Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T06:53:56.281Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Encouraging vaccination against COVID-19 has no compensatory spillover effects

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 March 2024

Ploutarchos Kourtidis*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Behavioural Science, London School of Economics and Political Science, London, UK
Barbara Fasolo
Affiliation:
Department of Management, London School of Economics and Political Science, London, UK
Matteo M. Galizzi
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Behavioural Science, London School of Economics and Political Science, London, UK
*
Corresponding author: Ploutarchos Kourtidis; Email: k.ploutarchos@lse.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Effective communication is essential for delivering public health messages and enabling behaviour change. Little is known about possible backfiring, or spillover effects, of COVID-19 vaccine messaging. In a study with n = 1,848 United Kingdom (UK) adults, we assess whether communication strategies that target vaccine hesitancy have any unintended, positive or negative, spillover effects on people's intention to engage in protective, compliance and prosocial behaviours. In June–July 2021, we conducted an online experiment to assess the potential spillover effects of three messages, emphasising (a) the medical benefits of COVID-19 vaccination, (b) the non-medical collective benefits of vaccination or (c) the non-medical individual benefits of holding a vaccination certificate. Exposure to different messages did not significantly affect people's intention to engage in protective, compliance, or prosocial behaviours. Instead, vaccination status (being vaccinated vs not) was positively associated with intentions to engage in protective, compliance and prosocial behaviours. Our results suggest that communication strategies that aim to increase vaccination uptake do not have any unintended effects on other health behaviours and vaccination campaigns can be tailored to specific populations to increase uptake and compliance.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Effective communication is a key non-pharmaceutical intervention (NPI) to protect people against health-related threats and misinformation about diseases (Islam et al., Reference Islam2020; Pennycook et al., Reference Pennycook2020; Lorna, Reference Lorna2021; World Health Organization, 2021). In the last few years, several frameworks have highlighted the importance of effective communication and the main elements that constitute such an intervention (European Centre for Disease Prevention and Control, 2012; World Health Organization, 2017; Jones et al., Reference Jones2019; Hyland-Wood et al., Reference Hyland-Wood2021). In the context of the COVID-19 pandemic, studies have tested the direct effects of different communication strategies on people's attitudes and behaviours, including vaccine hesitancy (Freeman et al., Reference Freeman2021; Merkley and Loewen, Reference Merkley and Loewen2021; Palm et al., Reference Palm, Bolsen and Kingsland2021; Steinert et al., Reference Steinert2022), support of vaccination certificate (Sotis et al., Reference Sotis2021), compliance (Kostopoulou and Schwartz, Reference Kostopoulou and Schwartz2021; Krpan and Dolan, Reference Krpan and Dolan2022), protective behaviours (Ahn et al., Reference Ahn, Hu and Vega2021; Gantiva et al., Reference Gantiva, Jiménez-Leal and Urriago-Rayo2021; Heffner et al., Reference Heffner, Vives and FeldmanHall2021; Vacondio et al., Reference Vacondio2021; Vereen et al., Reference Vereen2021), psychological reactance (Krpan and Dolan, Reference Krpan and Dolan2022) and perception of symptoms (Kostopoulou and Schwartz, Reference Kostopoulou and Schwartz2021). However, the indirect spillover effects of vaccine communication strategies have not yet been systematically explored.

The present study attempts to systematically explore whether information campaigns about COVID-19 vaccination have any unintended spillover effects on other protective, compliance or prosocial behaviours, which are critical to avoiding infection even in vaccinated populations (Howard et al., Reference Howard2021; MacIntyre et al., Reference MacIntyre, Costantino and Chanmugam2021). We study these questions by launching a second round of data collection in the UK during June–July 2021, immediately after the end of a randomised controlled experiment conducted in eight European countries, including the UK (Steinert et al., Reference Steinert2022). Referred to as “wave 1” in our study, that initial experiment involved the provision of information related to COVID-19 vaccination in three messages, each emphasising different medical and non-medical benefits of vaccination.

The first message contained information about the health benefits and the efficacy of COVID-19 vaccines, such as a reduction in risk of death and hospitalisation. The second message contained information about the collective, non-medical, benefits of vaccination including access to leisure activities and social events. The last message emphasised the exclusive, non-medical, benefits of holding a vaccination certificate, such as the ability to travel to other countries. The results of wave 1 showed that vaccine hesitancy in the UK ranged from 8.27 to 10.03% for men and women, respectively. Furthermore, during the period of data collection (i.e., summer 2021), the UK had already rolled out a large vaccination programme, resulting in two-thirds of all UK adults being double vaccinated by the end of July 2021 (GOV.UK, 2022b), while restrictions were still in place. Critically, the study revealed that none of the three messages increased participants’ willingness to get vaccinated consistently across the eight countries. In the present study, we investigate whether these communication strategies spill over to other behaviours.

Behavioural spillovers can be broadly defined as the effects of an intervention on a non-targeted behaviour (e.g., advertising a product can increase sales of another product) (Sahni, Reference Sahni2016). More specifically, behavioural spillovers refer to two sequential but different behaviours linked by an ‘underlying motive’ or a common purpose (e.g., environmental sustainability); they have been classified into four categories: permitting, promoting, purging and precipitating (Dolan and Galizzi, Reference Dolan and Galizzi2015; Galizzi and Whitmarsh, Reference Galizzi and Whitmarsh2019). Briefly, permitting (or compensatory) spillovers refer to people's perceived entitlement to disengage from an initial motive, given that this motive has been already served (e.g., saving energy at home could ‘licence’ compensatory overconsumption of energy at work). Promoting spillovers refers to people's continuous engagement with a motive (e.g., saving energy at home could ‘promote’ energy saving at work). Purging spillovers refer to people's tendency to engage in a subsequent behaviour to ‘purge’ themselves from a prior behaviour that did not serve the initial motive (e.g., overconsumption of energy at home could lead to energy saving at work). Finally, precipitating spillovers refer to people's disengagement from the initial motive all along (e.g., overconsumption of energy at home could perversely ‘promote’ the overconsumption of energy at work).

Besides the behavioural spillovers proposed by Dolan and Galizzi (Reference Dolan and Galizzi2015), the lateral attitude change (LAC) framework has been proposed by Glaser et al. (Reference Glaser2015) to explain how indirect attitudinal change happens. Briefly, the LAC framework suggests that a change in a focal attitude towards a topic or behaviour (e.g., vaccination) can be transferred (or ‘generalised’, in the authors’ words) to other related attitudes (e.g., protective behaviours). Alternatively, the framework further suggests that (peripheral) attitudes related to a focal attitude can change, while the focal attitude remains stable (what the authors call ‘displacement effect’).

Before the first COVID-19 vaccines were approved, a few – including World Health Organization (2020) – hinted at potential negative spillover effects of mask-wearing on other protective behaviours, such as a reduction in people's compliance with other COVID-19 rules (Lazzarino et al., Reference Lazzarino2020; Martin et al., Reference Martin2020; Trogen and Caplan, Reference Trogen and Caplan2021), although other researchers challenged those concerns (Greenhalgh et al., Reference Greenhalgh2020; Mantzari et al., Reference Mantzari, Rubin and Marteau2020). Later in the pandemic, with vaccines publicly available, there were some concerns that vaccinated people could develop a sense of ‘invulnerability’ that would ‘licence’ them to engage in risky behaviours, such as large gatherings and regular travelling. These speculations were based on risk compensation, an instantiation of permitting spillover that posits that people tend to adapt their behaviour relative to their perceived level of risk (e.g., wearing a seatbelt can lead to speeding, as it could alter people's sense of safety or perceived risk of speeding) (Peltzman, Reference Peltzman1975; Asch et al., Reference Asch1991; Evans and Graham, Reference Evans and Graham1991; Houston and Richardson, Reference Houston and Richardson2007).

Evidence of risk compensation in the context of COVID-19 behaviours is scarce. A UK study showed that face mask usage led to reduced physical distancing (Luckman et al., Reference Luckman2021). Likewise, a study in the United States (USA) showed that face mask mandates were associated with increased mobility in public places (Yan et al., Reference Yan2021). Similar results were found in France (Cartaud et al., Reference Cartaud, Quesque and Coello2020; Aranguren, Reference Aranguren2022) and Bangladesh (Wadud et al., Reference Wadud, Rahman and Enam2022). In contrast, studies in Germany and the UK showed that face mask usage increased physical distancing (Seres et al., Reference Seres2021) and other COVID-19 protective behaviours (Guenther et al., Reference Guenther, Galizzi and Sanders2021). Similar results were reported in Italy (Marchiori, Reference Marchiori2020), China (Sun et al., Reference Sun2022) and the Netherlands (Liebst et al., Reference Liebst2022), while a study in Denmark showed mixed results (Jørgensen et al., Reference Jørgensen2021). Finally, a study looking at financial incentives to take the first dose of a COVID-19 vaccine in Sweden found no negative unintended consequences of financial incentives on future vaccination uptake, morals, trust and perceived safety (Schneider et al., Reference Schneider2023). The same research further showed that not only financial incentives but also information regarding financial incentive programmes implemented in the USA had no unintended consequences on people's behaviours, including blood donation, further COVID-19 vaccine dose uptake, flu vaccination and trust in government (Schneider et al., Reference Schneider2023).

There is also systematic evidence of the unintended effects of measures taken against COVID-19 on other, health-related, behaviours. For instance, restrictions related to the COVID-19 pandemic (e.g., lockdowns and working-from-home mandates) have been shown to negatively affect physical activity (López-Valenciano et al., Reference López-Valenciano2021; Runacres et al., Reference Runacres2021; Stockwell et al., Reference Stockwell2021; Oliveira et al., Reference Oliveira2022; Strain et al., Reference Strain2022; Wunsch et al., Reference Wunsch, Kienberger and Niessner2022), while there has been an increase in sedentary behaviours (López-Valenciano et al., Reference López-Valenciano2021; Runacres et al., Reference Runacres2021; Stockwell et al., Reference Stockwell2021). Furthermore, dietary habits changed during the COVID-19 pandemic, with decreased adherence to healthy diets and increased consumption of alcohol and processed food (González-Monroy et al., Reference González-Monroy2021; Khan et al., Reference Khan2022). In India, domestic violence incidents increased during strict COVID-19 lockdown measures (Ravindran and Shah, Reference Ravindran and Shah2023).

There has also been research on the potential negative spillover effects of COVID-19 communication strategies. For instance, an experiment conducted in Sweden before vaccines were available found that participants, when exposed to information about the safety, effectiveness and availability of COVID-19 vaccines, became less willing to engage in protective behaviours (Andersson et al., Reference Andersson2021). This phenomenon was termed ‘vaccine anticipation effect’, a type of anticipatory spillover also called behavioural ‘spillunder’ (Krpan et al., Reference Krpan, Galizzi and Dolan2019, Reference Krpan, Galizzi and Dolan2021), suggesting that in the prospect of having a vaccine available, people tend to disengage from protective behaviours as the ‘end’ of the COVID-19 pandemic becomes more salient. On the other hand, providing information about the use of vaccination certificates in the past and people's opinions about the COVID-19 vaccination certificate had no spillover effects on people's willingness to get vaccinated in the USA (Sotis et al., Reference Sotis2021). Likewise, commanding vs non-commanding messages about compliance with COVID-19 rules had no spillover effects on other unrelated behaviours, such as charitable giving in the UK (Krpan and Dolan, Reference Krpan and Dolan2022).

Despite the recent interest in testing messaging strategies and their unintended effects on COVID-19 behaviours, to the best of our knowledge, the spillover effects of messages about COVID-19 vaccination on subsequent protective and compliance behaviours have not yet been systematically examined. The present study is an attempt to fill this gap. In particular, in our pre-analysis plan, we hypothesised that:

H1: exposure to messages that encourage COVID-19 vaccine uptake does not have any spillover effects on subsequent intent to engage in protective, compliance or prosocial behaviours. This is our main hypothesis based on previous research on COVID-19 messaging strategies (Sotis et al., Reference Sotis2021; Krpan and Dolan, Reference Krpan and Dolan2022).

H2: previous participation in a survey about COVID-19 does not associate with participants’ responses in a subsequent survey about COVID-19 protective, compliance or prosocial behaviours. This hypothesis is based on previous research on ‘survey’ or ‘mere-measurement’ effects, a type of behavioural spillover, suggesting that prior survey participation can affect subsequent behaviour (Morwitz and Fitzsimons, Reference Morwitz and Fitzsimons2004; Zwane et al., Reference Zwane2011). For instance, asking people whether they intend to buy a car increased subsequent car purchase rates (Morwitz et al., Reference Morwitz, Johnson and Schmittlein1993). Likewise, asking people about their health behaviours increased subsequent uptake rates of health insurance (Zwane et al., Reference Zwane2011). A potential mechanism via which this effect manifests is the increased salience of an underlying motive (e.g., owning a car) that serves as a reminder about people's intentions (e.g., buying a car) (Bordalo et al., Reference Bordalo, Gennaioli and Shleifer2012). In our study, we expect no such survey effects, given that the pandemic is already sufficiently salient in the respondents’ minds.

H3: being vaccinated does not associate with participants’ intent to engage in protective, compliance and prosocial behaviours. Specifically, we expect that vaccinated and unvaccinated participants would be equally likely to engage in protective behaviours and to comply with the rules in case of possible infection. The alternative hypothesis can, in principle, go in either direction. On the one hand, unvaccinated participants could be more likely to engage in prosocial behaviours, as they may perceive their choice not to get vaccinated as socially undesirable and try to restore this by donating and volunteering (purging spillover). It is also possible that vaccination against COVID-19 may reduce people's perceived risk and make them less compliant (risk compensation). On the other hand, self-selecting into COVID-19 vaccination could trigger a positive self-image, promote positive spillovers and make people more (rather than less) likely to engage in further protective, compliance or prosocial behaviours.

Methods

Participants

Participants were UK adults, aged from 18 to over 65 and recruited through Respondi, an online survey platform (www.respondi.com). The total sample size was 1,848 participants. About 51% of the participants were females (943/1,848). Data collection took place in June–July 2021, and participation was anonymous. The sample included participants who took part in both waves 1 (Steinert et al., Reference Steinert2022) and 2 (n = 1,205), as well as participants who took part only in wave 2 (n = 643). Altogether, participants were allocated under six different conditions:

  1. 1. Unvaccinated participants who received no vaccination messages, that is, the control condition in wave 1 (n = 299).

  2. 2. Unvaccinated participants who received information about the medical efficacy of vaccines in wave 1 (n = 304).

  3. 3. Unvaccinated participants who received information about the collective benefits of vaccination in wave 1 (n = 304).

  4. 4. Unvaccinated participants who received information about the exclusive benefits of vaccination certificates in wave 1 (n = 298).

  5. 5. Vaccinated participants who only took part in wave 2 (n = 332).

  6. 6. Unvaccinated participants who only took part in wave 2 (n = 311).

The reason for allocating participants into six conditions was to test our three hypotheses described above. Specifically, to test H1, we compared participants who were exposed to different messages about COVID-19 vaccination (i.e., vaccine efficacy condition vs collective benefit condition vs vaccination certificate condition vs control). For H2, the comparison was between participants who took part in wave 1 vs unvaccinated participants who took part in both waves 1 and 2. Finally, to test H3, we compared vaccinated vs unvaccinated participants from wave 2 only. The vaccination messages that participants saw in each condition in wave 1 can be found in Supplementary Material S2.

The size of the sample of wave 2 was calculated based on the first wave of the study.

Procedure

Participants initially took part in wave 1, which corresponds to the first four conditions described above. They were exposed to different messages about COVID-19 vaccination and asked about their willingness to get vaccinated (the procedure of the first wave is described in Supplementary Material S3). At the beginning of the first wave, and before being exposed to different experimental conditions, they provided their informed consent and were also asked to indicate their vaccination status and other sociodemographic information including age, gender, education, employment status and income. Subsequently, these participants, along with 643 participants recruited solely for wave 2, took part in wave 2 and conducted immediately after wave 1. Once they provided their informed consent, they were asked a number of questions about their intentions to engage in protective and prosocial behaviours related to COVID-19, as well as compliance behaviours in case of developing symptoms of illness. The full list of the dependent variables and scales of measurement can be found in Supplementary Material S4.

Participants were also asked whether they wished to donate 20%of the money they would receive for their participation in the survey. They were also provided with a hyperlink that would redirect them to the NHS Charities Together website, the umbrella organisation coordinating donations to the NHS (https://www.nhscharitiestogether.co.uk/be-there-for-them/). This was to measure participants’ actual behaviour by tracking whether they clicked on the link. After completing the survey, participants were debriefed. The online platform Respondi arranged to distribute a voucher of £3 to participants who completed the survey in full. Participants who chose to donate to the NHS received only 80% of it. The rest 20% was donated to the NHS Charities Together through the online platform.

Outcome measures

The main outcome measure was participants’ intention to engage in protective and prosocial behaviours (discrete and dichotomous variables, respectively), as well as in compliance behaviours in case of developing symptoms of illness (discrete variables). Similar to Campos-Mercade et al. (Reference Campos-Mercade2021), we also measured participants’ decision to donate money to the NHS and whether they clicked on the link to the NHS charity (dichotomous variables).

Predictors and control variables

We made three comparisons to test H1, H2 and H3 in separate regression models. The main predictors were message condition, participation in wave 1 and vaccination status for H1, H2 and H3, respectively. We used sociodemographic information, such as age (range 18–24 to 65+), gender (female, male and non-binary), education (range primary to higher), employment status (employed and unemployed) and income (range £0–£13,000 to £55,000+) as control variables. Vaccination willingness (willing and not willing) was an additional explanatory variable for H1.

Analyses

We initially used simple logistic regression models for dichotomous dependent variables and simple ordered probit regression models for discrete dependent variables. We subsequently controlled for sociodemographic factors such as gender, age, education, employment status and income (in one step). We also included vaccination willingness as another explanatory variable for H1. In this model, we also checked for interactions between vaccination willingness and the message condition. We also tested for interactions between participants’ vaccination status and age and education for H3. Finally, we ran three subgroup analyses, dividing the sample by age (above vs below 45 years of age), gender (females vs males) and education (higher vs lower). For all the dependent variables, we ran additional regressions as robustness checks using linear regression models. In all regressions, we adjusted all the p-values for multiple hypotheses testing using Bonferroni correction (significance level was set to 0.0019: conventional significance level of 0.05 divided by 26 dependent variables). In line with our hypotheses and light of the existing literature, we articulate our three hypotheses as null hypotheses (i.e., no spillover effects of vaccination messages, no spillover effects of previous survey participation and no risk compensation: H1, H2 and H3, respectively). To rigorously test whether the absence of evidence corresponds to the evidence of absence for H1, H2 and H3 (i.e., the null hypothesis is not rejected), we conducted a series of ‘equivalence tests’ (Lakens, Reference Lakens2017; Harms and Lakens, Reference Harms and Lakens2018; Lakens et al., Reference Lakens, Scheel and Isager2018).

Analysis and results

Comparison by the message condition (H1)

In this comparison, we included 1,205 participants. A detailed summary of the sociodemographic characteristics of the sample can be found in Supplementary Material (S1). The regression models revealed no statistically significant association between any condition and any of the variables of interest, suggesting that none of the messages significantly changed participants’ intentions to engage in protective, compliance or prosocial behaviours. The regression tables are available at https://osf.io/fy3ps.

We further excluded the presence of any effects by using Two One-Sided Equivalence Testing (TOST). The small effect size used in the TOST procedure was based on the expected effect size in wave 1 (https://osf.io/53zdk/), which was also the expected effect size in wave 2. A summary of the results of the TOST procedure is presented at https://osf.io/fy3ps. This, together with the fact that in the main analysis, we could not reject the null hypothesis that the spillover effect was not statistically significant from zero, provides rigorous evidence of statistical equivalence for each of the spillover effects, that is, the evidence of the absence of spillover effects in our sample.

Comparison by survey participation (H2)

We next looked at the potential spillover effects of having been exposed, more generally, to a survey, rather than to the specific messages about COVID-19 vaccination. In this comparison, we included 1,516 participants (1,205 unvaccinated participants who took part in both waves of the study vs 311 unvaccinated participants who took part only in wave 2). A detailed summary of participants’ characteristics can be found in Supplementary Material S1. We found no statistically significant association between survey participation and any of the variables of interest, suggesting that having previously been surveyed about COVID-19 did not significantly change participants’ intentions to engage in protective, compliance or prosocial behaviours. The regression tables are available at https://osf.io/fy3ps.

Comparison by vaccination status (H3)

We finally focused on the potential effects of being vaccinated on intentions to engage in COVID-19 behaviours. In this comparison, we included only participants who took part in the second wave of the study (n = 643). A summary of participants’ characteristics can be found in Supplementary Material S1. Unlike message condition and survey participation, vaccination status was found to be significantly associated with participants’ intentions to engage in protective, compliance and prosocial behaviours. Contrary to our null hypothesis and also to the risk compensation hypothesis, the association between vaccination status and the variables of interest was a positive one, indicating that vaccinated participants were more likely than unvaccinated ones to engage in almost all the protective, compliance and prosocial behaviours, even after correcting for multiple hypotheses testing (Figures 1–3).

Figure 1. Means of intention to engage in protective behaviours by vaccination status. Notes: This chart includes participants who took part only in wave 2. Protective behaviours were measured on a 1–5 scale with anchors from ‘Never’ to ‘Always’. Higher values indicate higher engagement in protective behaviours. Error bars represent standard error. The NHS Track and Trace app is a mobile app that has a number of different tools, including contact tracing and venue check-in (https://covid19.nhs.uk/). *Encouraging others to get vaccinated was classified as prosocial behaviour. Due to its measure (i.e., 1–5 scale), it has been included in this graph and not in the graph about prosocial behaviours (Figure 3), where results are presented in percentages.

Figure 2. Means of intention to engage in compliance behaviours by vaccination status. Notes: This chart includes participants who took part only in wave 2. Compliance was measured on a 1–5 scale with anchors from ‘Never’ to ‘Always’. Higher values indicate higher engagement in compliance behaviours. Error bars represent standard error.

Figure 3. Percentages of intention to engage in prosocial behaviours by vaccination status. Notes: This chart includes participants who took part only in wave 2. Prosocial behaviours were dichotomous variables with possible answers ‘Yes’ or ‘No’. Results are presented in percentages. A higher percentage indicates higher engagement in prosocial behaviours. Error bars represent standard error.

The results are presented in Table 1.

Table 1. Main effects of vaccination status on protective behaviours

Notes: This regression table provides information about the size of the effect of vaccination status and its significance on all the behaviours of interest. Standard errors are robust. P-values are adjusted using Bonferroni correction for multiple hypotheses testing.

a As shown in Figure 1, the dependent variable ‘Encourage friends to get vaccinated’ was conceptualised as a prosocial behaviour. However, since it was treated as a discrete variable measured on a 1–5 scale, as opposed to the other prosocial behaviours treated as dichotomous variables, we used the ordered probit regression model to test the effects of vaccination status on it. For this reason, we included this variable in this table.

b The effect was significant before Bonferroni correction.

The results of the exploratory analyses, including further estimations looking at the role of sociodemographic factors, are reported in Supplementary Material S5. Subgroup analyses are reported at https://osf.io/fy3ps.

Discussion

Our study revealed that messages about the medical and non-medical benefits of COVID-19 vaccination aiming to increase vaccine uptake did not affect people's intentions to subsequently engage in other COVID-19 or prosocial behaviours. Furthermore, being previously surveyed about COVID-19 vaccination was not associated with intention to engage in subsequent behaviours either. Self-reported vaccination status and vaccination intention were rather the factors associated with people's engagement in protective and prosocial behaviours and compliance with the COVID-19 rules.

The absence of behavioural spillovers of vaccination messages is in line with our null hypothesis, as well as with previous research on COVID-19 communication strategies (Sotis et al., Reference Sotis2021; Krpan and Dolan, Reference Krpan and Dolan2022). However, our finding is not in line with previous evidence from Sweden (Andersson et al., Reference Andersson2021). A possible reason for this discrepancy is related to the timing of the two studies. The experiment in Sweden was conducted at the end of 2020 when mass vaccination was only a hypothetical scenario, whereas the present study was conducted over the summer 2021 when the UK achieved one of the highest records of mass vaccinations in the world, while other restrictions such as mask-wearing and social distancing were still in place. Moreover, in the summer 2021, information about the ‘Delta’ variant also started circulating in the media. Both these new circumstances could have made respondents less likely to see the COVID-19 vaccine as the ‘silver bullet’ to conclude the pandemic and therefore less optimistic about the ‘end’ of the pandemic.

In addition to timing, motivation could also play a role. For instance, providing people with specific information about the various benefits of the vaccines (that were hardly available at the end of 2020) could increase their motivation for engaging in COVID-19 behaviours, their awareness of the pandemic and their sense of responsibility in contributing to combating the virus. This potential mechanism is not unambiguous, though, as specific information about vaccination could also create a false sense that the pandemic is reaching an end and therefore make people decrease their efforts.

The absence of behavioural spillovers of previous survey participation is in line with our null hypothesis but in contrast to the idea of a survey, or mere-measurement, effects (Morwitz et al., Reference Morwitz, Johnson and Schmittlein1993; Morwitz and Fitzsimons, Reference Morwitz and Fitzsimons2004; Zwane et al., Reference Zwane2011). A potential mechanism via which such an effect would be manifested is salience (Bordalo et al., Reference Bordalo, Gennaioli and Shleifer2012). Previous, or repeated, participation in studies related to COVID-19 could be perceived as a reminder that the pandemic has not ended. However, since the study was run in June–July 2021, when the average daily infections in the UK were more than 20,000 (GOV.UK, 2022a), the pandemic was likely sufficiently salient in the respondents’ minds. One could also argue that people's gradual disengagement from COVID-19 behaviours, possibly due to ‘pandemic fatigue’ (i.e., the alleged tendency to become tired of following COVID-19 rules) (Petherick et al., Reference Petherick2021), would undermine potential survey effects and make people insensitive to previous exposure. However, neither our data nor the existing evidence, at least in the UK, support the idea of pandemic fatigue (Michie et al., Reference Michie, West and Harvey2020).

Finally, being vaccinated was significantly associated with people's intention to engage in protective and prosocial behaviours and to comply with COVID-19 rules. This is not consistent with our null hypothesis nor with the risk compensation hypothesis (Luckman et al., Reference Luckman2021; Yan et al., Reference Yan2021). Rather, it is in line with previous studies that revealed a reverse pattern, a positive or promoting spillover effect where a protective intervention increases subsequent protective behaviours (Kasting et al., Reference Kasting2016; Guenther et al., Reference Guenther, Galizzi and Sanders2021; Seres et al., Reference Seres2021; Sun et al., Reference Sun2022). Therefore, this evidence speaks directly against the risk compensation hypothesis.

A potential explanation of this effect could be people's preference for, or tendency to maintain, cognitive consistency (Cialdini et al., Reference Cialdini, Trost and Newsom1995). This is primarily based on Festinger's Cognitive Dissonance Theory that suggests that people tend to feel psychological discomfort when their beliefs and behaviours are not aligned (Festinger, Reference Festinger1957). To this extent, one could argue that people who protect themselves from COVID-19 infection through vaccination also protect themselves through other health behaviours, thus maintaining a consistent attitude towards the virus. It is also possible that people who protect themselves through vaccination might be driven by an underlying motive (e.g., the common good), which is served by both vaccination and engagement in protective behaviours. Heterogeneity in people's risk preferences (which, unfortunately, we were not able to control) could also contribute to such an attitudinal consistency towards vaccination and protective behaviours (Guenther et al., Reference Guenther, Galizzi and Sanders2021). Another potential interpretation of attitudinal consistency may be related to possible ‘experimenter demand effects’ (Zizzo, Reference Zizzo2010; de Quidt et al., Reference de Quidt, Haushofer and Roth2018; Mummolo and Peterson, Reference Mummolo and Peterson2019; Haaland et al., Reference Haaland, Roth and Wohlfart2023); however, it is fair to note that, realistically, the likelihood of experimenter demand effects occurring in our case should have been limited by the temporal separation between wave 1 and wave 2 of data collection, together with the fact that wave 1 did not mention any reference to other health behaviours, which were only assessed in wave 2. This is confirmed by our finding, discussed above, of no significant survey effect (H2). Attitudinal consistency could also be related to the level of specificity between the attitudes and the behaviours, a key factor that, according to the Theory of Planned Behaviour, determines whether attitudes affect behaviours (Ajzen, Reference Ajzen1991). Finally, people may opt for both vaccination and protective behaviours simply because both are required to mitigate infection.

Our study has three main limitations. First, the effect of vaccination status cannot be interpreted causally because respondents did self-select for COVID-19 vaccination, and the vaccination status could not be randomly assigned to different participants. Second, the study sample might not be fully representative of the UK population, given that during data collection for wave 1, older people were more likely to have been vaccinated and therefore could not be included in our sample. This is also reflected in participants’ demographics, as approximately 90% of our sample were between 18 and 44 years of age (see Supplementary Material S1). Finally, with the exception of the behavioural measure (i.e., participants’ donation to the NHS), our results mainly rely on self-reported intentions, which may be susceptible to social desirability or self-reporting bias.

Despite these limitations, our study reveals some previously undocumented results, namely no spillover effects of vaccine communication strategies and no risk compensation for vaccinated participants. Future research could expand our work by measuring people's actual health behaviours or their interaction with health information through lab-field experiments (Galizzi and Navarro-Martinez, Reference Galizzi and Navarro-Martinez2019). In addition, future studies could test communication strategies about protective behaviours (as opposed to vaccination), given that some people may be against vaccination and perceive the messages as too distant from their prior beliefs (Yaniv and Kleinberger, Reference Yaniv and Kleinberger2000; Yaniv, Reference Yaniv2004). Finally, given the absence of behavioural spillovers, future policies could be less concerned about any unintended consequences when designing such messages. Instead, since vaccination status is associated with engagement in protective and compliance behaviours, policymakers could focus on vaccination campaigns to increase vaccine uptake that will, in turn, contribute to the engagement in compliance behaviours. Finally, since certain groups of our sample were found to be less willing to engage in health behaviours (e.g., males and unvaccinated), and given the importance of health-protective behaviours in tackling a pandemic, policymakers could try to design hybrid interventions that target specific populations and highlight the importance of protective behaviours, even when vaccination uptake in the population is generally high (Flemming, Reference Flemming2022).

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org.10.1017/bpp.2024.1

Data availability

The dataset and code needed to reproduce the analyses can be found at osf.io/fy3ps.

Acknowledgements

We thank Virginia Fedrigo and Claire Heard for advice on the initial design stage and Therese Holmqvist for early assistance in cleaning and analysing the data. We gratefully acknowledge helpful comments by Tim Büthe, Janina Steinert, Henrike Sternberg and Giuseppe A. Veltri.

Author contributions

B.F. and M.M.G. acquired funding for this research study. B.F. and M.M.G. conceptualised the study and led the design of the study. M.M.G. oversaw and managed the data collection. P.K. cleaned the data and conducted the quantitative data analyses and data visualisations. B.F., M.M.G. and P.K. contributed to the interpretation of the findings. P.K. drafted the first version of the manuscript and all authors provided substantial revisions and feedback.

Funding statement

This work is part of the PERISCOPE project (https://periscopeproject.eu/home). The Periscope project has received funding from the European Union's Horizon 2020 Research and Innovation programme under Grant Agreement number 101016233. The funding organisation had no role in the study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, or writing of the manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

References

Ahn, J. N., Hu, D. and Vega, M. (2021), ‘Changing pace: using implementation intentions to enhance social distancing behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 27(4): 762772. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000385.Google ScholarPubMed
Ajzen, I. (1991), ‘The theory of planned behavior’, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50(2): 179211. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andersson, O., et al. (2021), ‘Anticipation of COVID-19 vaccines reduces willingness to socially distance’, Journal of Health Economics, 80: 102530. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhealeco.2021.102530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aranguren, M. (2022), ‘Face mask use conditionally decreases compliance with physical distancing rules against COVID-19: gender differences in risk compensation pattern’, Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 56(4): 332346. https://doi.org/10.1093/abm/kaab072.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asch, P., et al. (1991), ‘Risk compensation and the effectiveness of safety belt use laws: a case study of New Jersey’, Policy Sciences, 24(2): 181197. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00138059.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bordalo, P., Gennaioli, N. and Shleifer, A. (2012), ‘Salience theory of choice under risk’, The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 127(3): 12431285. https://doi.org/10.1093/qje/qjs018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campos-Mercade, P., et al. (2021), ‘Prosociality predicts health behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic’, Journal of Public Economics, 195: 104367. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpubeco.2021.104367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cartaud, A., Quesque, F. and Coello, Y. (2020), ‘Wearing a face mask against Covid-19 results in a reduction of social distancing’, PLoS One, 15(12): e0243023. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243023.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cialdini, R. B., Trost, M. R. and Newsom, J. T. (1995), ‘Preference for consistency: the development of a valid measure and the discovery of surprising behavioral implications’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(2): 318328. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.2.318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Quidt, J., Haushofer, J. and Roth, C. (2018), ‘Measuring and bounding experimenter demand’, American Economic Review, 108(11): 32663302. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.20171330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dolan, P. and Galizzi, M. M. (2015), ‘Like ripples on a pond: behavioral spillovers and their implications for research and policy’, Journal of Economic Psychology, 47: 116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2014.12.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
European Centre for Disease Prevention and Control (2012) Communication on Immunisation – Building Trust. https://www.ecdc.europa.eu/sites/default/files/media/en/publications/Publications/TER-Immunisation-and-trust.pdf [22 November 2021].Google Scholar
Evans, W. N. and Graham, J. D. (1991), ‘Risk reduction or risk compensation? The case of mandatory safety-belt use laws’, Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 4(1): 6173. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00057886.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Festinger, L. (1957), A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flemming, A. (2022), ‘Omicron, the great escape artist’, Nature Reviews Immunology, 22(2): 7575. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41577-022-00676-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D., et al. (2021), ‘Effects of different types of written vaccination information on COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy in the UK (OCEANS-III): a single-blind, parallel-group, randomised controlled trial’, The Lancet Public Health, 6(6): e416e427. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2468-2667(21)00096-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galizzi, M. M. and Navarro-Martinez, D. (2019), ‘On the external validity of social preference games: a systematic lab-field study’, Management Science, 65(3): 9761002. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.2017.2908.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galizzi, M. M. and Whitmarsh, L. (2019), ‘How to measure behavioral spillovers: a methodological review and checklist’, Frontiers in Psychology, 10(342): 115. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gantiva, C., Jiménez-Leal, W. and Urriago-Rayo, J. (2021), ‘Framing messages to deal with the COVID-19 crisis: the role of loss/gain frames and content’, Frontiers in Psychology, 12: 18. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.568212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glaser, T., et al. (2015), ‘Lateral attitude change’, Personality and Social Psychology Review, 19(3): 257276. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868314546489.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
González-Monroy, C., et al. (2021), ‘Eating behaviour changes during the covid-19 pandemic: a systematic review of longitudinal studies’, International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(21): 11130. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182111130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
GOV.UK (2022a) Cases in United Kingdom. https://coronavirus.data.gov.uk/ [25 November 2021].Google Scholar
GOV.UK (2022b) Vaccinations in United Kingdom. https://coronavirus.data.gov.uk/details/vaccinations [30 November 2022].Google Scholar
Greenhalgh, T., et al. (2020), ‘Face masks for the public during the covid-19 crisis’, BMJ, 369: m1435. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.m1435.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guenther, B., Galizzi, M. M. and Sanders, J. G. (2021), ‘Heterogeneity in risk-taking during the COVID-19 pandemic: evidence from the UK lockdown’, Frontiers in Psychology, 12: 115. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.643653.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haaland, I., Roth, C. and Wohlfart, J. (2023), ‘Designing information provision experiments’, Journal of Economic Literature, 61(1): 340. https://doi.org/10.1257/jel.20211658.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harms, C. and Lakens, D. (2018), ‘Making “null effects” informative: statistical techniques and inferential frameworks’, Journal of Clinical and Translational Research, 3: 382393. https://doi.org/10.18053/jctres.03.2017S2.007.Google ScholarPubMed
Heffner, J., Vives, M.-L. and FeldmanHall, O. (2021), ‘Emotional responses to prosocial messages increase willingness to self-isolate during the COVID-19 pandemic’, Personality and Individual Differences, 170(2020): 110420. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110420.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Houston, D. J. and Richardson, L. E. (2007), ‘Risk compensation or risk reduction? Seatbelts, state laws, and traffic fatalities’, Social Science Quarterly, 88(4): 913936. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6237.2007.00510.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howard, J., et al. (2021), ‘An evidence review of face masks against COVID-19’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(4): 112. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2014564118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyland-Wood, B., et al. (2021), ‘Toward effective government communication strategies in the era of COVID-19’, Humanities and Social Sciences Communications, 8(1): 30. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-020-00701-w.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Islam, M. S., et al. (2020), ‘COVID-19-related infodemic and its impact on public health: a global social media analysis’, The American Journal of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene, 103(4): 16211629. https://doi.org/10.4269/ajtmh.20-0812.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, A., et al. (2019) RESPONSE: A Behavioural Insights Checklist for Designing Effective Communications. https://www.local.gov.uk/sites/default/files/documents/Response/Playbook/Final.pdf [22 November 2021].Google Scholar
Jørgensen, F., et al. (2021), ‘Does face mask use elicit risk-compensation? Quasi-experimental evidence from Denmark during the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic’, European Journal of Public Health, 31(6): 12591265. https://doi.org/10.1093/eurpub/ckab136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kasting, M. L., et al. (2016), ‘Tempest in a teapot: a systematic review of HPV vaccination and risk compensation research’, Human Vaccines & Immunotherapeutics, 12(6): 14351450. https://doi.org/10.1080/21645515.2016.1141158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khan, M. A. B., et al. (2022), ‘Systematic review of the effects of pandemic confinements on body weight and their determinants’, British Journal of Nutrition, 127(2): 298317. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007114521000921.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kostopoulou, O. and Schwartz, A. (2021), ‘To unpack or not? Testing public health messaging about COVID-19’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 27(4): 751761. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000359.Google ScholarPubMed
Krpan, D. and Dolan, P. (2022), ‘You must stay at home! the impact of commands on behaviors during COVID-19’, Social Psychological and Personality Science, 13(1): 333346. https://doi.org/10.1177/19485506211005582.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krpan, D., Galizzi, M. M. and Dolan, P. (2019), ‘Looking at spillovers in the mirror: making a case for “behavioral spillunders”’, Frontiers in Psychology, 10: 113. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krpan, D., Galizzi, M. M. and Dolan, P. (2021), ‘When the future “spills under”: general self-efficacy moderates the influence of expected exercise on present intellectual performance’, Social Psychological and Personality Science, 12(7): 12641273. https://doi.org/10.1177/19485506211018367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakens, D. (2017), ‘Equivalence tests: a practical primer for t tests, correlations, and meta-analyses’, Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8(4): 355362. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617697177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakens, D., Scheel, A. M. and Isager, P. M. (2018), ‘Equivalence testing for psychological research: a tutorial’, Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 1(2): 259269. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245918770963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lazzarino, A. I., et al. (2020), ‘Covid-19: important potential side effects of wearing face masks that we should bear in mind’, BMJ, 369: m2003. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.m2003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liebst, L. S., et al. (2022), ‘No evidence that mask-wearing in public places elicits risk compensation behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic’, Scientific Reports, 12(1): 1511. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-05270-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
López-Valenciano, A., et al. (2021), ‘Impact of COVID-19 pandemic on university students’ physical activity levels: an early systematic review’, Frontiers in Psychology, 11(624567): 110. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.624567.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorna, C. (2021), COVID-19 Vaccine Misinformation. https://post.parliament.uk/covid-19-vaccine-misinformation/ [22 November 2021].Google Scholar
Luckman, A., et al. (2021), ‘Risk compensation during COVID-19: the impact of face mask usage on social distancing’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 27(4): 722738. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000382.Google ScholarPubMed
MacIntyre, C. R., Costantino, V. and Chanmugam, A. (2021), ‘The use of face masks during vaccine roll-out in New York City and impact on epidemic control’, Vaccine, 39(42): 62966301. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.vaccine.2021.08.102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mantzari, E., Rubin, G. J. and Marteau, T. M. (2020), ‘Is risk compensation threatening public health in the covid-19 pandemic?’, BMJ, 370: m2913. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.m2913.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marchiori, M. (2020), ‘COVID-19 and the social distancing paradox: dangers and solutions’, arXiv preprint, 18. https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2005.12446.Google Scholar
Martin, G. P., et al. (2020), ‘Science, society, and policy in the face of uncertainty: reflections on the debate around face coverings for the public during COVID-19’, Critical Public Health, 30(5): 501508. https://doi.org/10.1080/09581596.2020.1797997.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merkley, E. and Loewen, P. J. (2021), ‘Assessment of communication strategies for mitigating COVID-19 vaccine-specific hesitancy in Canada’, JAMA Network Open, 4(9): e2126635. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2021.26635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michie, S., West, R. and Harvey, N. (2020), ‘The concept of “fatigue” in tackling covid-19’, BMJ, 371: m4171. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.m4171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morwitz, V. and Fitzsimons, G. (2004), ‘The mere-measurement effect: why does measuring intentions change actual behavior?’, Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(1&2): 6473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morwitz, V. G., Johnson, E. and Schmittlein, D. (1993), ‘Does measuring intent change behavior?’, Journal of Consumer Research, 20(1): 46. https://doi.org/10.1086/209332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mummolo, J. and Peterson, E. (2019), ‘Demand effects in survey experiments: an empirical assessment’, American Political Science Review, 113(2): 517529. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055418000837.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oliveira, M. R., et al. (2022), ‘Covid-19 and the impact on the physical activity level of elderly people: a systematic review’, Experimental Gerontology, 159(2021): 111675. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.exger.2021.111675.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palm, R., Bolsen, T. and Kingsland, J. T. (2021), ‘The effect of frames on COVID-19 vaccine resistance’, Frontiers in Political Science, 3(661257): 111. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpos.2021.661257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peltzman, S. (1975), ‘The effects of automobile safety regulation’, Journal of Political Economy, 83(4): 677725. https://doi.org/10.1086/260352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pennycook, G., et al. (2020), ‘Fighting COVID-19 misinformation on social media: experimental evidence for a scalable accuracy-nudge intervention’, Psychological Science, 31(7): 770780. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620939054.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petherick, A., et al. (2021), ‘A worldwide assessment of changes in adherence to COVID-19 protective behaviours and hypothesized pandemic fatigue’, Nature Human Behaviour, 5(9): 11451160. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01181-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ravindran, S. and Shah, M. (2023), ‘Unintended consequences of lockdowns, COVID-19 and the shadow pandemic in India’, Nature Human Behaviour [Preprint]. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-022-01513-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Runacres, A., et al. (2021), ‘Impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on sedentary time and behaviour in children and adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis’, International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(21): 11286. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182111286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sahni, N. S. (2016), ‘Advertising spillovers: evidence from online field experiments and implications for returns on advertising’, Journal of Marketing Research, 53(4): 459478. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmr.14.0274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, F. H., et al. (2023), ‘Financial incentives for vaccination do not have negative unintended consequences’, Nature, 613(7944): 526533. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-022-05512-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seres, G., et al. (2021), ‘Face mask use and physical distancing before and after mandatory masking: no evidence on risk compensation in public waiting lines’, Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 192: 765781. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2021.10.032.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sotis, C., et al. (2021), ‘‘COVID-19 vaccine passport and international traveling: the combined effect of two nudges on Americans’ support for the pass’’, International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(16): 8800. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18168800.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinert, J. I., et al. (2022), ‘COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy in eight European countries: prevalence, determinants, and heterogeneity’, Science Advances, 8(17): 115. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.abm9825.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stockwell, S., et al. (2021), ‘Changes in physical activity and sedentary behaviours from before to during the COVID-19 pandemic lockdown: a systematic review’, BMJ Open Sport & Exercise Medicine, 7(1): e000960. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjsem-2020-000960.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strain, T., et al. (2022), ‘Population level physical activity before and during the first national COVID-19 lockdown: a nationally representative repeat cross-sectional study of 5 years of active lives data in England’, The Lancet Regional Health – Europe, 12: 100265. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lanepe.2021.100265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sun, L.-X., et al. (2022), ‘Association between health behaviours and the COVID-19 vaccination: risk compensation among healthcare workers in Taizhou, China’, Human Vaccines & Immunotherapeutics, 18(1), https://doi.org/10.1080/21645515.2022.2029257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trogen, B. and Caplan, A. (2021), ‘Risk compensation and COVID-19 vaccines’, Annals of Internal Medicine, 174(6): 858859. https://doi.org/10.7326/M20-8251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vacondio, M., et al. (2021), ‘Worry, perceived threat and media communication as predictors of self-protective behaviors during the COVID-19 outbreak in Europe’, Frontiers in Psychology, 12: 19. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.577992.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vereen, R. N., et al. (2021), ‘Motivations, barriers, and communication recommendations for promoting face coverings during the COVID-19 pandemic: survey findings from a diverse sample’, PLoS One, 16(5): e0251169. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wadud, Z., Rahman, S. M. and Enam, A. (2022), ‘Face mask mandates and risk compensation: an analysis of mobility data during the COVID-19 pandemic in Bangladesh’, BMJ Global Health, 7(1): e006803. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjgh-2021-006803.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization (2017) Strategic Communications Framework for effective communications. https://cdn.who.int/media/docs/default-source/documents/communication-framework.pdf?sfvrsn=93aa6138_0 [22 November 2021].Google Scholar
World Health Organization (2020) Mask Use in the Context of COVID-19. https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/337199 [21 February 2022].Google Scholar
World Health Organization (2021) Fighting Misinformation in the Time of COVID-19, One Click at a Time. https://www.who.int/news-room/feature-stories/detail/fighting-misinformation-in-the-time-of-covid-19-one-click-at-a-time [22 November 2021].Google Scholar
Wunsch, K., Kienberger, K. and Niessner, C. (2022), ‘Changes in physical activity patterns due to the Covid-19 pandemic: a systematic review and meta-analysis’, International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(4): 2250. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19042250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yan, Y., et al. (2021), ‘Risk compensation and face mask mandates during the COVID-19 pandemic’, Scientific Reports, 11(1): 3174. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-82574-w.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yaniv, I. (2004), ‘Receiving other people's advice: influence and benefit’, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 93(1): 113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2003.08.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yaniv, I. and Kleinberger, E. (2000), ‘Advice taking in decision making: egocentric discounting and reputation formation’, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 83(2): 260281. https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.2000.2909.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zizzo, D. J. (2010), ‘Experimenter demand effects in economic experiments’, Experimental Economics, 13(1): 7598. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-009-9230-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zwane, A. P., et al. (2011), ‘Being surveyed can change later behavior and related parameter estimates’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(5): 18211826. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1000776108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Means of intention to engage in protective behaviours by vaccination status. Notes: This chart includes participants who took part only in wave 2. Protective behaviours were measured on a 1–5 scale with anchors from ‘Never’ to ‘Always’. Higher values indicate higher engagement in protective behaviours. Error bars represent standard error. The NHS Track and Trace app is a mobile app that has a number of different tools, including contact tracing and venue check-in (https://covid19.nhs.uk/). *Encouraging others to get vaccinated was classified as prosocial behaviour. Due to its measure (i.e., 1–5 scale), it has been included in this graph and not in the graph about prosocial behaviours (Figure 3), where results are presented in percentages.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Means of intention to engage in compliance behaviours by vaccination status. Notes: This chart includes participants who took part only in wave 2. Compliance was measured on a 1–5 scale with anchors from ‘Never’ to ‘Always’. Higher values indicate higher engagement in compliance behaviours. Error bars represent standard error.

Figure 2

Figure 3. Percentages of intention to engage in prosocial behaviours by vaccination status. Notes: This chart includes participants who took part only in wave 2. Prosocial behaviours were dichotomous variables with possible answers ‘Yes’ or ‘No’. Results are presented in percentages. A higher percentage indicates higher engagement in prosocial behaviours. Error bars represent standard error.

Figure 3

Table 1. Main effects of vaccination status on protective behaviours

Supplementary material: File

Kourtidis et al. supplementary material

Kourtidis et al. supplementary material
Download Kourtidis et al. supplementary material(File)
File 939 KB