There has been considerable controversy over the role of pharmacological treatments for psychological disorders in children and adolescents. Revelations by the media about the lack of efficacy and harmful effects of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor antidepressants (SSRIs), and the practice within parts of the pharmaceutical industry of hiding or failing to disclose negative drug trials, have heightened this controversy. The fact that these problems were made public by a few individual doctors and journalists rather than academic establishments or learned bodies is a further cause for concern.
The relationship between psychiatry and the pharmaceutical industry has not always been straightforward. Indeed, the discovery of many of the major drug treatments such as antipsychotics and anti-depressants initially owed more to serendipity than to a theoretically informed development. However, there can be little argument that first-generation anti-psychotic medications have benefited millions of patients since their introduction in the 1950s. Likewise, clinical practice and numerous double-blind randomised controlled trials (RCTs) have shown the benefits, at least in adults, of antidepressant medication, although recent evidence does question the side-effect profile and efficacy of SSRIs in younger adults (Reference Friedman and LeonFriedman & Leon, 2007).
There has been a huge increase in the rate of prescribing of medication for psychiatric disorders in children and adolescents. While this, in part, may be due to an increased recognition of disorders and demand for treatment, there is some cause for concern. For instance, in the USA there was a six-fold rise in the prescribing of antipsychotic medications between 1993 and 2002 (Reference Olfson, Blanco and LiuOlfson et al, 2006) and over one-third of these prescriptions are for children with mood disorders, an indication for which the evidence-base is lacking. Alongside this there is a trend for increased prescribing ahead of evidence. For example, although only a few RCTs show risperidone to be of benefit to children with disruptive behavioural disorders and a diagnosis of autism or pervasive developmental disorder (McCraken et al, 2002; Reference Shea, Turgay and CarrollShea et al, 2004; Reference Padina, Aman and FindlingPadina et al, 2006), there is widespread use of antipsychotics for the treatment of disruptive behavioural disorders in children in general (Reference Olfson, Blanco and LiuOlfson et al, 2006). Recently, the licence holder (Janssen-Cilag) applied to the UK licensing authority, the Medicines and Healthcare products Regulatory Agency (MHRA), to include irritability in autism as a licensed indication. After seeking views of experts and the National Autistic Society, the Committee on Safety of Medicines offered conditional approval, dependent on safety monitoring with written reports to MHRA. Unfortunately, the company withdrew its application (Reference Morgan and TaylorMorgan & Taylor, 2007). Thus, despite agreement on the use of risperidone for the management of aggression, a valuable opportunity for safety monitoring was lost and no doubt the medication will continue to be prescribed unlicensed. There is also a trend for use of this antipsychotic in children with a diagnosis of conduct disorder. Indeed, what evidence there is suggests that the effect size for a reduction of aggression in the case of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and conduct disorder is very modest (Reference Armenteros, Lewis and DavalosArmenteros et al, 2007). There is a concern that the widespread use of antipsychotics in youngsters might cause adverse neurological side-effects such as tardive dyskinesia, although a recent review (Reference Padina, Aman and FindlingPadina et al, 2006) showed that the rate of side-effects with low-dose antipsychotics was low.
Another area of concern, highlighted by the US Institute of Medicine of the National Academies (2006), centres on the system for ensuring the safety of drugs after their approval by the Food and Drug Administration (FDA). It has been argued (Reference Hennessy and StromHennessy & Strom, 2007) that limitations in the pre-approval drug-testing process have resulted in 20% of drugs receiving black box warnings after approval, and 4% of drugs being ultimately withdrawn from the market for safety reasons. Curiously, in a wealth-generating industry with substantial applicant fees, financial limitations at the heart of the FDA organisation have been identified as one possible reason for shortcomings.
The problem of bias
Belief in the scientific underpinnings of psychopharmacology is essential, and representation of research findings is an important part of that process. Any corruption of this information is absolutely to be decried. In relation to drug trials, there are two main sources of bias: first, biases in trial design and, second, biases in dissemination of results (Reference VietaVieta, 2007). Vieta's concerns focus on industry-funded drug trials for adults, but the principles are pertinent to all ages. Even though the FDA in North America and the European Medicines Agency (EMEA) (formerly the European Agency for the Evaluation of Medicinal Products) have set design guidelines for drug trials, trial design biases are still possible (Box 1). According to Reference VietaVieta (2007), biases in dissemination of results (Box 2) can be more difficult to detect, but it is clear that publishing negative trials is the responsibility of all those involved in the research process, including journal editors. There is an added ethical issue: patients who voluntarily participate in trials with the expectation that their input will contribute to furtherance of scientific knowledge and be placed in the public domain have rights that need to be respected ahead of commercial interests.
-
• False non-inferiority designs
-
• Enriched designs
-
• Underpowered comparator samples
-
• Unfair comparator doses
-
• Inclusion of patients who are non-responsive to the comparator
-
• Unfair rescue medication rules
-
• ‘Creative’ outcome measures favouring the drug of choice
-
• Suppression of the publication of negative results, which remain ‘data on file’, while positive results are published
-
• Presentation of negative results as posters at conferences, on small websites or in a very conservative format
-
• Analysis, sub-analysis and repeated publication of positive results, and presentation at scientific meetings
No one can support the failure to disclose negative trial results. Furthermore, Timimi's criticisms of the conduct of a number of the trials involving fluoxetine are legitimate, in that primary outcome measures were often not significant and secondary outcome measures were reported as being significant (Reference TimimiTimimi, 2007, this issue). This is clearly wrong and against the recommendations set out in the latest guidance on design and analysis of clinical trials (Food and Drug Administration, 1998), which state that primary outcome variables in regulatory trials must ‘provide a valid and reliable measure of some clinical and relevant and important treatment benefits in a patient population’.
Pharmaceutical industries are multibillion-pound, worldwide enterprises; it is possible that financial pressures underlie some of the malpractice in reporting drug trial results. As part of regulation transparency and policies of disclosure are the barest minimum standards that must be set. Cynically one might believe that large multinational companies are unlikely to respond solely to regulatory bodies, academics or editors of journals, often citing commercial sensitivities as a reason for not disclosing information. Is it likely, therefore, that what is required, as has been shown with the tobacco industry, is group legal action in US Courts taken by individuals – patients experiencing drug side-effects or relatives of patients who took their own lives. The likelihood of financial penalties is often a major driver for change in practice.
The persuaders?
The ubiquitous nature of pharmaceutical company sponsorship in the USA and probably the UK is quite astounding (Reference Campbell, Gruen and MountfordCampbell et al, 2007) – apparently around 93% of doctors accept gifts, drug samples or conference expenses. Timimi and others accuse medical healthcare professionals of, at best, naïvety. Do doctors change their prescribing practices in response to pharmaceutical company promotions? According to Timimi we do. However, the major changes in my prescribing practice – use of second-generation antipsychotics and long-acting stimulant formulations – were made to reduce side-effects and for ease of use and adherence respectively, rather than in blind response to pharmaceutical company promotions.
The claim that psychiatrists are unduly influenced by pharmaceutical company promotions can be examined by more than anecdotal evidence. In the case of eating disorders – bulimia nervosa in particular – guidelines from the UK's National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence (NICE) recommend cognitive–behavioural therapy (CBT) as first-line treatment, with antidepressant medication as an alternative or addition as necessary (National Collaborating Centre for Mental Health, 2004). Whether or not child psychiatrists follow these guidelines is more likely to depend on the availability of resources to deliver CBT. If, however, CBT is available then in my experience child psychiatrists do use psychological treatments first. A recent evidence-based manual for child psychiatry (Reference Wolpert, Fuggle and CottrellWolpert et al, 2006) recommended medication as the first-line treatment for only three out of fourteen disorders – ADHD, psychosis and Tourette syndrome. This suggests that child psychiatry is, if anything, psychotherapeutically oriented rather than drug dominated.
The profit incentive
Given the recent disclosures (e.g. Reference Healy, Langmaak and SavageHealy et al, 1999; Panorama, 2007), Timimi's view of pharmaceutical companies driven by commercial rather than scientific concerns may be difficult to refute. Tight regulatory mechanisms are needed. However, it is surely not in the interests of pharmaceutical com panies to promote ineffective or harmful drug treatments. Indeed, there is accumulating evidence from meta-analyses showing moderate benefits for drug treatments in child psychiatry in the areas of depression, obsessive–compulsive disorder and anxiety disorders, and large effects for ADHD (see below).
Although it is clear that one of the prime aims of pharmaceutical companies is to make profits for their shareholders, the spin-offs are not inconsiderable. For example, one pharmaceutical company has recently set up a UK centre for translational medicine, with huge resources devoted to neuroimaging. The ultimate aim is the development of drug treatments, but it is clear that the concomitant developments in neuroimaging and neuroimaging techniques are likely to lead to considerable insights that will ultimately benefit patients. It has been argued that the pharmaceutical industry invests £3.3 billion in research and development in the UK each year, and in 2003 it contributed a positive trade surplus of £3.6 billion. According to the Association of the British Pharmaceutical Industry (2007), over 300 000 jobs rely on the industry. Furthermore, pharmaceutical companies contribute significantly to non-pharmaceutical research projects (Reference GoodwinGoodwin, 2007).
Pharmaceutical company sponsorship
Worryingly, pharmaceutical company promotions are not limited to doctors – there has also been a growth in the funding of patient groups (Reference Kent and MintesKent & Mintes, 2007). Is it a concern, for instance, that pressure or lobby groups funded by pharmaceutical companies challenge the NICE guidelines over the use of cholinesterase inhibitors in Alzheimer's disease, despite the apparent lack of demonstrable evidence for their efficacy in certain stages of the illness?
Off-label prescription
Until recently, a major problem for child psychiatry has been that a large proportion of the medications we use have been unlicensed. In January 2007 the Paediatric Medicine Regulation came into force. Pharmaceutical companies will be expected to produce a ‘paediatric investigation plan’ (PIP) for all new medicines. These will be submitted to the EMEA's Paediatric Committee, which will be responsible for determining whether the medicine is likely to be of potential therapeutic benefit to children. This will apply to all new drugs unless they are for conditions occurring only in adulthood, or likely to be ineffective or unsafe in children. All results of PIP trials will be held on a EU database. Medicines already in use will need a paediatric marketing authorisation. Companies will be given a financial incentive to carry out the necessary extra studies via a 6-month extension to their supplementary patent protection. This generous inducement should result in more clinical trials involving paediatric patients. Alongside regulatory changes in the USA there has been an upsurge of interest in the practice of paediatric psychopharmacology (Reference DeVeaugh-Geiss, March and ShapiroDeVeaugh-Geiss et al, 2006). Other US initiatives have included the setting up of an infrastructure for paediatric psychopharmacology research (the Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Trials Network, CAPTN) (Research Unit on Pediatric Psychopharmacology Anxiety Study Group, 2001; Reference March, Silva and ComptonMarch et al, 2004a ), as well as government-funded comparative treatment trials of medications and psychotherapy (MTA Cooperative Group, 1999; Reference March, Silva and PetryckiMarch et al, 2004b ; Pediatric OCD Treatment Study Team, 2004).
Safety and efficacy
One of the major concerns for psychopharmacology research is the detection of rarer safety issues, not always detectable by traditional clinical trials and statistical approaches. To establish a statistical link between a treatment and rare events requires impractical sample sizes; alternatives such as long-term follow-up and statistical methods for evaluating selected groups have been suggested (Reference DeVeaugh-Geiss, March and ShapiroDeVeaugh-Geiss et al, 2006). DeVeaugh-Geiss et al argue that in the USA, tight bureaucratic procedures affect drug trials: frequently, acute-dose pharmacokinetic studies have not been completed before initiation of efficacy and safety studies; efficacy and safety trials may be conducted at doses that are inappropriate for demonstrating efficacy or tolerability in children and in adolescents; and population pharmacokinetic trials remain rare.
Previously research and trial studies have been limited typically to efficacy studies, mostly using the gold standard approach of RCTs. Unfortunately, these trials frequently exclude most comorbidity, and are often short-term and too small to identify subgrouping variables or other than common adverse events. Ethical and other considerations have spurred researchers to look at alternatives to the classical RCT (Reference March, Kratochvil and ClarkeMarch et al, 2004c ). Future studies are likely to include: active controlled non-inferiority (or superiority) trials, adaptive designs, group sequential designs, patient preference designs and equipoise-stratified designs (Reference DeVeaugh-Geiss, March and ShapiroDeVeaugh-Geiss et al, 2006). Nevertheless, it is clear that what is also needed is effectiveness trials. The establishment of large, simple effectiveness trials conducted via practical clinical trials networks is therefore a major step forward (Reference March, Silva and ComptonMarch et al, 2005). With sufficient power, large multicentred trials will allow not only the setting up of safety databases to define the risks and tolerability of drugs in the short and long term, but also identification of moderators of treatment outcome.
Another innovation in psychopharmacology research, perhaps belatedly, has been the inclusion of psychotherapy during trials (Reference March, Silva and PetryckiMarch et al, 2004b ; Reference Goodyer, Dubicka and WilkinsonGoodyer et al, 2007). One could argue that, with renewed academic input, regulatory changes and effective trial forums, more reliable psychopharmacology data should be become readily available.
Pharmacotherapy of ADHD
Timimi is correct in pointing to some of the deficiencies in the Multimodal Treatment Study of Children with ADHD (MTA) (MTA Co-operative Group, 2004), which have been incorrectly interpreted by some as a solely positive endorsement for the superiority of drug treatments. However, the treatment of ADHD is one of the most researched areas in medicine, with meta-analyses showing that pharmacological treatment has a substantial effect on the core features of the disorder (attention, impulsivity, hyperactivity) (Reference Crenshaw, Kavale and FornessCrenshaw et al, 1999; Reference Faraone and BiedermanFaraone & Biederman, 2002). The benefits of long-term surveillance is demonstrated by the finding that, although the MTA study showed an advantage of stimulant medication and combination treatments at 14 and 24 months, the 3-year follow-up (Reference Jensen, Arnold and SwansonJensen et al, 2007a ) showed equal benefits for all four treatments – stimulant medication, behavioural therapy, a combination of both, and community care as usual. Interestingly, there was a significant reduction in those taking medication during the follow-up period (a drop from 91% to 71%), and an increase in those undertaking behavioural therapy (from 14% to 45%). A recent meta-analysis (Reference Majewicz-Hefley and CarlsonMajewicz-Hefley & Carlson, 2007) of eight studies has shown large effect sizes for combined treatments, the psychosocial treatments having differing effects on the peripheral features of the disorder (deficits in social skills, academic performance and following directions). The combined treatment approach confirms the recommendations of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry (1997).
Pharmacotherapy of depression
There has been extensive debate about the safety of antidepressants for the treatment of anxiety and depression in children and adolescents. In June 2003 the UK's MHRA and the US FDA advised that paroxetine should not be prescribed for anyone under 18 years of age. After a re-evaluation of data on 4582 paediatric participants in 24 antidepressant trials of 4–16 weeks' duration (23 industry-sponsored trials and the Treatment of Adolescent Depression Study) (Reference Hammad, Laughren and RacoosinHammad et al, 2006) it was concluded that between 1% and 3% of treated patients could be at risk of antidepressant-induced suicidality, although no suicides had occurred.
One of the major methodological shortfalls in the meta-analyses of the relationship between suicide and SSRIs has been the classification of suicidal events, which is haphazard and therefore unreliable. Even with a reliable classification system for suicidal events (Reference Posner, Oquendo and GouldPosner et al, 2007), it is clear that only spontaneously reported adverse events are available for classification. Furthermore, the effect of medication on the threshold for spontaneous reporting between treatments and on actual suicidal ideation and acts remains unclear. Interestingly, however, an increase in the risk for spontaneously reported suicidal events in those taking antidepressant medication was associated with a tendency towards a protective effect against new-onset and worsening suicidal ideation (Reference Hammad, Laughren and RacoosinHammad et al, 2006).
In the USA, a black box warning on the labelling of all antidepressants for use with children and adolescents and a patient medication guide was mandated in 2005. The immediate effect (within 24 months) of the FDA black box warning was a significant reduction in diagnoses of paediatric depression and prescribing of SSRIs to children and adolescents by primary care physicians (Reference Libby, Brent and MorratoLibby et al, 2007). Worryingly, following the FDA black box warning there has been an 18% rise in the US adolescent suicide rate (Reference Hamilton, Minino and MartinHamilton et al, 2007). These findings need to be set in the context of the indirect evidence of an association between the increased prescription of SSRIs and a reduced youth suicide rate (Reference Olfson, Shaffer and MarcusOlfson et al, 2003; Reference Gibbons, Hur and BhaumikGibbons et al, 2006), indicating a protective effect of SSRIs against suicide.
The largest and most recent meta-analysis of 27 RCTs of antidepressant treatments involving 5310 children and adolescents (Reference Bridge, Iyengar and SalaryBridge et al, 2007) looked at major depressive disorder, anxiety disorder and obsessive–compulsive disorder. According to the study there was a small response rate (pooled risk differences) for major depressive disorder (11.0%; 95% CI 7.1–14.9), and a larger response for obsessive–compulsive disorder (19.8%; 95% CI 13.0–26.6), with the largest response for anxiety disorder (37.1%; 95% CI 22.5–51.7). The corresponding numbers needed to treat were 10 (95% CI 7–15), 6 (95% CI 4–8), and 3 (95% CI 2–5) respectively. There was increased risk difference of suicidal ideation/suicide attempt across all trials for drug v. placebo (0.7%; 95% CI 0.1–1.3), with a number needed to harm of 143 (95% CI 77–1000). Importantly, there were no completed suicides and, as has been pointed out, the rate of suicidal ideation in the trials is low and comparable to that seen in a normal adolescent population (Reference JamesJames, 2005). Age-stratified analyses showed that for children under 12 years old with major depressive disorder, only fluoxetine showed benefit over placebo, which accords with the current prescribing guidelines.
An important question is whether combination treatments are more effective. The NICE guidelines for the treatment of depression in children and adolescents (National Collaborating Centre for Mental Health, 2005), which Timimi does not mention, recommend psychological treatments as first line for mild and moderate depression. However, although the Treatment of Adolescent Depression Study showed the benefits of combined CBT and fluoxetine (Reference March, Silva and VitielloMarch et al, 2006), a more recent study from Cambridge and Manchester, which was more representative of routine clinical practice in child and adolescent mental health services (Reference Goodyer, Dubicka and WilkinsonGoodyer et al, 2007) demonstrated that in moderate and severe depression pharmacological treatment with an SSRI alone is effective, and CBT confers no additional benefit. This accords with the view expressed by Reference GoodwinGoodwin (2007) that drug–placebo differences seem to be magnified with illness severity, whereas psychotherapy tends to look less effective as illness severity increases.
Conclusions
It is clear that restoration of public and medical confidence in the pharmaceutical industry will require changes in practice. There is agreement that, at the very least, disclosure of researchers' interests and all trial outcomes are necessary. A few further suggestions could be made.
First, in the UK a strong case could be made for the setting up of academic departments of child psychopharmacology. These would be involved in conducting drug trials and disseminating findings. Such departments could be partly funded through NHS research and development schemes and partly by the pharmaceutical industry. The latter funding would need to be open and subject to scrutiny. Already there is a Centre for Interventional Paediatric Psychopharmacology at Great Ormond Street Hospital for Children in London.
Second, the International Committee of Medical Journal Editors (ICMJE) initiated a policy in 2005 requiring investigators to deposit information about trial design into an accepted clinical trials registry before any patient involvement (Reference Laine and HortonLaine et al, 2007). Despite initial hesitation, the story is of considerable success. For example 1 month after this policy came into effect, registrations with ClinicalTrials.gov (run by the US National Institutes of Health) had increased from 13 153 to 22 714, and the number now stands at over 40 000. The World Health Organization is supporting an International Clinical Trials Registry Platform (ICTRP), which will include preliminary pre-trial pharmacokinetic studies. As regards publication, the ICMJE favours a standard abstract-reporting process, which is likely to be supported in forthcoming guidelines from the Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) group (Reference Keech, Gebski and PikeKeech et al, 2007). Prior to publication full disclosure of all related postings in registries will be required. Hopefully this will lead to a reduction in misinformation and, as a recent BMJ editorial (Reference Laine and HortonLaine et al, 2007) states, ‘will [help] to assure trial participants that the information that accrues as a result of their altruism will become part of the public record’.
Third, it is clear that no matter how rigorous clinical trials are, the number of participants involved is relatively small. Post-trial drug surveillance is dependent on practitioners reporting adverse effects using systems such as the UK's Yellow Card Scheme. Perhaps we are all guilty of underreporting, otherwise how could the SSRI story have gone undetected for so long? There is a welcomed proposal, supported by the Mental Health Research Network, to set up a national child and adolescent psychiatric surveillance system in the UK.
One hopes that the ongoing debate regarding drug treatments will have beneficial effects. One example is the publication of evidence-based treatment algorithms (Reference Hughes, Emslie and CrismonHughes et al, 2007). Nevertheless, caution needs to be exercised when extending drug treatments outside diagnostic groupings, as for instance in the management of impulsive aggression. It is welcome, then, that a recent academic review highlighted this particular issue and offered advice to guide future research strategies (Reference Jensen, Youngstrom and SteinerJensen et al, 2007b ).
It would be naïve and probably wrong to say that all research misinformation is confined to drug trials. Side-effects and problems with treatment are not confined to drugs and occur in psychological treatments as well, but these are rarely, if ever, reported. Undoubtedly they should be.
Timimi's article has highlighted a number of extremely important issues. One welcomes the opportunity to critically examine the role of psychopharmacology in child and adolescent psychiatry. It is evident that some progress has been made in addressing very legitimate concerns; however, there is still some way to go.
Declaration of interest
A. J. has worked as a consultant to Sanofi Aventis and works collaboratively on a neuroimaging project with researchers from GlaxoSmithKline.
eLetters
No eLetters have been published for this article.