INTRODUCTION
Declarative memories are categorized as episodic, referring to an autonoetic conscious recollection of a specific experience, or semantic, referring to concept-based knowledge-of-the-world in which the “self” is not present (Tulving, Reference Tulving1983, Reference Tulving2002; Tulving & Markowitsch, Reference Tulving and Markowitsch1998). Neuroimaging evidence supports the existence of separate but highly interconnected semantic and episodic memory systems during encoding, consolidation, and retrieval (Burianova & Grady, Reference Burianova and Grady2007; Cabeza & Nyberg, Reference Cabeza and Nyberg2000; Lepage et al., Reference Lepage, Habib, Cormier, Houle and McIntosh2000; Moscovitch et al., Reference Moscovitch, Rosenbaum, Gilboa, Addis, Westmacott, Grady, McAndrews, Levine, Black, Winocur and Nadel2005; Prince et al., Reference Prince, Daselaar and Cabeza2005, Reference Prince, Tsukiura and Cabeza2007; Rajah & McIntosh, Reference Rajah and McIntosh2005; Wiggs et al., Reference Wiggs, Weisberg and Martin1999). It is hypothesized that episodic encoding is critically dependent upon the integrity of the semantic memory system, because all information encoded into episodic memory first passes through and activates the semantic memory system (Nyberg et al., Reference Nyberg, Cabeza and Tulving1996; Prince et al., Reference Prince, Tsukiura and Cabeza2007; Tulving et al., Reference Tulving, Kapur, Craik, Moscovitch and Houle1994; Tulving & Markowitsch, Reference Tulving and Markowitsch1998). Episodic memory tasks instructing subjects to elaborate on the semantic features of objects during learning result in increased encoding (Craik & Tulving, Reference Craik and Tulving1975; Schulman, Reference Schulman1971), presumably by increasing semantic retrieval beyond the baseline level normally invoked in episodic tasks without such instructions. Therefore, it is reasoned that greater activation of the semantic memory system can increase episodic encoding and enhance subsequent episodic retrieval.
Presentation of emotionally negative material during an episodic memory task activates the amygdala (Breiter et al., Reference Breiter, Etcoff, Whalen, Kennedy, Rauch, Buckner, Strauss, Hyman and Rosen1996; Hariri et al., Reference Hariri, Tessitore, Mattay, Fera and Weinberger2002; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Frith, Perrett, Rowland, Young, Calder and Dolan1996) and stimulates the central norepinephrine (NE) system (McGaugh et al., Reference McGaugh, Cahill and Roozendaal1996; Strange & Dolan, Reference Strange and Dolan2004), which then increases attentional orienting (Anderson & Phelps, Reference Anderson and Phelps2001; Öhman et al., Reference Öhman, Flykt and Esteves2001) and enhances encoding through interaction with the hippocampal/medial temporal lobe complex (HC/MTL) (Dolcos et al., Reference Dolcos, LaBar and Cabeza2004; Phelps, Reference Phelps2004; Richardson et al., Reference Richardson, Strange and Dolan2004); increasing episodic memory performance for the emotional material (Cahill et al., Reference Cahill, Prins, Weber and McGaugh1994, Reference Cahill, Haier, Fallon, Alkire, Tang, Keator, Wu and McGaugh1996; Cahill & McGaugh, Reference Cahill and McGaugh1995). Accordingly, amygdala lesions (Adolphs et al., Reference Adolphs, Cahill, Schul and Babinsky1997; Brierley et al., Reference Brierley, Medford, Shaw and David2004; Markowitsch et al., Reference Markowitsch, Calabrese, Würker, Durwen, Kessler, Babinsky, Brechtelsbauer, Heuser and Gehlen1994) or administration of centrally acting β-adrenergic receptor antagonists (Cahill et al., Reference Cahill, Prins, Weber and McGaugh1994; van Stegeren et al., Reference van Stegeren, Everaerd, Cahill, McGaugh and Gooren1998) reduce memory enhancements attributable to emotional arousal. Because declarative memory is processed by the HC/MTL at the level of encoding (Cabeza & Nyberg, Reference Cabeza and Nyberg2000; Mayes & Montaldi, Reference Mayes and Montaldi1999), emotionally arousing material that activate the amygdala should also produce enhancements during encoding for semantic and episodic memories, alike. However, because episodic memory involuntarily elicits retrieval of semantic information (Cabeza & Nyberg, Reference Cabeza and Nyberg2000; Nyberg et al., Reference Nyberg, Cabeza and Tulving1996; Tulving et al., Reference Tulving, Kapur, Craik, Moscovitch and Houle1994) examining the modulatory influence of emotion on semantic memory is especially challenging.
We propose using a modified oddball task to examine the effects of emotional arousal on semantic memory activation and subsequent contributions of semantic memory to episodic free-recall. Oddball tasks rely upon successful detection of deviant “oddball” stimuli among homogeneous “standard” stimuli to produce an attentional orienting response and enhanced encoding (Strange et al., Reference Strange, Henson, Friston and Dolan2000), which increases subsequent recall of the deviant stimuli; an effect known as the von Restorff phenomenon (for review, see Wallace, Reference Wallace1965). Oddball studies that serially present standard stimuli and randomly insert oddballs have previously been used to demonstrate enhanced episodic free-recall for emotional oddballs, and disrupted consolidation for neutral stimuli preceding an emotional oddball (Hurlemann et al., Reference Hurlemann, Hawellek, Matusch, Kolsch, Wollersen, Madea, Vogeley, Maier and Dolan2005; Miu et al., Reference Miu, Heilman, Opre and Miclea2005; Strange et al., Reference Strange, Hurlemann and Dolan2003; Tulving, Reference Tulving1969). Increased encoding and successful remembering of the emotionally negative oddball predicts the disrupted consolidation and subsequent forgetting of the preceding neutral stimulus (Strange et al., Reference Strange, Hurlemann and Dolan2003). Due to the facilitative role of semantic memory in episodic free-recall, and naturalistic accounts of increased memory for the moments preceding traumatic events (Ehlers et al., Reference Ehlers, Hackmann and Michael2004, Reference Ehlers, Hackmann, Steil, Clohessy, Wenninger and Winter2002), we will examine whether a semantic relationship between an oddball and preceding stimuli can reverse disrupted consolidation and be modulated by negative emotion, resulting in greater episodic free-recall.
To investigate the interaction between semantic relationships and emotion, two structural modifications to the previously described oddball tasks are performed for the present task to study the interactions between emotion and semantic and episodic memory. First, the semantic relationship between the oddball and the immediately preceding word is manipulated, either related or unrelated. Most of the relationships used for this study are associative relationships, such as burn-fire, or lexical co-occurrences, such as cockroach-dirt (Lund & Burgess, Reference Lund and Burgess1996; Moss et al., Reference Moss, Ostrin, Tyler and Marslen-Wilson1995). Semantic relationships such as these are used extensively in priming studies to show that the presentation of one word facilitates the retrieval of semantically related counterparts (Nelson & Goodmon, Reference Nelson and Goodmon2002; Tulving & Schacter, Reference Tulving and Schacter1990). Next, the emotional impact of the oddball and preceding word is manipulated to include either emotionally negative (E) or neutral (N) words, yielding four conditions; an E word preceding an E oddball, N preceding an E oddball, E preceding an N oddball, and an N preceding an N oddball. Therefore, emotional intensity can be modulated for the presentation of either word (oddball or preceding word), while the relative activation of semantic memory is only modulated upon the presentation of the oddball word, at which time a semantic relationship will or will not be evident.
Using the modified serial-presentation oddball task, we hypothesize that semantically related word pairs will increase semantic memory activation upon the presentation of second word in the associated pair, the oddball, resulting in greater free-recall of both words. Furthermore, we propose that upon presentation of emotionally negative oddballs for related word pairs, semantic memory encoding will be enhanced versus neutral oddballs. Words preceding an emotional oddball will benefit and be recalled significantly better than those preceding neutral oddballs, because increased encoding due to the semantic associations between words will occur in semantic memory. Such an effect will demonstrate the ability of emotion and semantic memory to interact to mutually enhance episodic free-recall.
METHODS
Participants
Twenty-eight paid subjects (14 male, 14 female; average age of 22.4 ± 2.5 years; average education level of 15.6 ± 1.5 years) were solicited through online advertisements and participated in this study after written informed consent. No participants offered a history of neurological or psychiatric illness or were currently taking medications to treat such illnesses. This protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of The Ohio State University.
Stimuli
Eight words, including exactly 1 oddball word, comprised each of 48 word lists. Within each list, a perceptual oddball word was inserted, in pseudorandom manner, at serial position 4, 5, 6, or 7. The oddball text was lime green in color, while standard stimuli text was white; both presented on a black background with all other perceptual characteristics remaining constant. Oddball words (W), and the words immediately preceding the oddball in each list (W−1) were chosen from the affective norms for English words (ANEW) (Bradley & Lang, Reference Bradley and Lang1999), with the exception of five words that did not originate from the ANEW list, but were chosen by the experimenter to create meaningful associative relationships, as later described. The remaining six words in each list (288 total) consisted of unique, randomly chosen, general and picturable nouns from Ogden's Basic Word List (Ogden, Reference Ogden1932) and were not strongly associated with W or W−1. The emotionality of the words chosen from the ANEW (positions W−1 and W) and their associative relationship to one another yielded 8 different experimental list-type conditions, as demonstrated in Table 1.
Procedure
Participants performed the experiment in a well-lit room while seated across from the experimenter. Using E-prime software (Psychological Software Tools, Inc., Pittsburgh, PA) the words were presented on a 15-inch diagonal screen. Instructions and a practice list were administered before beginning the experiment. During the experiment, words in each list were serially presented on a black background for 1 s each. Between each word, a 1-s rest slide consisting of a white “+” on a black background was presented, resulting in a stimulus onset asynchrony of 2 s. An audible tone indicated the end of a list and the beginning of a 20-s distractor task used to reduce the primacy and recency effects commonly observed in studies of immediate free-recall (Jahnke, Reference Jahnke1965). This distractor task required participants to continually subtract 7 from a random three-digit number presented on screen, after which, another audible tone sounded, prompting participants to orally “Please Recall” as many words from the most recently presented list as possible, in any order. On screen instructions also prompted participants to hit the space bar to advance to the next list when finished with free-recall.
The 48 lists of words were presented to each participant in two pseudorandomly ordered blocks of 24 lists, counterbalanced across subject, with an optional 3-min break offered between the blocks. Following presentation of all 48 lists, ratings of arousal and relatedness were taken for each word pair presented in Table 1. During the rating tasks, both words were presented simultaneously on screen, separated by a hyphen, while word order within each pair was consistent with presentation order during the memory task. Arousal for the word pairs was measured using a 9-point version of the arousal portion of the Standard Assessment Manikin (SAM) (Bradley & Lang, Reference Bradley and Lang1994). The SAM nonverbally measures arousal using a pictorially-based scale, therefore reducing cultural language bias. Relatedness for the word pairs was measured using a 9-point scale, where 1 corresponded to “unrelated” and 9 corresponded to “related.” This task concluded the experiment and participants were debriefed and released.
Analysis
Preliminary analyses and analysis design
SPSS was used for data analysis. To ensure homogeneous distribution of arousing words across list-type conditions, subjective arousal ratings from Bradley & Lang (Reference Bradley and Lang1999) for ANEW words (positions W−1 and W) were compared using independent sample t tests before beginning the study. Emotion – emotion (EE) and neutral – neutral (NN) word pairs were compared across the relatedness condition, while emotion – neutral (EN) and neutral – emotion (NE) word pairs were compared with each other within and across the relatedness condition.
A one-way ANOVA examining overall memory performance across each list-type condition was performed. Any significant difference in free-recall performance across list-type conditions would require that free-recall scores be adjusted and reported as a percentage of control word (C) performance for each condition. Control word performance for each participant was established by assessing delayed free-recall memory on a random word from any position in each list, excluding positions W−1, W, or W+1 (48 instances per participant; 1344 total instances). Using these difference scores, a 4 × 2 × 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA (position × associative relatedness × emotionality of W × emotionality of W−1) was performed to examine delayed free-recall memory performance. The first variable, position, considered the serial position of words relative to the oddball, including control word performance; W−1, W, W+1, and C. The second variable considered the semantic relationship between W and W−1; either semantically related or unrelated. The final two variables considered the emotionality of W and W−1; either emotionally negative or neutral.
To examine relative enhancements due specifically to semantic relatedness, unrelated free-recall performance was subtracted from related free-recall performance for each of the EE, NE, EN, and NN conditions within each serial position. It was assumed that semantic contributions to episodic free-recall could be obtained by subtracting performance in the unrelated condition from performance in the related condition. Because the subtraction left only enhancements due to semantic memory contributions and we are only interested in a single serial position for these analyses, a 2 × 2 ANOVA resulted from the remaining factors (emotionality of W × emotionality of W−1). Where applicable, significant main and interaction effects were further analyzed using Bonferroni-corrected independent-sample t tests and corrected p values are reported.
Emotional modulation of semantic memory and contributions to episodic free recall
Using the 4 × 2 × 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA described above, we examined semantic memory contributions to episodic free-recall. A significant main effect for relatedness and significant interaction effects for position × relatedness, specifically increased free-recall memory performance for associatively related words in both the W−1 and W positions would verify our hypothesis that semantic memory activation significantly contributes to episodic free-recall.
A significant interaction effect for position × relatedness × W emotion using the 4 × 2 × 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA described above would be consistent with our hypothesis that emotionally negative words can modulate semantic memory during encoding, and that emotional modulation of semantic memory can contribute to episodic free-recall. However, subsequent analyses examining this significant interaction for serial positions W and W−1 using the above described 2 × 2 ANOVA would be necessary to confirm each hypothesis. For stimuli in the W position, increased free-recall for emotional oddballs, as indicated by a significant main effect for W emotionality, would confirm our hypothesis that semantic memory is modulated by emotion, similar to episodic memory. For related stimuli in the W−1 position, increased recall of words preceding negative oddballs, as indicated by a significant main effect for W emotionality, would indicate that increased semantic memory encoding due to emotionally negative stimuli contributes to episodic free-recall.
Subjective ratings of arousal and relatedness
Subjective ratings of arousal and relatedness were analyzed using a 2 × 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA (relatedness × emotionality of W × emotionality of W−1). Significant main effects for the emotionality of W or W−1 showing greater ratings of arousal would indicate higher subjective arousal for the arousing stimuli. A significant main effect for relatedness would indicate that participants reported greater amounts of relatedness for related stimuli.
RESULTS
Preliminary Analyses
An a priori analysis of subjective arousal ratings from Bradley & Lang (Reference Bradley and Lang1999) found no significant differences across the compared experimental conditions (related vs. unrelated and EN vs. NE), thus ensuring homogeneous distribution of arousing words. Overall free-recall performance significantly differed across each list-type condition, F(1,7) = 2.08; p < .05, which required free-recall scores to be adjusted and reported as a proportion of control word performance. No primacy or recency effects were observed for control words.
Emotional Modulation of Semantic Memory and Contributions to Episodic Free Recall
The 4 × 2 × 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA examining overall memory performance revealed a significant main effect for serial word position relative to the oddball, F(3,81) = 104.17; p < .001, as shown in Figure 1. Further analysis revealed that oddball words (W) were remembered significantly better than control words (C), t(446) = 12.64; p < .001, verifying our ability to reproduce the von Restorff effect. A significant decrease in free-recall is observed for words following the oddball, t(446) = −3.77; p = .006. A significant main effect was also found for relatedness, F(1,27) = 57.74; p < .001, which indicated that semantic activation significantly contributed to episodic free-recall. Bonferroni-corrected t tests examining related versus unrelated stimuli in serial positions W−1 and W revealed significantly increased free-recall for related stimuli, t(110) = 3.086; p= .041 for W−1 and t(110) = 3.189; p = .030 for W.
Consistent with our hypothesis that emotionally negative words can enhance semantic contributions to episodic free-recall during encoding, a significant position × associative relatedness × W emotionality interaction effect was revealed by the 4 × 2 × 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA, F(3,81) = 3.16; p = .029. Further examination of stimuli in the W position, using the above described 2 × 2 ANOVA for free-recall enhancements due to semantic contributions, revealed a significant main effect of W emotionality, F(1,27) = 5.94; p = .022, confirming our hypothesis that emotionally negative material enhanced semantic contributions to episodic memory, as seen in Figure 2. Using the 2 × 2 ANOVA examining enhancements due to semantic memory, a significant main effect of W emotionality was also observed upon for stimuli in the W−1 position, F(1,27) = 13.54; p = .001. This result, portrayed in Figure 3, confirmed our hypothesis that emotional modulation of semantic memory encoding significantly contributed to the episodic free-recall of W−1 stimuli.
Subjective Ratings of Arousal and Relatedness
Word pairs containing one or more emotionally arousing word were rated significantly more arousing, regardless of whether the emotional word appeared in the W or W−1 position, F(1,27) = 62.50; p < .001 and F(1,27) = 55.19; p < .001, respectively. Analysis of subjective relatedness ratings revealed that related word pairs were rated significantly more related than unrelated pairs, F(1,27) = 1175.50; p < .001. Analysis of subjective arousal ratings revealed that related word pairs were rated significantly more arousing by participants, F(1,27) = 17.69; p < .001, despite no significant differences across lists on preliminary analyses of subjective arousal using data from Bradley & Lang (Reference Bradley and Lang1999). Likewise, the appearance of an emotional word in the W or W−1 position resulted in significantly greater ratings of relatedness, F(1,27) = 25.81; p < .001 and F(1,27) = 47.50; p < .001, respectively. This finding was likely driven by the significantly increased subjective relatedness ratings for the emotional W × emotional W−1 condition versus all other unrelated conditions, p < .001 for all pairwise comparisons. Figures 4a and 4b summarize obtained subjective rating data.
DISCUSSION
The present study demonstrates that semantic memory and negative emotion can interact to mutually increase episodic free-recall. It has long been known that activation of semantic memory during episodic encoding can greatly increase later episodic free-recall or recognition (Craik & Tulving, Reference Craik and Tulving1975; Schulman, Reference Schulman1971). However, the use of a serial presentation design during the present study allows for the demonstration of what appears to be a retroactive episodic memory increase due to contributions by the semantic memory system interacting with negative emotion. That is, free-recall enhancements for the earlier presented word are dependent upon the presentation of a second semantically related word which is further modulated by negative emotion. Negative emotion can also decrease free-recall for words following oddballs, as seen in Figure 1, similar to the reductions observed in previous studies (Hurlemann et al., Reference Hurlemann, Hawellek, Matusch, Kolsch, Wollersen, Madea, Vogeley, Maier and Dolan2005; Miu et al., Reference Miu, Heilman, Opre and Miclea2005).
Two possible factors could significantly contribute to a retroactive memory increase for semantically related words. Presentation of the second word might bolster the consolidation of the earlier presented word within episodic memory. Alternatively, presentation of the second related word results in greater semantic memory activation, creating stronger semantic memory cues that are available upon elicitation of episodic free-recall. Findings from the present study favor the latter explanation: previous oddball tasks demonstrate that items immediately preceding emotional oddballs are subject to consolidation disruptions (Hurlemann et al., Reference Hurlemann, Hawellek, Matusch, Kolsch, Wollersen, Madea, Vogeley, Maier and Dolan2005; Miu et al., Reference Miu, Heilman, Opre and Miclea2005; Strange et al., Reference Strange, Hurlemann and Dolan2003; Tulving, Reference Tulving1969). However, we find that words preceding and semantically related to negative oddballs are actually recalled better than those preceding and related to neutral oddballs, while both are recalled significantly better than words not semantically related to the oddball. It seems less likely that a bi-directional modulation for consolidation occurs based solely on the presence of a semantic relationship, especially because the presence of a semantic relationship does not manifest until the presentation of the second word. Instead, we find it more likely that when episodic encoding activates the retrieval of semantic information (Nyberg et al., Reference Nyberg, Cabeza and Tulving1996; Prince et al., Reference Prince, Tsukiura and Cabeza2007; Tulving et al., Reference Tulving, Kapur, Craik, Moscovitch and Houle1994; Tulving & Markowitsch, Reference Tulving and Markowitsch1998), it does so to varying degrees, based on the semantic associativity between the two items. Upon episodic memory retrieval, the semantic network is concurrently activated, and similar to the spreading-activation theory of semantic processing (Collins & Loftus, Reference Collins and Loftus1975), recall of any given word facilitates the recall of a second word to a degree proportional to the semantic relatedness between the two words. In summary, semantic memory activation occurs concurrently with episodic encoding and varies in intensity as a function of the relatedness between any two items within the semantic network on verbal tasks such as ours.
Negative emotion appears to modulate semantic memory in a manner consistent with that seen for episodic memory contributions. If emotionally negative material simply increases episodic encoding, it is expected that related words preceding the oddballs are not influenced by the emotional impact of the later presented oddballs. In the present study, however, increased episodic free-recall is observed for both emotionally negative oddballs and related words preceding the emotionally negative oddball, as compared to those related words preceding neutral oddballs (Figures 3 and 4). According to subjective ratings of arousal collected for this study, it also appears that semantic relationships can significantly increase arousal ratings (Figure 4a), perhaps further contributing to episodic free-recall.
The finding by Strange et al. (Reference Strange, Hurlemann and Dolan2003), demonstrating that the recall of the emotional word inhibited rather than facilitated the recall of the previous neutral word, appear to be in stark contrast to the above conclusion. However, the control words used by Strange et al. (Reference Strange, Hurlemann and Dolan2003) all shared a semantic category, while the emotional oddball words shared only a weak semantic relationship to the preceding neutral words and had no incongruent perceptual characteristic. This differs significantly from the present study in which control words were purposely not semantically related, while the emotional oddball and preceding word could share strong semantic ties. Additionally, all oddballs were perceptually incongruent from the standard stimuli, as they were lime green in color, while the emotional oddballs also differed in their emotionality. According to studies of proactive inhibition, continued presentation of words within a single semantic category results in a steady decline in free-recall memory until that proactive inhibition is released by changing a semantic component (Wickens, Reference Wickens1970). Therefore, in the study by Strange et al. (Reference Strange, Hurlemann and Dolan2003), the change in emotional impact upon the presentation of the weakly associated emotionally negative word could initiate a release from proactive inhibition, increasing free-recall for the emotional word, while preceding words show a free-recall decline. As discussed below, varying latencies between stimuli in the study by Strange et al. (Reference Strange, Hurlemann and Dolan2003) and the present study may also account for the observed differences.
Recall for serially presented information is also significantly influenced by the latency of the onset between serially presented stimuli, or SOA. For serial-presentation oddball studies, it appears as if a shorter SOA increases memory impairments for words preceding the oddball (Tulving, Reference Tulving1969). With the exception of the study by Tulving (Reference Tulving1969), each of the previously mentioned studies that found memory decrements attributable to the oddball used an SOA that was markedly longer than the 2 s SOA used in the present study, ranging from 3 s (Miu et al., Reference Miu, Heilman, Opre and Miclea2005; Strange et al., Reference Strange, Hurlemann and Dolan2003) to 5 s (Hurlemann et al., Reference Hurlemann, Hawellek, Matusch, Kolsch, Wollersen, Madea, Vogeley, Maier and Dolan2005). The shorter 2-s latency in the present study would, therefore, suggest that stimuli preceding the oddball are within a range susceptible to memory disruptions attributable to the oddball, but the presence of semantic relationships are able to mitigate those disruptions. Additionally, the SOA in the present study is sufficiently longer than the 550 ms required for the recognition of associative relationships and later declarative memory enhancements (Fischler & Goodman, Reference Fischler and Goodman1978).
It is demonstrated through the use of neuroimaging techniques that semantic memory is intimately involved in the encoding and retrieval of episodic memories (Nyberg et al., Reference Nyberg, Cabeza and Tulving1996; Prince et al., Reference Prince, Tsukiura and Cabeza2007; Tulving et al., Reference Tulving, Kapur, Craik, Moscovitch and Houle1994; Tulving & Markowitsch, Reference Tulving and Markowitsch1998). Therefore, understanding the modulatory role of emotion on semantic memory can contribute to the understanding of episodic memory formation and retrieval. Interactions between emotion and memory comprise fundamental components of several psychological disorders, such as intrusive memory symptoms in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Ehlers et al., Reference Ehlers, Hackmann, Steil, Clohessy, Wenninger and Winter2002, Reference Ehlers, Hackmann and Michael2004). Using the proposed oddball task on such populations might be beneficial in elucidating fundamental disturbances in interactions between emotion and semantic or episodic memory. Future work also needs to consider the temporal characteristics of the enhancements observed here. Changes in the SOA or time between learning and retrieval are likely to further clarify the roles of encoding versus consolidation. Institution of “know” versus “remember” designations (Tulving, Reference Tulving1985; Yonelinas, Reference Yonelinas2002) for delayed recognition memory using this task could also help determine the relative contribution of semantic activation to episodic memory. An overall understanding of naturalistic memory formation will require a better appreciation of contributions and interactions between memory systems studied in the laboratory.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This research was funded in part by grants from NIDA (R21 DA015734) (Beversdorf) and NINDS (K23 NS43222) (Beversdorf). No financial or other relationships currently exist that could be interpreted as a conflict of interest affecting this manuscript. Portions of this research were presented at the Cognitive Neuroscience Society, 2007.