Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T21:11:23.133Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Thinking dispositions and cognitive reflection performance in schizotypy

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2023

Annabel Broyd*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of East London, Water Lane, London, E15 4LZ
Ulrich Ettinger
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Bonn, Germany
Volker Thoma
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of East London, UK
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Schizotypy refers to the continuum of normal variability of psychosis-like characteristics and experiences, often classified as positive schizotypy (‘unusual experiences’; UE) and negative schizotypy (‘introvertive anhedonia’; IA). Here, we investigated the link between schizotypy and cognitive processing style and performance. A particular focus was on whether schizotypy is associated more with Type 1 (automatic/heuristic) than Type 2 (reflective/effortful) processes, as may be expected from findings of impaired top-down control in schizophrenia. A large sample (n = 1,512) completed online measures pertaining to schizotypy (Oxford-Liverpool Inventory for Feelings and Experiences; O-LIFE), thinking style (Rational Experiential Inventory-10, Actively Open-Minded Thinking Scale), and reasoning performance (Cognitive Reflection Test). Higher positive (UE) and negative (IA) schizotypy were associated with more pronounced Type 1 processing, i.e. greater self-reported Faith in Intuition (FI), lower Need for Cognition (NFC), lower Actively Open-Minded Thinking (AOT), and lower cognitive reflection test (CRT) scores. Canonical correlation analysis confirmed a significant association between UE and increased FI, lower AOT and lower CRT performance, accounting for 12.38% of the shared variance between schizotypy and thinking dispositions. IA was more highly associated with reduced NFC. These findings suggest that schizotypy may be associated with similar thinking dispositions to those reported in psychosis, with different patterns of associations for positive and negative schizotypy. This result informs research on reasoning processes in psychosis and has clinical implications, including potential treatment targets and refinements for cognitive therapies.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
The authors license this article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License.
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors [2019] This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

1 Introduction

Schizotypy refers to a continuum of psychosis-like characteristics and experiences, ranging in intensity from low to states that might be observed in clinical psychosis (Ettinger, Meyhöfer, Steffens, Wagner & Koutsouleris, Reference Ettinger, Meyhöfer, Steffens, Wagner and Koutsouleris2014). Schizotypy is thought to encompass a range of human experiences and traits including unusual beliefs and perceptions, magical thinking, anhedonia and introversion (Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Mohanty, Herrington, Koven, Miller and Heller2004), and has been linked to creativity and artistic pursuits (Acar, Chen & Cayirdag, Reference Acar, Chen and Cayirdag2018; Burch, Pavelis, Hemsley & Corr, Reference Burch, Pavelis, Hemsley and Corr2006; O’Reilly, Dunbar & Bentall, Reference O’Reilly, Dunbar and Bentall2001), but has also been found to be associated with deficits in cognitive task performance (Cohen, Mohr, Ettinger, Chan & Park, Reference Cohen, Mohr, Ettinger, Chan and Park2015).

Positive and negative schizotypy are considered to be the two most reliable dimensions of schizotypy (Kwapil, Barrantes-Vidal & Silvia, Reference Kwapil, Barrantes-Vidal and Silvia2008), although other factors such as cognitive disorganisation and impulsive non-conformity have also been proposed (Mason, Reference Mason and Claridge2006). Positive schizotypy refers to unusual experiences, perceptions, beliefs and magical thinking, while negative schizotypy refers to experiences such as anhedonia (attenuated ability to experience pleasure) and introversion (Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Mohanty, Herrington, Koven, Miller and Heller2004). Schizotypy arguably captures the range, heterogeneity and multi-dimensionality (Barrantes-Vidal, Grant & Kwapil, Reference Barrantes-Vidal, Grant and Kwapil2015) of psychosis and psychosis-like experiences. Furthermore, investigation of schizotypy as a trait may circumvent the confounding effects of medication, which can have global sedative effects that can greatly impede validity when exploring cognition (Barnes & McPhillips, Reference Barnes and McPhillips1999; Lambert et al., Reference Lambert, Conus, Eid, Mass, Karow, Moritz and Naber2004).

Dual-process theories of thinking and decision-making have become ubiquitous and influential within cognitive psychology, and are supported by considerable empirical evidence (Evans, Barston & Pollard, Reference Evans, Barston and Pollard1983; Klauer, Musch & Naumer, Reference Klauer, Musch and Naumer2000). Within these frameworks, ‘Type 1’ processing is thought to be fast, intuitive and automatic, relying on heuristics (i.e. mental shortcuts) or ‘gut feelings’, and out of conscious cognitive control, while ‘Type 2’ processing is considered to be slow, reflective, effortful, and generally more rational (Bryan & Harter, Reference Bryan and Harter1899; Evans, Reference Evans2003, Reference Evans2008, Reference Evans2010; Shiffrin & Schneider, Reference Shiffrin and Schneider1977).

Importantly, dual process theories have been integrated into psychosis-related frameworks, particularly in relation to positive symptoms such as delusional beliefs and persecutory ideation. In general, the proposition is that a combination of over-reliance on Type 1 reasoning alongside a lower propensity to use Type 2 reflective reasoning is associated with the maintenance of unusual or distressing beliefs (Freeman, Evans & Lister, Reference Freeman, Evans and Lister2012; Ward & Garety, in press; Ward, Peters, Jackson, Day & Garety, Reference Ward and Garety2018). For example, in people with delusional ideation, Freeman and colleagues (Reference Freeman, Evans and Lister2012) identified a positive association between paranoid thoughts and intuitive thinking, while higher levels of deliberative thinking were associated with fewer paranoid thoughts. People with delusional beliefs have also been found to exhibit a cognitive disposition towards lower belief flexibility than people without delusions (Colbert, Peters & Garety, Reference Colbert, Peters and Garety2010), which may indicate a less flexible and open-minded reasoning style. Indeed, both delusion-proneness (Bronstein, Pennycook, Bear, Rand & Cannon, in press) and reduced data gathering (Ross et al., Reference Ross, Pennycook, McKay, Gervais, Langdon and Coltheart2016) have been associated with a less analytical thinking style. Furthermore, intuitive (Type 1) thinking has also been associated with parapsychological/anomalous experiences (Irwin & Wilson, Reference Irwin and Wilson2013) as well as paranormal explanations for anomalous experiences (Irwin & Wilson, Reference Irwin and Wilson2013; Ross, Hartig & McKay, Reference Ross, Hartig and McKay2017).

Studies also suggest that people with psychosis demonstrate a greater propensity for a ‘jumping to conclusions’ (JTC) bias than non-clinical subjects (Garety, Kuipers, Fowler, Freeman & Bebbington, Reference Garety, Kuipers, Fowler, Freeman and Bebbington2001; So, Siu, Wong, Chan & Garety, Reference So, Siu, Wong, Chan and Garety2016). This bias refers to the tendency to make hastier decisions and/or decisions with greater conviction (Huq, Garety & Hemsley, Reference Huq, Garety and Hemsley1988). Two independent meta-analyses have identified a more prominent JTC bias in people with psychosis than control participants (Dudley, Taylor & Wickham, Reference Dudley, Taylor and Wickham2015; McLean, Mattiske & Balzan, Reference McLean, Mattiske and Balzan2017) as well as an increased JTC bias in people with delusions vs. those without delusions in psychosis (Dudley et al., Reference Dudley, Taylor and Wickham2015; McLean et al., Reference McLean, Mattiske and Balzan2017). Another meta-analysis (Ross, McKay, Coltheart & Langdon, Reference Ross, McKay, Coltheart and Langdon2015) found an association between delusions and the JTC bias in delusion-prone samples. Investigating the mechanisms involved in JTC, Glöckner and Moritz (Reference Glöckner and Moritz2009) found that more extreme confidence ratings were endorsed by people with schizophrenia than healthy controls when making decisions, while there was no difference in the amount of information gathered across the groups. However, the clinical group resorted to a less normatively effective strategy under stress; weighing all information equally regardless of its validity.

A further observation in the literature that is linked to the concept of thinking styles in people with psychosis (Eisenacher & Zink, Reference Eisenacher and Zink2017) is the cognitive bias against disconfirmatory evidence (BADE), reported particularly in people scoring highly in delusion-proneness (McLean et al., Reference McLean, Mattiske and Balzan2017; Woodward, Buchy, Moritz & Liotti, Reference Woodward, Buchy, Moritz and Liotti2007). BADE refers to a tendency to be less likely to integrate new evidence that disconfirms one’s existing beliefs. A recent meta-analysis identified greater BADE in people with psychosis than healthy controls as well as in people with delusions in psychosis than people without delusions in psychosis (McLean et al., Reference McLean, Mattiske and Balzan2017).

In contrast to the substantial literature in psychosis, the evidence base for reasoning processes in schizotypy is less well-established. Positive schizotypy, i.e. unusual beliefs, has been linked to intuitive thinking (Boden, Berenbaum & Topper, Reference Boden, Berenbaum and Topper2012) (assessed by the Rational Experiential Inventory; REI, Pacini & Epstein, Reference Pacini and Epstein1999; see also Genovese, Reference Genovese2005). In terms of deliberative thinking, there is some evidence that Type 2 logical reasoning ability is reduced in schizotypy, but findings vary across tasks related to logical reasoning and across studies. For example, Dagnall, Denovan, Drinkwater, Parker & Clough (Reference Dagnall, Denovan, Drinkwater, Parker and Clough2016) found that higher positive schizotypy (unusual experiences) was associated with poorer performance on reasoning problems involving statistical bias. Tsakanikos (Reference Tsakanikos2004) found that all dimensions out of the four schizotypy dimensions tested (unusual experiences, introvertive anhedonia, cognitive disorganisation and impulsive non-conformity) were associated with impaired performance on logical reasoning problems. Interestingly, Karimi, Windmann, Güntürkün & Abraham (Reference Karimi, Windmann, Güntürkün and Abraham2007) reported enhanced reasoning in people with higher schizotypy scores for creative or lateral problem solving (as opposed to goal focused problem solving), although another study reported no such association (Webb, Little, Cropper & Roze, Reference Webb, Little, Cropper and Roze2017).

Several studies have reported a significant JTC bias in high compared with low schizotypy (Brugger & Graves, Reference Brugger and Graves1997; Moritz et al., Reference Moritz, Göritz, Balzan, Kulagin and Andreou2017; Moritz, Van Quaquebeke & Lincoln, Reference Moritz, Van Quaquebeke and Lincoln2012; Sellen, Oaksford & Gray, Reference Sellen, Oaksford and Gray2005), although some studies have found no evidence for a JTC bias in positive and negative schizotypy (Juárez-Ramos et al., Reference Juárez-Ramos, Rubio, Delpero, Mioni, Stablum and Gómez-Milán2014; Sellen et al., Reference Sellen, Oaksford and Gray2005). Research into the association between BADE and schizotypy have similarly yielded inconclusive results (Buchy, Woodward & Liotti, Reference Buchy, Woodward and Liotti2007; Orenes, Navarrete, Beltrán & Santamaría, Reference Orenes, Navarrete, Beltrán and Santamaría2012). This may reflect the need to use more sensitive measures in schizotypy compared with psychosis or clinical samples, in which the effect sizes are likely to be larger and more detectable.

In summary, previous research seems to point to an association between schizotypy and reasoning processes (thinking dispositions as well as performance), but the exact nature of this interplay has not been comprehensively investigated. Thus far these processes have been considered only in isolation, e.g., the association between schizotypy and thinking dispositions, or between thinking dispositions and thinking performance. Here, we sought to more comprehensively investigate the relationship between individual differences in schizotypy and reasoning processes in the context of dual process models. In addition to self-reported intuitive and deliberative thinking, we also considered actively open-minded thinking beliefs and a performance-based measure of cognitive reflection. The cognitive reflection test (Frederick, Reference Frederick2005) was used as it exhibits properties related to the JTC tasks mentioned above (Evans & Stanovich, Reference Evans and Stanovich2013). We hypothesised that people with higher schizotypy scores would demonstrate greater experiential/intuitive reasoning (Faith in Intuition; FI), lower propensity for deliberative/effortful reasoning (Need for Cognition; NFC), reduced open-minded thinking beliefs (AOT), and reduced reflective thinking performance (CRT).

2 Method

The study employed a cross-sectional quantitative design. Participants completed an online survey created through the Qualtrics (2018) survey platform. Participation took approximately 10–15 minutes and upon completion participants received brief, general and non-pathologising feedback summaries based on their scores for positive and negative schizotypy.

2.1 Participants

The study was advertised and distributed through personal social media, university contacts, small ads websites (Gumtree), psychology research websites, and local community settings (e.g. Cafés, Yoga studios). Purposive recruitment was also undertaken by advertising the study in Facebook groups dedicated to topics such as spirituality, esoteric knowledge, paranormal beliefs, gaming and introversion. Participants were required to be 17 years of age or older to participate.

2.2 Materials

2.2.1 Demographics

Participants recorded their age, gender and highest completed level of education.

2.2.2 Schizotypy

The Oxford-Liverpool Inventory of Feelings and Experiences (O-LIFE) short form schizotypy scale (Mason et al., Reference Mason and Claridge2005) was used to measure positive and negative schizotypy. The O-LIFE short form is briefer and easier to administer than the original, whilst retaining favourable psychometric properties (Mason et al., Reference Mason and Claridge2005). Twelve self-report items assess Unusual Experiences (UE) and ten items assess Introvertive Anhedonia (IA). There are two other subscales, Cognitive Disorganisation (CD) and Impulsive Non-Conformity (IN), which were not relevant for the present study and were therefore not administered. Items are rated as 1 for a ‘yes’ response and 0 for a ‘no’ (except reverse-coded items), and are summed to provide a score for each dimension of schizotypy.

Sample Items:

When in the dark do you often see shapes and forms even though there is nothing there? (UE)

Do you feel very close to your friends? (IA) (reverse-coded)

2.2.3 Cognitive Reflection (CRT)

The combined Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT; Frederick, Reference Frederick2005) was used to measure cognitive reflection or ‘miserly processing’. The CRT is a performance-based measure consisting of brief math-like puzzles that assess the ability to withhold a tempting intuitive (but incorrect) Type 1 response and engage in reflective Type 2 processing to generate a correct response. It is negatively correlated with a variety of cognitive biases (Frederick, Reference Frederick2005).

All items proposed from across three papers (Frederick, Reference Frederick2005; Toplak, West & Stanovich, Reference Toplak, West and Stanovich2014; Thomson & Oppenheimer, Reference Thomson and Oppenheimer2016) were included, except for the ‘bat and ball’ problem, due to its now high level of familiarity in the public domain. A ‘control’ item consisting of a simple mathematical problem (with no ‘lure’ response) was embedded amongst the items, but did not contribute to CRT performance score. Correct responses were awarded a score of 1, while incorrect responses were awarded 0, with a maximum total score of 10. Higher total scores were indicative of greater ability to think analytically and override heuristic processing.

Sample Item (from CRT-2; Thomson & Oppenheimer, Reference Thomson and Oppenheimer2016):

A farmer had 15 sheep and all but 8 died. How many are left? [Intuitive answer: 7, Correct answer: 8]

An additional question “have you seen any of the above puzzles before?” was included and participants were asked to specify which puzzles they were familiar with. Accuracy scores were calculated based on the proportion of correct responses only for previously unseen items for each respondent.

2.2.4 Thinking Beliefs (AOT)

A shorter 7-item version (Haran, Ritov & Mellers, Reference Haran, Ritov and Mellers2013) of the original Actively Open-minded Thinking Scale (AOTS; Stanovich & West, Reference Stanovich and West1997) was used to measure beliefs about good thinking, particularly the tendency to engage in open-minded thinking and attend to evidence opposing favored conclusions. The short form has good face validity and reliability (Haran et al., Reference Haran, Ritov and Mellers2013). Questions are answered on a 7-point scale and are summed to provide an overall score ranging from 7 to 49. Higher total score favors a more open-minded and flexible approach to thinking.

Sample Item:

People should revise their beliefs in response to new information or evidence.

2.2.5 Type 1 and Type 2 Processing

The Rational Experiential Inventory (REI-10; Epstein, Pacini, Denes-Raj, & Heier, Reference Epstein, Pacini, Denes-Raj and Heier1996) comprises two unipolar scales each consisting of five items; Faith in Intuition (FI) and Need for Cognition (NFC). Based on Epstein’s Cognitive Experiential Self Theory (Epstein, Reference Epstein, Millon, Lerner and Weiner2003), they are thought to tap into Type 1 (intuitive-experiential) and Type 2 (analytic-rational) processes respectively. The original 40-item version (Pacini & Epstein, Reference Pacini and Epstein1999) is reported to have good psychometric properties including high internal validity (Cronbach’s alphas > 0.85). Responses are rated on a 5-point Likert scale and total scores indicate a preponderance to engage in intuitive or rational thinking processes respectively.

Sample Items:

My initial impressions of people are almost always right. (FI)

I would prefer complex to simple problems. (NFC)

2.3 Data Analysis

Gender was transformed from into a binary variable ‘sex’ with male coded as 0 and female coded as 1. This meant that for the 0.7% of respondents who entered ‘Other’ or ‘Would prefer not to say’ into the gender field, data for ‘sex’ was not coded and was not included in the analysis.

164 incomplete survey responses were retained in the dataset, and were included in the analysis where possible through pairwise deletion. Otherwise, cases with one-off item-level missing data were subject to mean imputation (Kalton & Kasprzyk, Reference Kalton and Kasprzyk1986). Mean imputation was carried out on a very small proportion of the data (0.0007% of items on both the AOT and REI-10) falling well within the acceptable recommended limits (Shrive, Stuart, Quan & Ghali, Reference Shrive, Stuart, Quan and Ghali2006).

3 Results

3.1 Sample Characteristics

A sample size of 1,512 participants was achieved. We sought a large sample in order to have a sufficiently powered study to detect potential nuances in the relationships between the different variables. Descriptive statistics are reported in Table 1. The majority of respondents were female (76.9%) and participant age spanned 17–78 years. Highest level of completed education was also well spread, with decent representation from those who had not completed higher education beyond school (15.0%), those who had completed sixth form (20.4%), vocational qualifications (9.7%), undergraduate degrees (28.4%), postgraduate degrees (22.0%) and doctoral degrees (4.0%).

Table 1: Sample characteristics

Note: Continuous variables were found to be normally distributed as assessed by histograms and Q-Q plots.

3.2 Associations between Variables

Pairwise independent samples t-tests were performed between males and females, with all continuous variables of interest as dependent variables. The results are displayed in Table 2. Homogeneity of variance as assessed by Levene’s test was not violated for any variables, except for CRT. Therefore, statistics for assumed equal variances are reported generally and unassumed equal variances are reported for CRT. CRT, IA, AOT and NFC was significantly higher in males than females, while UE and FI were significantly higher in females than males.

Table 2: T-tests for schizotypy, thinking processes and thinking styles by sex

Note: UE=unusual experiences, IA=introvertive anhedonia, CRT=cognitive reflection test, AOT=actively open-minded thinking, FI=faith in intuition, NFC=need for cognition. p-values are two-tailed. The minimum N is 303 for males and 1031 for males.

Pairwise Pearson’s correlation coefficients are displayed in Table 3. As for significant correlations, positive schizotypy (UE) was positively associated with IA and FI, and negatively associated with age, CRT, AOT and NFC. Similarly, negative schizotypy (IA) was positively correlated with FI and negatively correlated with CRT, AOT and NFC, but was not significantly associated with age. None of the variables were found to be correlated to a degree that suggested they were measuring the same underlying construct, with the highest r being .39 for the association between Cognitive Reflection and AOT. While some of the variables undoubtedly overlap, they appear to be conceptually distinct.

Table 3: Correlation coefficients

Note: CRT=cognitive reflection test, UE=unusual experiences, IA=introvertive anhedonia; AOT=actively open-minded thinking, FI=faith in intuition, NFC=need for cognition. All tests two-tailed. 95% confidence intervals are all ± .05. N’s are at least 1330. r = .06 required for p<.05, r = .07 required for p<.01, r = .10 required for p<.001.

3.3 Canonical Correlation Analysis

A canonical correlation analysis was conducted between the two schizotypy variables (UE and IA) and the four thinking process variables (FI, NFC, AOT and performance based CRT) to evaluate the multivariate shared relationship between the two sets of variables.Footnote 1 The analysis yielded two functions with squared canonical correlations () of .123 and .014 for each successful function. Overall, the full model across both functions was statistically significant (Wilks’ λ = .86, F (8, 2656) = 25.16, p < .001).Footnote 2 Tests of dimensionality further indicated that both of the canonical dimensions were statistically significant at the level p < .001. Thus, the best linear predictor of IA was not the same combination of the four predictors as the best predictor of UE.

Dimension reduction analysis tested the hierarchical arrangement of functions to establish their statistical significance. In addition to the full model (Functions 1 to 2) reaching statistical significance as highlighted above, the remaining function (Function 2) also explained a statistically significant amount of shared variance between the variable sets, F(3,1329) = 6.19, p < .001. However, referring to effects, Function 1 is considered far more noteworthy as it accounted for 12.38% of the shared variance. Function 2 needs to be interpreted with the caveat that it explained only 1.38% of the remaining variance after prior functions were extracted.

Standardized canonical function coefficients and structure coefficients for both functions are displayed in Table 4, in addition to the communalities (h 2) across the two functions for each variable.

Table 4: Canonical correlation analysis between schizotypy and thinking process variables

Note: Structure coefficients (r[s]) greater than |.45| are displayed in bold. Coef = standardized canonical function coefficient (weight of each variable); r[s] = structure coefficient (loading; correlation of each variable with function score); h2 = communality coefficient (percent of variance in each measure accounted for by both functions); UE = unusual experiences; IA = introvertive anhedonia; FI = faith in intuition; NFC = need for cognition; AOT = actively open-minded thinking; CRT = cognitive reflection test.

It is clear that Function 1 concerns mostly the predictor of UE (.977 vs. .076, upper left of Table 4) while Function 2 concerns the predictors of IA (−.351 vs. 1.035).

For Function 1, FI, AOT and CRT were the primary predictors of UE (but not NFC). UE was positively associated with FI and negatively associated with AOT and CRT.

For Function 2, the coefficients indicate that IA and NFC, especially NFC, are the primary variables relevant to the prediction of (mostly) IA and are inversely associated.

In summary, UE is most significantly associated with a combination of increased FI, lower AOT and lower CRT performance, while IA is best predicted by reduced NFC.

4 Discussion

This study sought to investigate how individual differences in schizotypy are related to reasoning processes in the context of dual process models. The aim was to generate a more comprehensive understanding of reasoning and decision-making in schizotypy, to build on current theoretical understandings and inform clinical interventions and practical applications in schizophrenia spectrum populations.

Sex differences in schizotypy mirrored previous literature (Fonseca-Pedrero et al., Reference Fonseca-Pedrero, Chan, Cicero, Zhang, Brenner and Ortuño-Sierra2018; Mason & Claridge, Reference Mason and Claridge2006), with women reporting higher UE than men, and men reporting higher IA than women. Men were also found to attain higher average scores on the CRT, as reported elsewhere (Frederick, Reference Frederick2005). Both positive (UE) and negative (IA) schizotypy dimensions were associated with higher FI, lower NFC, lower self-reported AOT and lower cognitive reflection test (CRT) performance. This suggests that overall, schizotypy was associated with greater reliance on intuitive processing and less reliance on deliberative processing, as well as a less open-minded and reflective reasoning style. Further analyses of the shared relationship between the two sets of variables, found that positive schizotypy (UE) was positively associated with intuitive thinking (FI) and negatively associated with AOT and thinking performance (CRT). Negative schizotypy (IA), on the other hand, was inversely associated with deliberative thinking (NFC), but only to a small degree. This suggests that, in our sample, reasoning processes co-varied to a greater extent with positive schizotypy than they did with negative schizotypy.

A preponderance for greater intuitive thinking and lower deliberative thinking fits with our hypotheses related to reasoning processes in schizotypy. Such a profile has been reported in delusions (Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Evans and Lister2012; Ward & Garety, in press; Ward et al., Reference Ward and Garety2018) as well as in belief in conspiracy theories (Swami, Voracek, Stieger, Tran & Furnham, Reference Swami, Voracek, Stieger, Tran and Furnham2014). This is particularly consistent with our observed association between positive schizotypy (which is characterized by unusual experiences) and more intuitive, less open-minded thinking and a less reflective reasoning performance. While there appears to be a lower preponderance for rational Type 2 thinking in high schizotypy scorers, this may not extend to all types of higher-level thinking. For example, creative thinking has previously been reported to be enhanced in high schizotypy scorers (Karimi et al., Reference Karimi, Windmann, Güntürkün and Abraham2007), which could be tested in future studies with objective performance-based measures of creative Type 2 reasoning.

A novel finding is that both UE and IA were inversely related to cognitive reflection test (CRT) performance. The negative association between schizotypy and AOT also suggests a lower propensity to consider alternative or conflicting evidence when reasoning (and a lower value placed on such consideration). The association was stronger between AOT and UE than it was for IA, suggesting a more robust association for positive than negative schizotypy. This is consistent with the literature, in which delusional beliefs are reported to be associated with lower belief flexibility, JTC and BADE in clinical samples (Ward & Garety, in press).

Similarly, UE (but not IA) was significantly associated with cognitive reflection after subjecting the two sets of variables to a canonical correlation analysis. The reason for this dissociation might be that negative schizotypy has a less cognitive quality, tapping more strongly into introversion or deriving less pleasure from activities: given a lower tendency to derive pleasure from reflective thinking, people scoring more highly in IA would reasonably be expected to gain less satisfaction from engaging in effortful and deliberative thought. This draws parallels with the lack of motivation (or ‘amotivation’) that is thought to be characteristic of clinical psychosis (Jean-Michel, Raoul & Marc, Reference Jean-Michel, Raoul and Marc2014), and the relatively strong negative correlation between IA and NFC suggests that NFC may be more sensitive than the other cognitive measures to motivation (as the NFC items also suggest).

“Epistemic self-regulation” (Evans & Stanovich, Reference Evans and Stanovich2013) thus seems to play a role in cognitive reflection. Schizotypy then seems to affect (if we can infer causation) the ability (or desire in the case of IA) to self-regulate one’s goals: there is less willingness to collect and consider more information or consider other points of view (AOT), and less of a tendency to think extensively about a situation and the future consequences. In addition, there is a direct relationship of UE (but not IA) with CRT score, which may reflect the assumed positive or additional processes causing either an increase in Type 1 activity (automatic responses) or an interference with Type 2 processing (constructive, reflective thought), or both, within positive schizotypy.

4.1 Implications

The present study has both theoretical and practical implications. Increasing people’s capacity for cognitive reflection, through encouraging consideration of alternative options or further deliberative reasoning could be clinically useful. For example, eliciting analytic thinking has been successful in reducing beliefs in conspiracy theories (Swami et al., Reference Swami, Voracek, Stieger, Tran and Furnham2014). Furthermore, as Type 2 reasoning is reportedly poorer in clinical psychosis compared with psychosis-like experiences in non-clinical samples (Ward et al., Reference Ward, Peters, Jackson, Day and Garety2018), effective rational reasoning may be protective in preventing paranoia or distressing appraisals associated with psychosis-like experiences.

Clinical interventions that target thinking processes have shown promise in treating various aspects of psychosis. For example, metacognitive training programs, which focus on amending reasoning biases have had favorable outcomes for people with psychosis, evidence which has been replicated and extended to the “gold-standard” of randomized-control trials (Aghotor, Pfueller, Moritz, Weisbrod & Roesch-Ely, Reference Aghotor, Pfueller, Moritz, Weisbrod and Roesch-Ely2010; Briki et al., Reference Briki, Monnin, Haffen, Sechter, Favrod, Netillard and Vandel2018; Moritz & Woodward, Reference Moritz and Woodward2007) and group formats (Moritz et al., Reference Moritz, Kerstan, Veckenstedt, Randjbar, Vitzthum, Schmidt and Woodward2011; Moritz et al., Reference Moritz, Veckenstedt, Bohn, Hottenrott, Scheu, Randjbar and Roesch-Ely2018). Cognitive Enhancement Therapy, a multidimensional program consisting of neurocognitive training and social cognitive group exercises, has also shown promising results (Eack et al., Reference Eack, Greenwald, Hogarty, Cooley, DiBarry, Montrose and Keshavan2009; Hogarty et al., Reference Hogarty, Flesher, Ulrich, Carter, Greenwald, Pogue-Gueile and Zoretich2004), as has Cognitive Remediation Therapy, which aims to improve cognitive flexibility and psychosocial functioning (McGurk, Twamley, Sitzer, McHugo & Mueser, Reference McGurk, Twamley, Sitzer, McHugo and Mueser2007; Wykes, Huddy, Cellard, McGurk & Czobor, Reference Wykes, Huddy, Cellard, McGurk and Czobor2011). While the effect sizes in our study were relatively small, it could reasonably be hypothesized that these effects would be more pronounced in clinical samples, which could be investigated in future research.

Our findings may suggest that clinical interventions related to negative schizotypy would be better placed to also focus on other aspects, such as potentially increasing wellbeing or psychosocial functioning (Greenwood, Landau & Wykes, Reference Greenwood, Landau and Wykes2005; Hunter & Barry, Reference Hunter and Barry2018; Lincoln, Mehl, Kesting & Rief, Reference Lincoln, Mehl, Kesting and Rief2011). Interestingly, however, increased cognitive flexibility has been found to be a predictor of increased treatment response to Cognitive Behavioural Therapy for Psychosis (Garety et al., Reference Garety, Fowler, Kuipers, Freeman, Dunn, Bebbington and Jones1997; So et al., Reference So, Freeman, Dunn, Kapur, Kuipers, Bebbington and Garety2012), one of the recommended psychological interventions for psychosis (NICE, 2014). This finding suggests that encouraging flexible thinking or AOT could potentially also exert beneficial effects through enhancing therapeutic responses to evidence-based psychological interventions.

4.2 Strengths and Limitations

Strengths of the current study include the large sample size, which enhances the reliability and generalizability of the findings. There was also good representation across a wide range of ages and education levels and from across the schizotypy continuum. The present study is novel in its approach and adds meaningful and valuable contributions to the literature on schizotypy and decision-making.

Nevertheless, the study needs to be couched in a number of limitations. The psychological constructs examined are not absolute, clear-cut and categorical phenomena. As with any cognitive constructs, they likely tap into multiple processes and share a degree of overlap. The multiple variations in dual process theories in the literature reflect this heterogeneity (Evans & Stanovich, Reference Evans and Stanovich2013). However, this is a caveat that applies across the field of personality research, and the approach we took arguably maximizes our ability to capture and identify these nuanced and reciprocal relationships.

The use of self-report measures also assumes that people are aware of and able to report their own reasoning tendencies and beliefs. Furthermore, intelligence or cognitive ability beyond the CRT were not explicitly assessed (Toplak, West & Stanovich, Reference Dudley, Taylor and Wickham2011) and mental health difficulties, substance misuse and neurological problems were not specifically screened for. However, it could be argued that this makes the data more representative of ‘real’ people and the population at large which it is intending to emulate.

4.3 Conclusions

The present study provides evidence that thinking processes and attitudes may vary according to schizotypy levels. These findings may highlight similar reasoning processes to those reported in psychosis; higher positive and negative schizotypy appeared to be related to greater reliance on intuitive thinking, less reliance on deliberative reasoning, a less open-minded thinking style and poorer reasoning performance, with more marked effects in positive than negative schizotypy. These findings carry clinical implications, including potential useful treatment targets and refinements for cognitive therapies.

Footnotes

1 Canonical correlation looks for a linear combination of one set of variables that maximally correlates with a linear combination of another set, then for a second pair of linear combinations that account for what the first set misses, and so on, but here there can only be two of these functions because one set of variables has only two members. So the question comes down to whether the cognitive measures can predict the schizotypy measures overall, and, if so, which measures are more important, and whether different cognitive measures are relatively more important for one schizotypy measure than the other.

2 As Wilks’ λ represents how much variance is unexplained by the model, 1−Wilks’ λ provides the full effect size for the model. Therefore, for the two canonical functions, the r 2 type effect size was .14. This suggests that the full model explained approximately 14% of the variance shared between schizotypy and thinking style.

References

Acar, S., Chen, X., & Cayirdag, N. (2018). Schizophrenia and creativity: A meta-analytic review. Schizophrenia Research, 195, 2331. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.SCHRES.2017.08.036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aghotor, J., Pfueller, U., Moritz, S., Weisbrod, M., & Roesch-Ely, D. (2010). Metacognitive training for patients with schizophrenia (MCT): Feasibility and preliminary evidence for its efficacy. Journal of Behavior Therapy & Experimental Psychiatry, 41(3), 207211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.01.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnes, T. R., & McPhillips, M. A. (1999). Critical analysis and comparison of the side-effect and safety profiles of the new antipsychotics. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 174(38), 3443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrantes-Vidal, N., Grant, P., & Kwapil, T. R. (2015). The role of schizotypy in the study of the etiology of schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 41(2), S408–S416. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbu191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boden, M. T., Berenbaum, H., & Topper, M. (2012). Intuition, affect, and peculiar beliefs. Personality & Individual Differences, 52(7), 845848. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.01.016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Briki, M., Monnin, J., Haffen, E., Sechter, D., Favrod, J., Netillard, C., … Vandel, P. (2018). Metacognitive training for schizophrenia: A multicentre randomised controlled trial. Schizophrenia Research, 157(1), 99106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2014.06.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bronstein, M. V., Pennycook, G., Bear, A., Rand, D. G., & Cannon, T. D. (in press). Belief in Fake News is Associated with Delusionality, Dogmatism, Religious Fundamentalism, and Reduced Analytic Thinking. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JARMAC.2018.09.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brugger, P., & Graves, R. E. (1997). Testing vs. believing hypotheses: Magical ideation in the judgement of contingencies. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 2(4), 251272. https://doi.org/10.1080/135468097396270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryan, W., & Harter, N. (1899). Studies on the telegraphic language: The acquisition of a hierarchy of habits. Psychological Review, 6(4), 345375. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0073117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buchy, L., Woodward, T. S., & Liotti, M. (2007). A cognitive bias against disconfirmatory evidence (BADE) is associated with schizotypy. Schizophrenia Research, 90(13), 334337. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2006.11.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burch, G. S. J., Pavelis, C., Hemsley, D. R., & Corr, P. J. (2006). Schizotypy and creativity in visual artists. British Journal of Psychology, 97(2), 177190. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712605X60030.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, A. S., Mohr, C., Ettinger, U., Chan, R. C. K., & Park, S. (2015). Schizotypy as an organizing framework for social and affective sciences. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 41(2), S427–S435. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbu195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colbert, S. M., Peters, E. R., & Garety, P. A. (2010). Delusions and belief flexibility in psychosis. Psychology & Psychotherapy: Theory, Research & Practice, 83(1), 4557. https://doi.org/10.1348/147608309X467320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dagnall, N., Denovan, A., Drinkwater, K., Parker, A., & Clough, P. (2016). Toward a better understanding of the relationship between belief in the paranormal and statistical bias: The potential role of schizotypy. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(1045), 114. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01045.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dudley, R., Taylor, P., & Wickham, S. (2015). Psychosis, delusions and the “jumping to conclusions” reasoning bias: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 42(3), 652665. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbv150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eack, S. M., Greenwald, D. P., Hogarty, S. S., Cooley, S. J., DiBarry, A. L., Montrose, D. M., & Keshavan, M. S. (2009). Cognitive enhancement therapy for early-course schizophrenia: Effects of a two-year randomized controlled trial. Psychiatric Services, 60(11), 14681476. https://doi.org/10.1176/ps.2009.60.11.1468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenacher, S., & Zink, M. (2017). Holding on to false beliefs: The bias against disconfirmatory evidence over the course of psychosis. Journal of Behavior Therapy & Experimental Psychiatry, 56, 7989. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2016.08.015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epstein, S. (2003). Cognitive-experiential self-theory of personality. In Millon, T., Lerner, M. J., & Weiner, I. B. (Eds.), Handbook of psychology (Volume 5: Personality and social psychology) (pp. 159184). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes-Raj, V., & Heier, H. (1996). Individual differences in intuitive–experiential and analytical–rational thinking styles. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 71(2), 390405. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ettinger, U., Meyhöfer, I., Steffens, M., Wagner, M., & Koutsouleris, N. (2014). Genetics, cognition, and neurobiology of schizotypal personality: a review of the overlap with schizophrenia. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 5(18), 116. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J. St B. T. (2003). In two minds: dual-process accounts of reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(10), 454459. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.08.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J. St B. T. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59(1), 255278. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093629.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J. St B. T. (2010). Intuition and reasoning: A dual-process perspective. Psychological Inquiry, 21, 313326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, J. St B. T., Barston, J., & Pollard, P. (1983). On the conflict between logic and belief in syllogistic reasoning. Memory & Cognition, 11(3), 295306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J. St B. T., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 223241. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612460685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, J. E., Mohanty, A., Herrington, J. D., Koven, N. S., Miller, G. A., & Heller, W. (2004). Neuropsychological evidence for dimensional schizotypy: Implications for creativity and psychopathology. Journal of Research in Personality, 38(1), 2431. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JRP.2003.09.014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fonseca-Pedrero, E., Chan, R. C. K., Debbané, M., Cicero, D., Zhang, L. C., Brenner, C., … Ortuño-Sierra, J. (2018). Comparisons of schizotypal traits across 12 countries: Results from the International Consortium for Schizotypy Research. Schizophrenia Research, 199, 128134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2018.03.021.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19(4), 2542. https://doi.org/10.1257/089533005775196732.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, D., Evans, N., & Lister, R. (2012). Gut feelings, deliberative thought, and paranoid ideation: a study of experiential and rational reasoning. Psychiatry Research, 197(12), 119122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.12.031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garety, P., Fowler, D., Kuipers, E., Freeman, D., Dunn, G., Bebbington, P., … Jones, S. (1997). London–East Anglia randomised controlled trial of cognitive–behavioural therapy for psychosis: II: Predictors of outcome. British Journal of Psychiatry, 171(5), 420426. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.171.5.420.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garety, P., Kuipers, E., Fowler, D., Freeman, D., & Bebbington, P. (2001). A cognitive model of the positive symptoms of psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 31(2), 189195. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291701003312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Genovese, J. E. C. (2005). Paranormal beliefs, schizotypy, and thinking styles among teachers and future teachers. Personality & Individual Differences, 39(1), 93102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2004.12.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glöckner, A., & Moritz, S. (2009). A fine-grained analysis of the jumping to conclusions bias in schizophrenia: data-gathering, response confidence, and information integration. Judgment & Decision Making, 4, 587600. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.1313623.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenwood, K. E., Landau, S., & Wykes, T. (2005). Negative symptoms and specific cognitive impairments as combined targets for improved functional outcome within cognitive remediation therapy. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 31(4), 910921. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbi035.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haran, U., Ritov, I., & Mellers, B. (2013). The role of actively open-minded thinking in information acquisition, accuracy, and calibration. Judgment & Decision Making, 8(3), 188201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hogarty, G., Flesher, S., Ulrich, R., Carter, M., Greenwald, D. P., Pogue-Gueile, M., … Zoretich, R. (2004). Cognitive enhancement therapy for schizophrenia: Effects of a 2-year randomized trial on cognition and behavior. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61(9), 866876. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.61.9.866.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunter, R., & Barry, S. (2018). Negative symptoms and psychosocial functioning in schizophrenia: Neglected but important targets for treatment. European Psychiatry, 27(6), 432436. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2011.02.015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huq, S., Garety, P., & Hemsley, D. (1988). Probabilistic judgements in deluded and non-deluded subjects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40(4), 801812. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640748808402300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Irwin, H. J., & Wilson, K. (2013). Anomalous experiences and the intuitive-experiential style of thinking. Journal of the Society for Psychical Research, 77, 6571.Google Scholar
Jean-Michel, A., Raoul, B., & Marc, A. (2014). Negative symptoms in schizophrenia: Where we have been and where we are heading. CNS Neuroscience & Therapeutics, 20(9), 801808. https://doi.org/10.1111/cns.12292.Google Scholar
Juárez-Ramos, V., Rubio, J. L., Delpero, C., Mioni, G., Stablum, F., & Gómez-Milán, E. (2014). Jumping to conclusions bias, BADE and feedback sensitivity in schizophrenia and schizotypy. Consciousness & Cognition, 26, 133144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.03.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalton, G., & Kasprzyk, D. (1986). The treatment of missing survey data. Survey Methodology, 12(1), 116.Google Scholar
Karimi, Z., Windmann, S., Güntürkün, O., & Abraham, A. (2007). Insight problem solving in individuals with high versus low schizotypy. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(2), 473480. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2006.03.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klauer, K. C., Musch, J., & Naumer, B. (2000). On belief bias in syllogistic reasoning. Psychological Review, 107(4), 852884. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.107.4.852.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kwapil, T. R., Barrantes-Vidal, N., & Silvia, P. J. (2008). The dimensional structure of the Wisconsin Schizotypy Scales: Factor identification and construct validity. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 34(3), 444457. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbm098.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lambert, M., Conus, P., Eid, P., Mass, R., Karow, A., Moritz, S., … Naber, D. (2004). Impact of present and past antipsychotic side effects on attitude toward typical antipsychotic treatment and adherence. European Psychiatry, 19(7), 415422. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.EURPSY.2004.06.031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lincoln, T. M., Mehl, S., Kesting, M.-L., & Rief, W. (2011). Negative symptoms and social cognition: Identifying targets for psychological interventions. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 37(2), S23–S32. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbr066.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mason, O. (2006). A confirmatory factor analysis of the structure of schizotypy. European Journal of Personality, 9(4), 271281. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2410090404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mason, O., & Claridge, G. (2006). The Oxford-Liverpool Inventory of Feelings and Experiences (O-LIFE): Further description and extended norms. Schizophrenia Research, 82(23), 203211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2005.12.845.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mason, O., Claridge, G., & Jackson, M. (1995). New scales for the assessment of schizotypy. Personality & Individual Differences, 18(1), 713. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(94)00132-C.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGurk, S. R., Twamley, E. W., Sitzer, D. I., McHugo, G. J., & Mueser, K. T. (2007). A meta-analysis of cognitive remediation in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164(12), 17911802. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07060906.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLean, B. F., Mattiske, J. K., & Balzan, R. P. (2017). Association of the jumping to conclusions and evidence integration biases With delusions in psychosis: A detailed meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 43(2), 344354. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbw056.Google ScholarPubMed
Moritz, S., Göritz, A. S., Balzan, R. P., Gawęda, Ł., Kulagin, S. C., & Andreou, C. (2017). A new paradigm to measure probabilistic reasoning and a possible answer to the question why psychosis-prone individuals jump to conclusions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126(4), 406415. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moritz, S., Kerstan, A., Veckenstedt, R., Randjbar, S., Vitzthum, F., Schmidt, C., … Woodward, T. S. (2011). Further evidence for the efficacy of a metacognitive group training in schizophrenia. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 49(3), 151157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2010.11.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moritz, S., Van Quaquebeke, N., & Lincoln, T. M. (2012). Jumping to conclusions is associated with paranoia but not general suspiciousness: a comparison of two versions of the probabilistic reasoning paradigm. Schizophrenia Research & Treatment, 19. https://doi.org/10.1155/2012/384039.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moritz, S., Veckenstedt, R., Bohn, F., Hottenrott, B., Scheu, F., Randjbar, S., … Roesch-Ely, D. (2018). Complementary group Metacognitive Training (MCT) reduces delusional ideation in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 151(1), 6169. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2013.10.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moritz, S., & Woodward, T. S. (2007). Metacognitive training for schizophrenia patients (MCT): a pilot study on feasibility, treatment adherence, and subjective efficacy. German Journal of Psychiatry, 10(3), 6978.Google Scholar
NICE. (2014). Psychosis and schizophrenia in adults: Treatment and management. NICE guideline [CG178].Google Scholar
O’Reilly, T., Dunbar, R., & Bentall, R. (2001). Schizotypy and creativity: an evolutionary connection? Personality & Individual Differences, 31(7), 10671078. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00204-X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orenes, I., Navarrete, G., Beltrán, D., & Santamaría, C. (2012). Schizotypal people stick longer to their first choices. Psychiatry Research, 200(23), 620628. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.03.030.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pacini, R., & Epstein, S. (1999). The relation of rational and experiential information processing styles to personality, basic beliefs, and the ratio-bias phenomenon. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 76(6), 972987. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.972.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Qualtrics. (2018). Qualtrics software. Provo, UT: Qualtrics.Google Scholar
Ross, R. M., Hartig, B., & McKay, R. (2017). Analytic cognitive style predicts paranormal explanations of anomalous experiences but not the experiences themselves: Implications for cognitive theories of delusions. Journal of Behavior Therapy & Experimental Psychiatry, 56, 9096. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JBTEP.2016.08.018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, R. M., McKay, R., Coltheart, M., & Langdon, R. (2015). Jumping to conclusions about the beads task? A meta-analysis of delusional ideation and data-gathering. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 41(5), 11831191. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbu187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ross, R. M., Pennycook, G., McKay, R., Gervais, W. M., Langdon, R., & Coltheart, M. (2016). Analytic cognitive style, not delusional ideation, predicts data gathering in a large beads task study. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 21(4), 300314. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546805.2016.1192025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sellen, J. L., Oaksford, M., & Gray, N. S. (2005). Schizotypy and conditional reasoning. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 31(1), 105116. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbi012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84(2), 127190. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shrive, F. M., Stuart, H., Quan, H., & Ghali, W. A. (2006). Dealing with missing data in a multi-question depression scale: a comparison of imputation methods. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 6(57), 110. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2288-6-57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
So, S. H., Freeman, D., Dunn, G., Kapur, S., Kuipers, E., Bebbington, P., … Garety, P. A. (2012). Jumping to conclusions, a lack of belief flexibility and delusional conviction in psychosis: A longitudinal investigation of the structure, frequency, and relatedness of reasoning biases. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(1), 129139. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
So, S. H., Siu, N. Y., Wong, H., Chan, W., & Garety, P. A. (2016). ‘Jumping to conclusions’ data-gathering bias in psychosis and other psychiatric disorders — Two meta-analyses of comparisons between patients and healthy individuals. Clinical Psychology Review, 46, 151167. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.CPR.2016.05.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanovich, K., & West, R. (1997). Reasoning independently of prior belief and individual differences in actively open-minded thinking. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(2), 342357. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.89..CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swami, V., Voracek, M., Stieger, S., Tran, U. S., & Furnham, A. (2014). Analytic thinking reduces belief in conspiracy theories. Cognition, 133(3), 572585. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomson, K. S., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2016). Investigating an alternate form of the cognitive reflection test. Judgment & Decision Making, 11(1), 99113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toplak, M. E., West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (2011). The Cognitive Reflection Test as a predictor of performance on heuristics-and-biases tasks. Memory & Cognition, 39, 12751289. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-011-0104-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toplak, M. E., West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (2014). Assessing miserly information processing: An expansion of the Cognitive Reflection Test. Thinking & Reasoning, 20(2), 147168. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2013.844729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsakanikos, E. (2004). Logical reasoning in schizotypal personality. Personality & Individual Differences, 37(8), 17171726. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2004.03.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, T., & Garety, P. A. (in press). Fast and slow thinking in distressing delusions: A review of the literature and implications for targeted therapy. Schizophrenia Research. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.SCHRES.2017.08.045.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, T., Peters, E., Jackson, M., Day, F., & Garety, P. A. (2018). Data-gathering, belief flexibility, and reasoning across the psychosis continuum. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 44(1), 126136. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbx029.Google ScholarPubMed
Webb, M. E., Little, D. R., Cropper, S. J., & Roze, K. (2017). The contributions of convergent thinking, divergent thinking, and schizotypy to solving insight and non-insight problems. Thinking & Reasoning, 23(3), 235258. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2017.1295105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodward, T. S., Buchy, L., Moritz, S., & Liotti, M. (2007). A bias against disconfirmatory evidence is associated with delusion proneness in a nonclinical sample. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33(4), 10231028. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbm013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wykes, T., Huddy, V., Cellard, C., McGurk, S. R., & Czobor, P. (2011). A meta-analysis of cognitive remediation for schizophrenia: Methodology and effect sizes. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(5), 472485. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10060855.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1: Sample characteristics

Figure 1

Table 2: T-tests for schizotypy, thinking processes and thinking styles by sex

Figure 2

Table 3: Correlation coefficients

Figure 3

Table 4: Canonical correlation analysis between schizotypy and thinking process variables

Supplementary material: File

Broyd et al. supplementary material

Broyd et al. supplementary material 1
Download Broyd et al. supplementary material(File)
File 2.3 MB
Supplementary material: File

Broyd et al. supplementary material

Broyd et al. supplementary material 2
Download Broyd et al. supplementary material(File)
File 15.4 KB