While the correlational pattern in typical subjects (similar-rsfMRI pattern group) was found in frontal (FMO and FIO), parietal (Cun and PrCun), and temporal (ITL) regions, an interesting variation in the pattern with age was seen for atypical subjects (dissimilar-rsfMRI pattern group). The pattern in atypical children was clustered in parietal (MC, Cun, and PrCun) regions, subsequently shifting to frontal regions (FSO, FMO, and FIO) in adolescents, and finally simmering to parieto-temporal (MC, Cun, and PrCun; and TPSup, TPMid, and ITL) regions in young adults. The instability in the pattern provided an essence of how the brain might have adapted to adversity across the three developmental windows.
Significance of DMN and childhood adversity
Both good and bad experiences shape the human brain during development (Tost, Champagne, & Meyer-Lindenberg, Reference Tost, Champagne and Meyer-Lindenberg2015). The DMN involved in self-referential mental activity plays a vital role in accounting for these experiences during the early phase (Buckner et al., Reference Buckner, Andrews-Hanna and Schacter2008; Davis, Hirsch, Gee, Andover, & Roy, Reference Davis, Hirsch, Gee, Andover and Roy2022; Rebello et al., Reference Rebello, Moura, Pinaya, Rohde and Sato2018). Several studies using the conventional top-down approach have reported adverse childhood experiences to be associated with structural and functional abnormalities of DMN and its interconnections with other brain areas (Hair, Hanson, Wolfe, & Pollak, Reference Hair, Hanson, Wolfe and Pollak2015; Hanson et al., Reference Hanson, Adluru, Chung, Alexander, Davidson and Pollak2013; Sripada et al., Reference Sripada, Rutherford, Angstadt, Thompson, Luciana, Weigard and Heitzeg2020; Tottenham, Reference Tottenham2014). There is enough support showing childhood trauma and adversities to alter DMN activity (Barch, Belden, Tillman, Whalen, & Luby, Reference Barch, Belden, Tillman, Whalen and Luby2018; Davis et al., Reference Davis, Hirsch, Gee, Andover and Roy2022; Holz et al., Reference Holz, Berhe, Sacu, Schwarz, Tesarz, Heim and Tost2022; McLaughlin & Lambert, Reference McLaughlin and Lambert2017; McLaughlin, Peverill, Gold, Alves, & Sheridan, Reference McLaughlin, Peverill, Gold, Alves and Sheridan2015; McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Weissman and Bitrán2019; Rebello et al., Reference Rebello, Moura, Pinaya, Rohde and Sato2018; Zeev-Wolf, Levy, Goldstein, Zagoory-Sharon, & Feldman, Reference Zeev-Wolf, Levy, Goldstein, Zagoory-Sharon and Feldman2019) and positive parenting to buffer the DMN development against environmental disturbances (Dégeilh, Bernier, Leblanc, Daneault, & Beauchamp, Reference Dégeilh, Bernier, Leblanc, Daneault and Beauchamp2018; Whittle et al., Reference Whittle, Vijayakumar, Simmons, Dennison, Schwartz, Pantelis and Allen2017). Though a study has reported different dimensions of adversity (experiences of unpredictability, threat, and deprivation) to be related to DMN and other resting state brain networks (fronto-parietal network and salience network) (Chahal, Miller, Yuan, Buthmann, & Gotlib, Reference Chahal, Miller, Yuan, Buthmann and Gotlib2022), in our study, only abuse emerged as a significant contributor of DMN heterogeneity. Higher adversity and abuse affect mental and physical health throughout life (Nelson, Bhutta, Harris, Danese, & Samara, Reference Nelson, Bhutta, Harris, Danese and Samara2020). As our exploratory approach also finds distinct differences in the resting state pattern to be associated with the frequency of adverse experiences and abuse, alteration of DMN can be considered as an objective marker of atypical neurodevelopment.
A similar approach, when applied to the rsfMRI dataset of aging subjects from HCP (late adulthood to old age), revealed the differences in the DMN pattern to be associated with antisocial personality, substance use, and higher consumption of alcohol, and tobacco (Kashyap et al., Reference Kashyap, Bhattacharjee, Yeo and Chen2019a). A similar trend can also be observed in this study (see supplementary table 2S for the p values), though we did not observe any statistically significant difference in consumption habits and personality between the groups. This may be because of the (i) stringent criteria for statistical significance (Bonferroni's correction) adopted in the study, (ii) majority of the cVEDA subjects with imaging measures are minors and have reported no use (or minimal use) of alcohol, tobacco, and illicit drugs, and (iii) it may be too early for the manifestation of these behaviors in the features of rsfMRI. We say this because another study that considered only the young adults of cVEDA with a large sample size (n = 9010) found associations between adversity and predisposition toward externalizing disorders, including substance use (Fernandes et al., Reference Fernandes, Spiers, Vaidya, Zhang, Sharma, Holla and Chakrabarti2021). Similarly, several behavioral studies (using large sample sizes) have also reported significant associations between adversities of early life and antisocial personality, consumption of illicit drugs, alcohol and tobacco in later stages of life (Acheson, Vincent, Cohoon, & Lovallo, Reference Acheson, Vincent, Cohoon and Lovallo2021; He et al., Reference He, Yan, Wang, Zhao, Liu, Zhou and Zeng2022; Krinner, Warren-Findlow, & Bowling, Reference Krinner, Warren-Findlow and Bowling2020; Lui et al., Reference Lui, Witbrodt, Li, Tam, Williams, Guo and Mulia2023; Whitesell, Beals, Mitchell, Manson, & Turner, Reference Whitesell, Beals, Mitchell, Manson and Turner2009; Yazgan et al., Reference Yazgan, Hanson, Bates, Lansford, Pettit and Dodge2021), and it can be inferred that alterations to the DMN during the early stage can have long-lasting effects on the mental health. This was also reported in a recent meta-analysis study that the neurodevelopmental period (6–25 years) is a seed time for neuropsychiatric disorders (Meredith, Reference Meredith2015; Solmi et al., Reference Solmi, Radua, Olivola, Croce, Soardo, Salazar de Pablo and Fusar-Poli2022). Knotting the current findings with our previous research (Kashyap et al., Reference Kashyap, Bhattacharjee, Yeo and Chen2019a), a nexus map emerges, suggesting that the neural basis of atypical behaviors is in DMN, and the environment prunes this system from early childhood. This is inline with the recent structural imaging study that investigated the neural correlates of adversity over a longitudinal period and found areas in frontal, cingulate, and limbic regions to be stable (Holz et al., Reference Holz, Zabihi, Kia, Monninger, Aggensteiner, Siehl and Marquand2023). Since cross-sectional data poses some limitations, future studies should apply such methodologies to longitudinal datasets to establish the continuum of mental health and illness by investigating the cumulative effect of protective- and risk-factors (e.g. education, diet, genetics, and environment) (Walhovd, Lövden, & Fjell, Reference Walhovd, Lövden and Fjell2023) that fabricate the DMN from infancy (Gao, Lin, Grewen, & Gilmore, Reference Gao, Lin, Grewen and Gilmore2017; Gao et al., Reference Gao, Zhu, Giovanello, Smith, Shen, Gilmore and Lin2009) to maturity (Rebello et al., Reference Rebello, Moura, Pinaya, Rohde and Sato2018; Washington & VanMeter, Reference Washington and VanMeter2015) to the old age (Buckner et al., Reference Buckner, Andrews-Hanna and Schacter2008; Jones et al., Reference Jones, Machulda, Vemuri, McDade, Zeng, Senjem and Jack2011). Altogether, the study suggests that the failure to develop a coherent DMN system due to childhood adversities might have cascading effects on an individual's trajectory of growth and aging.
The differential utilization of DMN
The sensitive period from childhood to young adulthood is where neural systems mature, including those involved in the regulation of threat, stress, and reward (Uhlhaas et al., Reference Uhlhaas, Davey, Mehta, Shah, Torous, Allen and Wood2023). The regions of DMN that regulate these functions undergo developmental changes over this period (Rebello et al., Reference Rebello, Moura, Pinaya, Rohde and Sato2018). The network adapts according to the environment and matures accordingly (Menon, Reference Menon2013). However, the knowledge about how environmental demands affect DMN maturation and how this could be related to an atypical developmental pattern is limited (Fair et al., Reference Fair, Posner, Nagel, Bathula, Dias, Mills and Nigg2010; Rebello et al., Reference Rebello, Moura, Pinaya, Rohde and Sato2018). Our study finds that in typical development, there is consistency in the correlational pattern across the three age bands. This suggests that the integrity of the DMN over the developmental trajectory is crucial for the efficient processing of neural information (Sporns, Reference Sporns2013). The integrity of the DMN plays an important role in normal development (Raichle, Reference Raichle2015; Sonuga-Barke & Castellanos, Reference Sonuga-Barke and Castellanos2007), and its alteration has been associated with neurocognitive disorders (Dajani et al., Reference Dajani, Burrows, Odriozola, Baez, Nebel, Mostofsky and Uddin2019; Fair et al., Reference Fair, Posner, Nagel, Bathula, Dias, Mills and Nigg2010; Nair, Jolliffe, Lograsso, & Bearden, Reference Nair, Jolliffe, Lograsso and Bearden2020; Uddin et al., Reference Uddin, Kelly, Biswal, Margulies, Shehzad, Shaw and Milham2008). Several measures from graph theory (e.g. small-world topology and modularity) have found that though the neural system of typically developing children undergoes radical changes, the fundamental network characteristics seen in the brains of older children and adults get established during childhood (Menon, Reference Menon2013).
In an atypical population, the correlational pattern fluctuates from parietal to frontal to parieto-temporal regions of DMN over the course of development (childhood, adolescence, and young adulthood). This is in support of a recent review article that found that exposure to stress/adversity at different sensitive periods might perturb different brain areas and affect different behaviors with different psychopathological outcomes (Andersen, Reference Andersen2022). It might be possible that cumulative adversity leads to the failure in the development of a coherent DMN system – a key network contributing to the emergence of efficient social information processing in the youth (Blakemore & Mills, Reference Blakemore and Mills2014). Studies have suggested that abnormal synaptic pruning in the local circuit leads to heterogeneity in the pattern of brain functioning, a feature commonly seen in atypical development (Chattopadhyaya & Cristo, Reference Chattopadhyaya and Cristo2012; Germann et al., Reference Germann, Brederoo and Sommer2021; Gogolla et al., Reference Gogolla, LeBlanc, Quast, Südhof, Fagiolini and Hensch2009; Patel, Leathem, Currin, & Karlsgodt, Reference Patel, Leathem, Currin and Karlsgodt2021). This experience-dependent plasticity, particularly during sensitive periods, may contribute to functional and structural differences in the developing brain. This can lead to differences in a variety of complex social and cognitive abilities (Barch et al., Reference Barch, Belden, Tillman, Whalen and Luby2018; McLaughlin & Lambert, Reference McLaughlin and Lambert2017; McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Peverill, Gold, Alves and Sheridan2015, Reference McLaughlin, Weissman and Bitrán2019; Milbocker et al., Reference Milbocker, Campbell, Collins, Kim, Smith, Roth and Klintsova2021; Rebello et al., Reference Rebello, Moura, Pinaya, Rohde and Sato2018). Though it is difficult to underpin the exact reason behind such a shift in the pattern with age, an underdeveloped DMN may deprive the neural system of the faculties of normal functioning. On the other hand, recent studies found the DMN and associated areas to have a protective role in coping with stress (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Zhao, Suo, Zhang, Pan, Kemp and Wang2023; Sinha, Lacadie, Constable, & Seo, Reference Sinha, Lacadie, Constable and Seo2016), though acute stress alters its processing (Zeev-Wolf et al., Reference Zeev-Wolf, Levy, Goldstein, Zagoory-Sharon and Feldman2019; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Hashemi, Kaldewaij, Koch, Beckmann, Klumpers and Roelofs2019). Differential activation of these areas has also been found depending on the stress level (Sinha et al., Reference Sinha, Lacadie, Constable and Seo2016). Therefore, it cannot be denied that adversity-related neuroplasticity could also be a protective mechanism that provides the flexibility to cope with adverse environmental conditions (Sinha et al., Reference Sinha, Lacadie, Constable and Seo2016).
Altogether, the differential utilization of DMN areas emphasizes that adversity (particularly abuse) that has maximal influence in bifurcating the trajectory of development can drive the neurodevelopmental pattern towards atypicality. While typical development follows a constant pattern of utilization of DMN areas, the pattern fluctuates with age in atypical neurodevelopment. This highlights that DMN that is known to imprint the environmental cues (Rebello et al., Reference Rebello, Moura, Pinaya, Rohde and Sato2018) is malleable to the situation where it develops. The adaptable nature of DMN might be a compensatory mechanism to protect an individual in an abusive environment, though such benefits are incurred at the cost of normal functioning and may have long-term effects on the psyche.
Our findings are to be interpreted within the recently proposed youth mental health paradigm (Uhlhaas et al., Reference Uhlhaas, Davey, Mehta, Shah, Torous, Allen and Wood2023), which emphasizes a shift from studying individuals with fully established disorders to studying emerging mental disorders or their behavioral substrates during youth. First, using a hypothesis-free bottom-up approach, we identified groups of individuals within a diverse developmental cohort that are characterized by similar or dissimilar rsfMRI patterns; these groups differed primarily on properties of brain nodes that traditionally comprise the default mode network (DMN). Second, using a statistically stringent measure, we identified a significantly greater frequency of adversities experienced, particularly that of abuse, by individuals in the dissimilar group. Lastly, we observed age-band dependent associations between functional brain features within the DMN regions – critical for social information processing, particularly in a developmental context (Blakemore & Mills, Reference Blakemore and Mills2014) and cumulative adversity in the atypical/dissimilar group, but age-band independent associations in the typical/similar group. We did not find significant differences in cognition or psychopathology between these groups. This indicates that the evolution of DMN is an allostatic feature of environmental conditions experienced during neurodevelopment (Rebello et al., Reference Rebello, Moura, Pinaya, Rohde and Sato2018). Longitudinal studies in the future will be able to reveal if adversity experiences and their neural correlates, as identified in this study can have cascading or domino effects in the emergence of fully established mental disorders. Together, it can be inferred that our bottom-up approach helps stratify a potentially vulnerable youth group (with greater adversity experiences) where more targeted and systematic intervention can be provided.
Conclusions
In this exploratory work, we intended to find those factors that drive neurodevelopment in India's children, adolescents, and young adults towards atypicality. Leveraging the potential of large rsfMRI datasets (n = 987) from the cVEDA neurodevelopmental cohort (6–23 years) we explored the heterogeneity in the brain pattern. We classified subjects based on the rsfMRI features, separating a subset with divergent patterns indicative of atypical development, while the others exhibiting similar rsfMRI patterns represented typical development. Significant contrasts emerged in regions pertaining to the DMN across these groups.
Interestingly, those exhibiting atypical rsfMRI patterns were exposed more frequently to adversities and faced higher abuses. While typically developing subjects maintained a consistent association of DMN areas with adversity across all ages, atypically developing individuals displayed variable and age-band-dependent patterns across parietal, frontal, and parieto-temporal regions, stratified by children, adolescents, and young adults. Collectively, these insights suggest that DMN's integrity is maintained during typical development, whereas recurring adversities may instigate differential utilization of the DMN, resulting in an altered pattern across different developmental stages in atypical development.
Supplementary material
The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291724000710.
Acknowledgements
Kashyap and Bhattacharjee are supported by the Department of Biotechnology (DBT) Ramalingaswami Re-entry fellowship (2021), under the Government of India (BT/HRD/35/02/2006). Mehta acknowledges support of the Department of Biotechnology – Wellcome Trust Indian Alliance Clinical Research Centre in Neuromodulation Grant (No. IA/CRC/19/1/610005). c-VEDA (Consortium on Vulnerability to Externalizing Disorders and Addictions) study was supported by ICMR, India grant (ICMR/MRC-UK/3/M/2015-NCD-I) to Benegal and Medical Research Council grant (MR/N000390/1) to Schumann.
Author contributions
Concept and design – Kashyap, and Bhattacharjee. Framework Implementation and advance analysis- Kashyap, Bhattacharjee, Sharma, Bharath, Mehta, and Benegal. Data acquisition, and preliminary analysis – Holla, Vaidya, Sharma, Basu, Nanjayya, Singh, Lourembam, Chakrabarti, Kalyanram, Kartik, Kumaran, Krishnaveni, Krishna, Kuriyan, Kurpad, Desrivieres, Purushottam, Barker, Orfanos, Hickman, Heron, Toledano, Schumann. Initial data pre-processing and interpretation – Kashyap, Sharma, Holla, and Vaidya. Initial Drafting of the manuscript- Kashyap, and Bhattacharjee. Critical Revision and intellectual input – Kashyap, Mehta, Holla, Sharma, Vaidya, Bharath, Benegal, Schumann, and Barker. Administrative, technical, or material support – Murthy, Benegal, Bharath, Holla, Vaidya, Sharma, and Mehta. Supervision – Bharath, Mehta, and Benegal.
Competing interests
The authors declare that there is no conflict of interest.