Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T04:46:06.823Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The long-term effects of a polygenetic predisposition to general cognition on healthy cognitive ageing: evidence from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 February 2022

Olesya Ajnakina*
Affiliation:
Department of Behavioural Science and Health, Institute of Epidemiology and Health Care, University College London, 1-19 Torrington Place, London, WC1E 7HB, UK Department of Biostatistics & Health Informatics, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, 16 De Crespigny Park, Camberwell, London, SE5 8AF, UK
Robin Murray
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK Department of Psychiatry, Experimental Biomedicine and Clinical Neuroscience, University of Palermo, Palermo, Italy
Andrew Steptoe
Affiliation:
Department of Behavioural Science and Health, Institute of Epidemiology and Health Care, University College London, 1-19 Torrington Place, London, WC1E 7HB, UK
Dorina Cadar
Affiliation:
Department of Behavioural Science and Health, Institute of Epidemiology and Health Care, University College London, 1-19 Torrington Place, London, WC1E 7HB, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Olesya Ajnakina, E-mail: o.ajnakina@ucl.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

As an accelerated cognitive decline frequently heralds onset of severe neuropathological disorders, understanding the source of individual differences in withstanding the onslaught of cognitive ageing may highlight how best cognitive abilities may be retained into advanced age.

Methods

Using a population representative sample of 5088 adults aged •50 years from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing, we investigated relationships of polygenic predisposition to general cognition with a rate of change in cognition during a 10-year follow-up period. Polygenic predisposition was measured with polygenic scores for general cognition (GC-PGS). Cognition was measured employing tests for verbal memory and semantic fluency.

Results

The average baseline memory score was 11.1 (s.d. = 2.9) and executive function score was 21.5 (s.d. = 5.8). An increase in GC-PGS by one standard deviation (1-s.d.) was associated with a higher baseline verbal memory by an average 0.27 points (95% CI 0.19–0.34, p < 0.001). Similarly, 1-s.d. increase in GC-PGS was associated with a higher semantic fluency score at baseline in the entire sample (β = 0.45, 95% CI 0.27–0.64, p < 0.001). These associations were significant for women and men, and all age groups. Nonetheless, 1-s.d. increase in GC-PGS was not associated with decreases in verbal memory nor semantic fluency during follow-up in the entire sample, as well stratified models by sex and age.

Conclusion

Although common genetic variants associated with general cognition additively are associated with a stable surplus to cognition in adults, a polygenic predisposition to general cognition is not associated with age-related cognitive decline during a 10-year follow-up.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re- use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Cognitive decline, including verbal memory and executive function, is among the most feared aspects of growing old, as it frequently heralds onset of dementia spectrum (Rajan, Wilson, Weuve, Barnes, & Evans, Reference Rajan, Wilson, Weuve, Barnes and Evans2015) and other adverse health-related outcomes including mortality (Deary et al., Reference Deary, Corley, Gow, Harris, Houlihan, Marioni and Starr2009; Wilson, Beckett, Bienias, Evans, & Bennett, Reference Wilson, Beckett, Bienias, Evans and Bennett2003). Although multiple factors may determine individual paths of cognitive decline, investigating the mechanisms underlying individual differences in withstanding the onslaught of cognitive ageing is likely to highlight how best cognitive abilities may be retained into advanced age (Deary, Reference Deary2013).

General cognitive function has a heritability of >50–70% from adolescence through adulthood to older ages (Plomin & Deary, Reference Plomin and Deary2015; Tucker-Drob, Briley, & Harden, Reference Tucker-Drob, Briley and Harden2013). More specifically, heritability of general cognitive ability has been shown to have a linear increase from childhood (~40%) to adolescence (~50%) to adulthood (~60%) (Haworth et al., Reference Haworth, Wright, Luciano, Martin, de Geus, van Beijsterveldt and Plomin2010; Plomin & Deary, Reference Plomin and Deary2015; Tucker-Drob et al., Reference Tucker-Drob, Briley and Harden2013). Genetic correlations among different cognitive domains are estimated to be >0.6, indicating that the same genes may be responsible for the heritability of these domains (Plomin & Kovas, Reference Plomin and Kovas2005). More recent genomic studies of general cognition revealed that its genetic architecture is characterised by multiple common genetic markers spread across the entire genome where the identified genes additively explained 12–25% of variation in the trait of cognition (Davies et al., Reference Davies, Lam, Harris, Trampush, Luciano, Hill and Deary2018; Savage et al., Reference Savage, Jansen, Stringer, Watanabe, Bryois, de Leeuw and Posthuma2018). Building on the results from these genome-wide association studies (GWAS), the polygenic scores (PGS), which measure an individual genetic predisposition to a trait by combining the effects of many common genetic variants associated with it (Dudbridge, Reference Dudbridge2013), further demonstrated that individual differences in cognitive function are driven by thousands of common genetic markers scattered across the whole genome (Davies et al., Reference Davies, Lam, Harris, Trampush, Luciano, Hill and Deary2018; Savage et al., Reference Savage, Jansen, Stringer, Watanabe, Bryois, de Leeuw and Posthuma2018). Cumulatively, these studies demonstrated that a higher load of common genetic markers for general cognition are associated with better cognition.

Because genetic variants are determined randomly at conception and segregated independently of environmental influences, PGSs can be seen as unconfounded proxies for the life-time predisposition to general cognition. Consequently, one of possible ways to investigate the source of individual differences in normal, non-pathological cognitive ageing is by ascertaining whether a polygenetic propensity to general cognition is also associated with the rate of cognitive change in adults using the PGS approach. Although a decline in cognitive performance in adults is part of the normative ageing process (Wilson et al., Reference Wilson, Beckett, Barnes, Schneider, Bach, Evans and Bennett2002), having a higher load of common genetic markers associated with general cognition would be expected to associate with a lower rate of cognitive decline over time. However, this question has not been investigated.

Due to their vital functionality, verbal memory and semantic fluency cognitive domains are important predictors of clinically significant cognitive decline in healthy older adults (Masur, Sliwinski, Lipton, Blau, & Crystal, Reference Masur, Sliwinski, Lipton, Blau and Crystal1994). Indeed, verbal memory is necessary for storing and processing internally held information for use in guiding behaviours (Kofler et al., Reference Kofler, Rapport, Bolden, Sarver, Raiker and Alderson2011). Whereas semantic fluency is arguably the most complex aspect of one's cognitive capacities. This is because semantic fluency is necessary for everyday living, such as attention, initiation, mental flexibility, organisation, abstract thinking, planning and problem-solving, all of which are required to implement behavioural responses appropriate to a constantly changing world (Kofler et al., Reference Kofler, Rapport, Bolden, Sarver, Raiker and Alderson2011). Because these cognitive domains have varying functionalities, it is feasible that a polygenic predisposition to general cognition may be associated with a slower decline in these cognitive domains to varying degree. There is further evidence suggesting that age and gender differences exist in the cognitive ageing processes (Deary et al., Reference Deary, Corley, Gow, Harris, Houlihan, Marioni and Starr2009; Weber, Skirbekk, Freund, & Herlitz, Reference Weber, Skirbekk, Freund and Herlitz2014). For example, earlier twin studies highlighted that boys had a higher heritability for a verbal measure of cognitive ability compared to girls (Galsworthy, Dionne, Dale, & Plomin, Reference Galsworthy, Dionne, Dale and Plomin2000). More recent research in turn highlighted that this genetic propensity to a higher verbal measure of cognitive ability in men extends to a later part of life (Kępińska et al., Reference Kępińska, MacCabe, Cadar, Steptoe, Murray and Ajnakina2020). However, the questions of the polygenic influence underlying these cognitive domains, and if their impact differs across genders and age groups, have not been investigated.

Despite some uncertainty about their ultimate clinical utility (Wray et al., Reference Wray, Yang, Hayes, Price, Goddard and Visscher2013), PGSs can advance our knowledge of the genetic nature underlying rate of change in verbal memory and semantic fluency cognitive domains in adults. Therefore, we investigated whether a higher load of common genetic markers for general cognition associated with higher cognitive domains at baseline and with a deaccelerating decline in cognitive domains over the 10-year follow-up in the general population of adults. Assuming a variation in cognitive domains is a function of the degree of genetic liability to general cognition, we hypothesised that a PGS for general cognition would be significantly associated with a higher score in verbal memory and semantic fluency at baseline, and a lesser rate of decline in these cognitive domains during follow-up in health adults. Given age and gender differences in cognitive functions and cognitive ageing (Kępińska et al., Reference Kępińska, MacCabe, Cadar, Steptoe, Murray and Ajnakina2020; Weber et al., Reference Weber, Skirbekk, Freund and Herlitz2014), we also investigated whether the potential relationships of a PGS for general cognition with cognition at baseline and during follow-up in healthy adults differed by age and gender.

Methods

Sample

We analysed data from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA), which is an ongoing large, multidisciplinary study of a nationally representative sample of the English adults aged ⩾50 years old (Steptoe, Breeze, Banks, & Nazroo, Reference Steptoe, Breeze, Banks and Nazroo2013). The ELSA study started in 2002–2003 (wave 1) with participants recruited from the Health Survey for England (HSE), who were then followed-up every 2 years. The sample is periodically refreshed with younger participants to ensure that the full age spectrum is maintained. Comparisons of the ELSA sample with the national census showed that the baseline sample was representative of the non-institutionalised general population aged ⩾50 living in the UK (Steptoe et al., Reference Steptoe, Breeze, Banks and Nazroo2013). For the present study, the baseline data were obtained from either wave 2 (2004–2005) for the core members who started at wave 1, or wave 4 (2008–2009) for the participants joining the study through refreshment samples; the included participants took part in the blood draws during home visits by a nurse. Follow-up data were ascertained from waves 4 (2008–2009) to wave 8 (2016–2017). We excluded participants with pathological causes of cognitive decline, such as clinical stroke (including self-reported stroke) or dementia at baseline and throughout the follow-up waves of data collection; we have further moved responders who died during the follow-up period. Our final analytic sample comprised 5088 responders. The full process of sample selection is depicted in online Supplementary Fig. S1. Compared to those who were excluded, the ELSA participants who were included in the study tended to be younger, had higher educational attainment and higher accumulated wealth; the latter group also included a lower proportion of people with long-standing health conditions, severe depressive symptoms and smokers compared to the respondents who were excluded from this study (online Supplementary Table S1). Ethical approval for each of the ELSA waves was granted by the National Research Ethics Service (London Multicentre Research Ethics Committee). All participants gave informed consent.

Study variables

Cognition

To measure verbal memory, immediate and delayed verbal memory were assessed using a word-learning task, which entailed recalling as many out of 10 common words that were read out to them as possible immediately and after a short delay during which they completed other cognitive tests (Yin, Lassale, Steptoe, & Cadar, Reference Yin, Lassale, Steptoe and Cadar2019). Following the protocol of previous studies (Fancourt & Steptoe, Reference Fancourt and Steptoe2019), the results for immediate and delayed recall were then combined to give an overall verbal memory variable. Semantic fluency was measured with a verbal fluency test, where participants were asked to think of as many animal names as they could in one minute. The total number of animals named by participants was used as a measure of semantic fluency. Although this task primarily focused on semantic fluency, it combined various aspects of broader executive function including cognitive flexibility, processing speed, inhibitory control and verbal fluency. As the semantic fluency test was not administered at wave 6, there was an extended gap in the follow-up assessments of this cognitive domain during the follow-up. The distribution of these cognitive domains across all waves of data collection is provided in online Supplementary Table S2.

Covariates

Covariates include gender, year of birth and age; to capture the non-linear effects of ageing, we further included age2 as a covariate. Because the ɛ4 allele of the apolipoprotein E gene (APOE-ɛ4) has previously been associated with cognitive decline in normal ageing (Zhang & Pierce, Reference Zhang and Pierce2014), we adjusted our analyses for APOE-ɛ4. Consistent with previous research (Zhang, Zhao, Xu, & Jiang, Reference Zhang, Zhao, Xu and Jiang2018), APOE-ɛ4 status was defined according to the absence or presence of APOE alleles (ɛ2/4, ɛ3/4 and ɛ4/4). Lastly, genetic ancestry [as was measured with principal components (see below)) was included among the covariates to account for any ancestry differences in genetic structures that could bias our results (Price et al., Reference Price, Patterson, Plenge, Weinblatt, Shadick and Reich2006).

Genetic data

Quality control

The genome-wide genotyping was performed at University College London Genomics in 2013–2014 using the Illumina HumanOmni2.5 BeadChips (HumanOmni2.5-4v1, HumanOmni2.5-8v1.3) which measures ~2.5 million markers that capture the genomic variation down to 2.5% minor allele frequency. Using PLINK (Purcell et al., Reference Purcell, Neale, Todd-Brown, Thomas, Ferreira, Bender and Sham2007) and VCFtools (Danecek et al., Reference Danecek, Auton, Abecasis, Albers, Banks, DePristo and Durbin2011), SNPs were excluded if they were non-autosomal, the minor allele frequency was <1%, if more than 2% of genotype data were missing and if the Hardy–Weinberg Equilibrium p value <10−4. Samples were removed based on call rate (<0.99), suspected non-European ancestry, heterozygosity and relatedness; participants were further removed if theier recorded sex phenotype was inconsistent with genetic sex. The summary of full quality control employed in the ELSA study is provided in online Supplementary Fig. S2 and Table S3. We employed the principal components analysis (Price et al., Reference Price, Patterson, Plenge, Weinblatt, Shadick and Reich2006) to identify those individuals who deviated from European ancestry (i.e. ethnic outliers). This set of analyses demonstrated the presence of ancestral admixture in the 65 individuals, who were subsequently removed; individuals who self-reported to be of non-European ethnicity were also removed. Using the updated sample, we calculated principal component, which then was used to adjust for possible population stratification in the association analyses (Price et al., Reference Price, Patterson, Plenge, Weinblatt, Shadick and Reich2006; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Sun, Stang, Berlin, Wilcox and Li2009).

PGS analyses

PGS are calculated from the number of trait-associated alleles weighted by corresponding genotype effect sizes as estimated in a reference GWAS meta-analysis (Choi, Mak, & O'Reilly, Reference Choi, Mak and O'Reilly2020; Purcell et al., Reference Purcell, Wray, Stone, Visscher, O'Donovan, Sullivan and Sklar2009). Therefore, the predictive accuracy of PGS is dependent on the strength of the GWAS summary statistics. Currently, there are two GWASs that provided summary statistics for general cognition (Davies et al., Reference Davies, Lam, Harris, Trampush, Luciano, Hill and Deary2018; Savage et al., Reference Savage, Jansen, Stringer, Watanabe, Bryois, de Leeuw and Posthuma2018). The first was carried out by Davies et al. that combined individuals of European ancestry from 56 population-based cohorts brought together by the Cohorts for Heart and Aging Research in Genomic Epidemiology (CHARGE), the Cognitive Genomics Consortium (COGENT) consortia and UK Biobank (UKB) (Davies et al., Reference Davies, Lam, Harris, Trampush, Luciano, Hill and Deary2018); the second was carried out by Savage et al. that measured multiple dimensions of cognitive functioning across 14 cohorts including UKB and the COGENT (Savage et al., Reference Savage, Jansen, Stringer, Watanabe, Bryois, de Leeuw and Posthuma2018). To ascertain which of these summary statistics to employ, we calculated an expected predictive accuracy of GC-PGS based on the results of each GWAS using Avengeme package implemented in R (http://www.rstudio.com/). Specifically, using information on sample sizes utilised in each GWAS and in the present study (n ELSA = 6831, n Davies et al. (2018) = 300 486, n Savage et al. (2018) = 269 867), total number of independent markers in genotyping panel (m Davies et al. (2018) = 1 348 174 and m Savage et al. (2018) = 1 269 550), and lower and upper p values used to select markers into GC-PGS (p0, p1), our estimates showed that GC-PGS based on GWAS from Savage et al. (power = 0.504, p = 0.027) would have more power for the subsequent analyses compared to the predictive power of GC-PGS calculated based on summary statistics from Davies et al. (power = 0.386, p = 0.052). Using summary statistics provided by Savage et al., we calculated PGS for general cognition using PRSice based on genotyped data at different p value cut-offs (Euesden, Lewis, & O'Reilly, Reference Euesden, Lewis and O'Reilly2015). As it was highlighted that PGSs calculated based all available SNPs either explain the most amount of variation in a trait or are not significantly different than PGSs based on different p value thresholds, we utilised PGS at p value threshold of 1 (Ware et al., Reference Ware, Schmitz, Faul, Gard, Smith, Mitchell and Kardia2017). To aid the interpretability of the results, GC-PGS was standardised to a mean of 0 [standard deviation (s.d.) = 1].

Statistical analysis

To assess the relationships of PGSs with verbal memory and semantic fluency and to estimate the mean change in each of these cognitive domains over a period of 10 years, we employed linear mixed-effect models (LMMs) with maximum likelihood estimation (Kristjansson, Kircher, & Webb, Reference Kristjansson, Kircher and Webb2007). LMMs are flexible analytic tools for modelling correlated continuous data where correlations among values on continuous dependent variables arise from repeated measurements (West & Galecki, Reference West and Galecki2012). LMM models further maximise the use of longitudinal data by weighing estimates for missing data between waves and as such increase statistical power and precision (Mallinckrodt, Clark, & David, Reference Mallinckrodt, Clark and David2001). The maximum likelihood estimation aspect of LMMs seeks to find parameter values that make the model's predicted values most similar to the observed values (Martínez-Huertas, Olmos, & Ferrer, Reference Martínez-Huertas, Olmos and Ferrer2021). As LMMs have been developed to capture individual differences, while at the same time allowing generalisations across populations (Martínez-Huertas et al., Reference Martínez-Huertas, Olmos and Ferrer2021), we further captured random or stochastic variability in the data that comes from participants by estimating variance. Having considered linear, quadratic and cubic LMMs, Akaike Information Criterion and Bayesian Information Criterion (George, Seals, & Aban, Reference George, Seals and Aban2014; Royston & Parmar, Reference Royston and Parmar2002) showed that the linear model was the most appropriate for our analyses. We first fitted a model adjusted for age, sex, year of birth and genetic ancestry as measured with 10 principal components (we referred to this model as Model 1). Next, to test for incremental predictive validity over APOE-ɛ4, which is a well-established risk factor for rapid cognitive decline, we fitted a second model which included all covariates from Model 1 in addition to APOE-ɛ4 (we referred to this model as Model 2). Interaction between GC-PGS and year of birth in the analyses of each cognitive domain was shown to be non-significant; thus, these interactions were not included in the models. For the age-stratified analyses, age groups were formed based on tertile results (i.e. 50–57, 58–65 and ⩾66 years). All analyses were weighted for non-response to requests for blood collections to wave 2 for the core members, or wave 4 for the participants who joined the study at wave 4 through the refreshment sample. Due to multiple testing, Bonferroni correction was applied (Benjamini & Hochberg, Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995), and significance was indicated within each model if p < 0.004 (p = 0.05/12). All association analyses were conducted in STATA release 14 (STATA Corp LP, USA).

Results

Sample characteristics

The baseline demographic and health characteristics of the total sample are presented in Table 1. The analytic sample comprised 5088 individuals with an average age for the entire cohort of 61.7 years old (s.d. = 7.4, median = 60, IQR = 56–67); of these, n = 1812 (35.6%) were aged 50–57 years old, n = 1745 (34.3%) were aged 58–65 years old, and n = 1531 (30.1%) were ⩾66 years old. Of the entire analytic sample, 24.8% (n = 1260) were APOE-ɛ4 carriers and 44.2% (n = 2250) were men. The average baseline memory score was 11.1 (s.d. = 2.9) and executive function score was 21.5 (s.d. = 5.8).

Table 1. Sample characteristics at baseline

APOE-ɛ4, two ɛ4 alleles of the apolipoprotein E gene; s.d., standard deviation.

Change in cognition over the 10-year follow-up

A significant decline in verbal memory (β = −0.13, 95% CI −0.15 to −0.12, p < 0.001) and semantic fluency (β = −0.31, 95% CI −0.36 to −0.25, p < 0.001) was observed during the follow-up period of 10 years (Fig. 1, online Supplementary Table S2). As further depicted in Fig. 1, a comparable decline in these cognitive domains was observed in men and women. While the decline in verbal memory and semantic fluency scores was comparable between the adults who were aged 50–65 years of age, the most striking decline was observed in adults who were aged ⩾66 years old.

Fig. 1. The average distribution of the verbal memory and semantic fluency cognitive domains across all waves of data collection over the 10-year follow up for the entire sample, different age groups and genders.

PGS for general cognition and verbal memory

An increase in GC-PGS by one standard deviation (1-s.d.) was associated with a higher baseline verbal memory by an average 0.27 points (Model 2: 95% CI 0.19–0.34, p < 0.001) (Table 2) indicating that a higher GC-PGS the adults had higher they scored on verbal memory test. This association was significant for women (β model 2 = 0.23, 95% CI 0.14–0.33, p < 0.001) and men (β model 2 = 0.32, 95% CI 0.21–0.42, p < 0.001). In the age-stratified analyses, 1-s.d. increase in GC-PGS was associated with the higher baseline verbal memory score by approximately 0.31 points in adults who were aged ⩾66 years old (Model 2: 95% CI 0.17–0.45, p = 0.001), 0.29 points in adults aged 50–57 years (Model 2: 95% CI 0.17–0.41, p < 0.001) and 0.19 points in adults who were aged 50–57 years older (Model 2: 95% CI 0.07–0.31, p = 0.001). Nonetheless, 1-s.d. increase in GC-PGS was not associated with decreases in verbal memory during follow-up in the entire sample, as well stratified models by sex and age.

Table 2. Associations between polygenic score for general cognition (GC-PGS) and longitudinal measure of verbal memory in older adults over the 10-year follow-up

The presented β are standardised; CI, confidence intervals.

Model 1 is adjusted for baseline age, gender, year of birth and 10 principal components to account for any ancestry differences in genetic structures that could bias; Model 2 is adjusted for baseline age, gender, year of birth, APOE-ɛ4 and 10 principal components to account for any ancestry differences in genetic structures that could bias.

a The estimated variance captured random or stochastic variability in the data that comes from participants, here showing that there were significant individual differences in measure of verbal memory over time.

PGS for general cognition and semantic fluency

A 1-s.d. increase in GC-PGS was associated with a higher semantic fluency score at baseline in the entire sample (β model 2 = 0.45, 95% CI 0.27–0.64, p < 0.001) and separately in women (β model 2 = 0.40, 95% CI 0.17–0.63, p = 0.001) and men (β model 2 = 0.54, 95% CI 0.24–0.83, p < 0.001) (Table 3). In the age-stratified analyses, 1-s.d. increase in GC-PGS was associated with a higher baseline semantic fluency score in adults who were aged 50–57 years old (β model 2 = 0.46, 95% CI 0.17–0.75, p = 0.002), and in adults aged 58–65 years (β model 2 = 0.54, 95% CI 0.22–0.86, p = 0.001). During the 10-year follow-up, 1-s.d. increase in GC-PGS was not associated with decreases in semantic fluency score in the entire sample and in stratified models.

Table 3. Associations between polygenic score general cognition (GC-PGS) and longitudinal measure of semantic fluency in older adults over the 10-year follow-up

The presented β are standardised; CI, confidence intervals.

Model 1 is adjusted for baseline age, gender, year of birth and 10 principal components to account for any ancestry differences in genetic structures that could bias; Model 2 is adjusted for baseline age, gender, year of birth, APOE-ɛ4 and 10 principal components to account for any ancestry differences in genetic structures that could bias.

a The estimated variance captured random or stochastic variability in the data that comes from participants, here showing that there were significant individual differences in measure of semantic fluency over time.

*These results did not survive Bonferroni correction for multiple testing (p = 0.05/12 = p < 0.004).

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study to have investigated the relationships of a polygenic predisposition for general cognition, as measured with the PGS approach, with individual differences in verbal memory and semantic fluency cognition domain at baseline and change in each of these cognitive domains over a period of 10 years independently of APOE-ɛ4 status in a large population-representative sample of adults.

Our results showed that a higher load of nominally associated loci for general cognition was associated with better verbal memory and semantic fluency in adults at baseline. All effect sizes for these significant effects ranged from modest (the standardised βs = 0.19 for verbal memory) to moderate (the standardised βs = 0.46 for semantic fluency). These results in turn highlight that individual differences in these cognitive domains are influenced by an individual load of common genetic markers associated with general cognition. It was previously argued that men aged 44–88 years old might differ from women of the similar age in their cognitive capacity (Finkel, Reynolds, Berg, & Pedersen, Reference Finkel, Reynolds, Berg and Pedersen2006), possibly due to differences in the prevalence of neuronal efficiency (Payton, Reference Payton2009). Our results, however, demonstrated that these potential gender differences could not be explained by a polygenic predisposition to general cognition. In contrast to the results from cross-sectional samples that appeared to lend support for the notion that the heritability of cognitive functions decreases as adults age (Lee, Henry, Trollor, & Sachdev, Reference Lee, Henry, Trollor and Sachdev2010), we showed that the additive contribution of common genetic markers for general cognition were significantly associated with higher baseline verbal memory and semantic fluency scores across all age groups.

Although a significant decline in verbal memory and semantic fluency was observed over the follow-up time, in contrast to our hypothesis, common genetic variants associated with general cognition additively were not associated with a greater rate of decline in verbal memory and semantic fluency during the 10-year follow-up in adults from the general population. Employing PGS for educational attainment, neuroticism, Alzheimer's disease, major psychiatric disorders and physical health, Ritchie et al. showed that there were no statistically significantly associations between PGS and variation in cognitive change between ages 11 and 70–79 in the longitudinal Lothian Birth Cohort 1936 study (Ritchie et al., Reference Ritchie, Hill, Marioni, Davies, Hagenaars, Harris and Deary2019). Our results extend these findings by showing that a PGS for general cognition also was not associated with cognitive domains in adults aged 50 years old and older. Our results are further aligned with other longitudinal studies which, having investigated determinants of the rate of cognitive decline on various cognitive domains over a 5-year (Reynolds, Finkel, Gatz, & Pedersen, Reference Reynolds, Finkel, Gatz and Pedersen2002), 8-year (McGue & Christensen, Reference McGue and Christensen2002) and even a 16-year follow-up (Finkel, Reynolds, McArdle, Hamagami, & Pedersen, Reference Finkel, Reynolds, McArdle, Hamagami and Pedersen2009), concluded the rate of cognitive decline in the general population is primarily driven by environmental factors.

The observed non-significant association between the genetic influences and cognitive change may also reflect attrition effects, which are unavoidable in longitudinal cohorts. Similarly, because the results presented in the study are based on longitudinal study with prospectively collected data, collider bias may have contributed to this finding (Arnold et al., Reference Arnold, Davies, de Kamps, Tennant, Mbotwa and Gilthorpe2021), which might have arisen from selection bias or attrition. Even though comparisons with the national census showed that the ELSA sample was representative of the non-institutionalised general population aged ⩾50 residing in the UK (Steptoe et al., Reference Steptoe, Breeze, Banks and Nazroo2013), to ensure we completely minimised any issues related to the selection bias we used inverse probability weighting in our models (Hernán, Hsu, & Brian, Reference Hernán, Hsu and Brian2019). In terms of attrition, the proportion of missingness in the present study was comparable to many longitudinal cohorts (Ajnakina et al., Reference Ajnakina, Agbedjro, McCammon, Faul, Murray, Stahl and Steptoe2021; Power & Elliott, Reference Power and Elliott2006; Sonnega et al., Reference Sonnega, Faul, Ofstedal, Langa, Phillips and Weir2014); we further imputed missing values using robust approaches (Ajnakina et al., Reference Ajnakina, Agbedjro, McCammon, Faul, Murray, Stahl and Steptoe2021; Oba et al., Reference Oba, Sato, Takemasa, Monden, Matsubara and Ishii2003; Stekhoven & Bühlmann, Reference Stekhoven and Bühlmann2012). Therefore, it is unlikely that attrition or selection bias influenced our results. It is nevertheless possible that only a subset of the genetic factors for general cognition may still have an impact on individual differences in non-pathological, age-associated change in semantic fluency, which, due to the nature of the PGS approach, might not have been captured in the present study. Therefore, further analyses, such as pathway-specific PGS analyses, genomic structural equation modelling and gene-set enrichment analyses, may be needed before we can draw more precise conclusions of the role common genetic loci may play in the rate of cognitive ageing.

Methodological considerations

We analysed a large population-based cohort of nationally representative of older adults in England who were followed-up every 2 years. We further benefitted from the availability of repeated measures of cognition across a 10-year span. Our study included a relatively equal proportion of women and men from socio-economically diverse backgrounds. Confidence in these findings is strengthened using a LMM, which is an optimal way to describe the change in continuous dependent variables over time all the while taking the intra-individual and inter-individual variation into account. Instead of using composite scores for broad cognitive functions, we explored verbal memory and semantic fluency separately (Szoke et al., Reference Szoke, Trandafir, Dupont, Meary, Schurhoff and Leboyer2008).

Nonetheless, several methodological limitations warrant further reflection and discussion. Although PGSs have the potential to improve health outcomes through their eventual routine implementation as clinical biomarkers, the low generalisability of genetic studies across populations is noteworthy (Martin et al., Reference Martin, Kanai, Kamatani, Okada, Neale and Daly2019). This is because the construction of PGSs is mainly dependent on the availability of the summary statistics from GWASs, which are currently predominately based on European participants (Martin et al., Reference Martin, Kanai, Kamatani, Okada, Neale and Daly2019). It may be argued that the non-significant findings of the present study might have arisen due to the cognitive measures employed not being sufficiently sensitive to detect changes in cognitive status over the follow-up period of 10 years. However, having utilised the same sample, it was previously shown that APOE-ɛ4 (Kępińska et al., Reference Kępińska, MacCabe, Cadar, Steptoe, Murray and Ajnakina2020) and loneliness in older people contributed to a decline in these cognitive domains over the 10-year follow-up period (Yin et al., Reference Yin, Lassale, Steptoe and Cadar2019). This in turn demonstrates that the ELSA sample had the necessary sensitivity to detect changes in cognition over time.

Conclusion

Drawing on a large, phenotypically well-defined sample of population-representative adults, we demonstrated that common genetic variants associated with general cognition additively are associated with a stable surplus to cognition in adults. However, our results showed that a polygenic predisposition to general cognition is not associated with age-related cognitive decline during a 10-year follow-up reiterating previous assertations that the rate of cognitive decline in the general population may be primarily driven by environmental factors.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721004827.

Acknowledgements

The English Longitudinal Study of Ageing is funded by the National Institute on Aging (grant RO1AG7644) and by a consortium of UK government departments coordinated by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR). OA is further funded by an NIHR Post-Doctoral Fellowship (PDF-2018-11-ST2-020). RM is supported by the NIHR Maudsley BRC. DC is funded by the National Institute on Aging (Grant R01AG017644) and Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC, Grant ES/T014091/1). The views expressed in this publication are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the National Institute for Health Research or the Department of Health and Social Care.

Conflict of interest

RM has received honoraria from Janssen, Sunovian, Lundbeck and Otsuka. All other authors declare no conflict of interest.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Ajnakina, O., Agbedjro, D., McCammon, R., Faul, J., Murray, R. M., Stahl, D., & Steptoe, A. (2021). Development and validation of prediction model to estimate 10-year risk of all-cause mortality using modern statistical learning methods: A large population-based cohort study and external validation. BMC Medical Research Methodology 21, 8. doi: 10.1186/s12874-020-01204-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnold, K. F., Davies, V., de Kamps, M., Tennant, P. W. G., Mbotwa, J., & Gilthorpe, M. S. (2021). Reflection on modern methods: Generalized linear models for prognosis and intervention-theory, practice and implications for machine learning. International Journal of Epidemiology 49, 20742082.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society 57(1), 289300.Google Scholar
Choi, S. W., Mak, T. S. H., & O'Reilly, P. F. (2020). Tutorial: A guide to performing polygenic risk score analyses. Nature Protocols 15 (9), 27592772.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Danecek, P., Auton, A., Abecasis, G., Albers, C. A., Banks, E., DePristo, M. A, … Durbin, R. (2011). The variant call format and VCFtools. Bioinformatics 27, 21562158. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btr330CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davies, G., Lam, M., Harris, S. E., Trampush, J. W., Luciano, M., Hill, , … Deary, I. J. (2018). Study of 300,486 individuals identifies 148 independent genetic loci influencing general cognitive function. Nature Communications 9, 20982114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deary, I. J. (2013). Intelligence. Current Biology 23, 673676. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.07.021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deary, I. J., Corley, J., Gow, A. J., Harris, S. E., Houlihan, L. M., Marioni, R. E., … Starr, J. M. (2009). Age-associated cognitive decline. British Medical Bulletin 92, 135152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dudbridge, F. (2013). Power and predictive accuracy of polygenic risk scores. PLoS Genetics 9(3), 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Euesden, J., Lewis, C. M., & O'Reilly, P. F. (2015). PRSice: Polygenic Risk Score software. Bioinformatics 31, 14661468. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btu848CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fancourt, D., & Steptoe, A. (2019). Television viewing and cognitive decline in older age: Findings from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Scientific Reports 9, 28512859.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finkel, D., Reynolds, C. A., Berg, S., & Pedersen, N. L. (2006). Surprising lack of sex differences in normal cognitive aging in twins. The International Journal of Aging and Human Development 62, 335357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finkel, D., Reynolds, C. A., McArdle, J. J., Hamagami, F., & Pedersen, N. L. (2009). Genetic variance in processing speed drives variation in aging of spatial and memory abilities. Developmental Psychology 45, 820834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galsworthy, M. J., Dionne, G., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2000). Sex differences in early verbal and non-verbal cognitive development. Developmental Science 3, 206215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
George, B., Seals, S., & Aban, I. (2014). Survival analysis and regression models. Journal of Nuclear Cardiology 21, 686694.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haworth, C. M., Wright, M. J., Luciano, M., Martin, N. G., de Geus, E. J., van Beijsterveldt, C. E., … Plomin, R. (2010). The heritability of general cognitive ability increases linearly from childhood to young adulthood. Molecular Psychiatry 15, 11121120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hernán, M. A., Hsu, J., & Brian, H. (2019). A second chance to get causal inference right: A classification of data science tasks. Chance 32, 4249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kępińska, A. P., MacCabe, J. H., Cadar, D., Steptoe, A., Murray, R. M., & Ajnakina, O. (2020). Schizophrenia polygenic risk predicts general cognitive deficit but not cognitive decline in healthy older adults. Translational Psychiatry 10(422), 19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kofler, M. J., Rapport, M. D., Bolden, J., Sarver, D. E., Raiker, J. S., & Alderson, R. M. (2011). Working memory deficits and social problems in children with ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 39, 805817.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kristjansson, S. D., Kircher, J. C., & Webb, A. K. (2007). Multilevel models for repeated measures research designs in psychophysiology: An introduction to growth curve modeling. Psychophysiology 44, 728736.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, T., Henry, J. D., Trollor, J. N., & Sachdev, P. S. (2010). Genetic influences on cognitive functions in the elderly: A selective review of twin studies. Brain Research Reviews 64, 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mallinckrodt, C. H., Clark, W. S., & David, S. R. (2001). Accounting for dropout bias using mixed-effects models. Journal of Biopharmaceutical Statistics 11, 921.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, A. R., Kanai, M., Kamatani, Y., Okada, Y., Neale, B. M., & Daly, M. J. (2019). Clinical use of current polygenic risk scores may exacerbate health disparities. Nature Genetics 51, 584591.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martínez-Huertas, J., Olmos, R., & Ferrer, E. (2021). Model selection and model averaging for mixed-effects models with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 117. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2021.1889946Google ScholarPubMed
Masur, D. M., Sliwinski, M., Lipton, R. B., Blau, A. D., & Crystal, H. A. (1994). Neuropsychological prediction of dementia and the absence of dementia in healthy elderly persons. Neurology 44, 14271432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGue, M., & Christensen, K. (2002). The heritability of level and rate-of-change in cognitive functioning in Danish twins aged 70 years and older. Experimental Aging Research 28, 435451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oba, S., Sato, M. A., Takemasa, I., Monden, M., Matsubara, K., & Ishii, S. (2003). A Bayesian missing value estimation method for gene expression profile data. Bioinformatics 19, 20882096.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Payton, A. (2009). The impact of genetic research on our understanding of normal cognitive ageing: 1995 to 2009. Neuropsychology Review 19, 451477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plomin, R., & Deary, I. J. (2015). Genetics and intelligence differences: Five special findings. Molecular Psychiatry 20, 98108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plomin, R., & Kovas, Y. (2005). Generalist genes and learning disabilities. Psychological Bulletin 131, 592617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Power, C., & Elliott, J. (2006). Cohort profile: 1958 British birth cohort (National Child Development Study). International Journal of Epidemiology 35, 3441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, A. L., Patterson, N. J., Plenge, R. M., Weinblatt, M. E., Shadick, N. A., & Reich, D. (2006). Principal components analysis corrects for stratification in genome-wide association studies. Nature Genetics 38, 904909.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purcell, S., Neale, B., Todd-Brown, K., Thomas, L., Ferreira, M. A., Bender, D., … Sham, P. C. (2007). PLINK: A tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. American Journal of Human Genetics 81, 559575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purcell, S. M., Wray, N. R., Stone, J. L., Visscher, P. M., O'Donovan, M. C., Sullivan, P. F., & Sklar, P. (2009). Common polygenic variation contributes to risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nature 460, 748752.Google ScholarPubMed
Rajan, K. B., Wilson, R. S., Weuve, J., Barnes, L. L., & Evans, D. A. (2015). Cognitive impairment 18 years before clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer disease dementia. Neurology 85, 898904.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reynolds, C. A., Finkel, D., Gatz, M., & Pedersen, N. L. (2002). Sources of influence on rate of cognitive change over time in Swedish twins: An application of latent growth models. Experimental Aging Research 28, 407433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ritchie, S. J., Hill, W. D., Marioni, R. E., Davies, G., Hagenaars, S. P., Harris, S. E., … Deary, I. J. (2019). Polygenic predictors of age-related decline in cognitive ability. Molecular Psychiatry 25, 25842598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Royston, P., & Parmar, M. K. (2002). Flexible parametric proportional-hazards and proportional-odds models for censored survival data, with application to prognostic modelling and estimation of treatment effects. Statistics in Medicine 21, 21752197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savage, J. E., Jansen, P. R., Stringer, S., Watanabe, K., Bryois, J., de Leeuw, C. A., … Posthuma, D. (2018). Genome-wide association meta-analysis in 269,867 individuals identifies new genetic and functional links to intelligence. Nature Genetics 50, 912919.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sonnega, A., Faul, J. D., Ofstedal, M. B., Langa, K. M., Phillips, J. W., & Weir, D. R. (2014). Cohort profile: The health and retirement study (HRS). International Journal of Epidemiology 43, 576585.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stekhoven, D. J., & Bühlmann, P. (2012). MissForest – non-parametric missing value imputation for mixed-type data. Bioinformatics 28, 112118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steptoe, A., Breeze, E., Banks, J., & Nazroo, J. (2013). Cohort profile: The English longitudinal study of ageing. International Journal of Epidemiology 42, 16401648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szoke, A., Trandafir, A., Dupont, M. E., Meary, A., Schurhoff, F., & Leboyer, M. (2008). Longitudinal studies of cognition in schizophrenia: Meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry 192, 248257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tucker-Drob, E. M., Briley, D. A., & Harden, K. P. (2013). Genetic and environmental influences on cognition across development and context. Current Directions in Psychological Science 22, 349355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, D., Sun, Y., Stang, P., Berlin, J. A., Wilcox, M. A., & Li, Q. (2009). Comparison of methods for correcting population stratification in a genome-wide association study of rheumatoid arthritis: Principal-component analysis versus multidimensional scaling. BMC Proceedings 3, 16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ware, E. B., Schmitz, L. L., Faul, J. D., Gard, A. M., Smith, J. A., Mitchell, C. M., … Kardia, S. L. R. (2017). Heterogeneity in polygenic scores for common human traits. Preprint at https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/106062v1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weber, D., Skirbekk, V., Freund, I., & Herlitz, A. (2014). The changing face of cognitive gender differences in Europe. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 111, 1167311678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
West, B. T., & Galecki, A. T. (2012). An overview of current software procedures for fitting linear mixed models. American Statistical Association 65, 274282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, R. S., Beckett, L. A., Barnes, L. L., Schneider, J. A., Bach, J., Evans, D. A., & Bennett, D. A. (2002). Individual differences in rates of change in cognitive abilities of older persons. Psychology and Aging 17, 179193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, R. S., Beckett, L. A., Bienias, J. L., Evans, D. A., & Bennett, D. A. (2003). Terminal decline in cognitive function. Neurology 60, 17821787.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wray, N. R., Yang, J., Hayes, B. J., Price, A. L., Goddard, M. E., & Visscher, P. M. (2013). Pitfalls of predicting complex traits from SNPs. Nature Reviews Genetics 14, 507515.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yin, J., Lassale, C., Steptoe, A., & Cadar, D. (2019). Exploring the bidirectional associations between loneliness and cognitive functioning over 10 years: The English longitudinal study of ageing. International Journal of Epidemiology 48(6), 19371948.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, A., Zhao, Q., Xu, D., & Jiang, S. (2018). Brain APOE expression quantitative trait loci-based association study identified one susceptibility locus for Alzheimer's disease by interacting with APOE epsilon4. Scientific Reports 8, 80688072.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, C., & Pierce, B. L. (2014). Genetic susceptibility to accelerated cognitive decline in the US Health and Retirement Study. Neurobiology of Aging 35, 1512.e11–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Sample characteristics at baseline

Figure 1

Fig. 1. The average distribution of the verbal memory and semantic fluency cognitive domains across all waves of data collection over the 10-year follow up for the entire sample, different age groups and genders.

Figure 2

Table 2. Associations between polygenic score for general cognition (GC-PGS) and longitudinal measure of verbal memory in older adults over the 10-year follow-up

Figure 3

Table 3. Associations between polygenic score general cognition (GC-PGS) and longitudinal measure of semantic fluency in older adults over the 10-year follow-up

Supplementary material: File

Ajnakina et al. supplementary material

Figures S1-S2 and Tables S1-S3

Download Ajnakina et al. supplementary material(File)
File 201.1 KB