Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T01:54:34.260Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effectiveness of a parental school-based intervention to improve young children’s eating patterns: a pilot study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 April 2018

Ana Isabel Gomes*
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology, University of Lisbon, Alameda da Universidade, 1649-013 Lisbon, Portugal
Luisa Barros
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology, University of Lisbon, Alameda da Universidade, 1649-013 Lisbon, Portugal
Ana Isabel Pereira
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology, University of Lisbon, Alameda da Universidade, 1649-013 Lisbon, Portugal
Magda Sofia Roberto
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology, University of Lisbon, Alameda da Universidade, 1649-013 Lisbon, Portugal
*
*Corresponding author: Email ana_fernandes_gomes@hotmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

To evaluate the effectiveness of a pilot study of a parental school-based intervention to promote healthy eating behaviours in young children.

Design

A quasi-experimental longitudinal design with three conditions (complete intervention (CIG), minimal intervention (MIG), control (CG)), with repeated measures at baseline, immediately after the intervention, 6 months and 1 year after intervention.

Setting

Fourteen public and state-funded kindergartens near Lisbon, Portugal.

Subjects

Parents (n 349) of 3- to 6-year-old children assigned to the three conditions completed the baseline protocol. The ‘Red Apple’ intervention included four parental group sessions about young children’s growth, nutritional guidelines and positive parental feeding strategies, which was combined with adult–child activities at home and in the classroom, and newsletters (CIG). MIG included only a single nutritional counselling session, whereas the CG had no intervention. At the end, thirty-eight, twenty-six and fifty-four parents in the CIG, MIG and CG, respectively, had completed all evaluation components. Data regarding parental perception of children’s weight, self-efficacy, nutritional knowledge, feeding strategies, eating behaviours and BMI were collected at the four assessment moments.

Results

The CIG showed improvements in children’s healthy food intake, compared with the MIG and CG. Parental self-efficacy regarding the regulation of children’s eating behaviours decreased in the CG but not in both intervention groups.

Conclusions

Considering the low dosage of the intervention, the results obtained were positive. Future studies should offer additional solutions to overcome barriers to parents’ participation.

Type
Research paper
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2018 

Early childhood overweight has been progressively recognized as a public health problem, with systematic reviews reporting rates of excessive weight of up to one-third in children and adolescents( Reference Rietmeijer-Mentink, Paulis and van Middelkoop 1 , Reference Ng, Fleming and Robinson 2 ). In Portuguese samples, the prevalence of overweight in young children reaches similar values( Reference Aparício, Cunha and Duarte 3 , Reference Viveiro, Brito and Moleiro 4 ). The development and implementation of comprehensive programmes that positively impact early childhood weight- and obesity-related behaviours is crucial. The first years of life have been acknowledged as an exceptional period for intervention on healthy eating promotion and obesity prevention( Reference Wofford 5 ). BMI undergoes important changes between 4 and 6 years of age, constituting one of the three critical periods for the development of overweight( Reference Dietz 6 ). Consequently, this period offers a major opportunity to keep the child’s growth on a healthy trajectory( Reference Campbell, Williams and Hampton 7 ). Moreover, there is extensive evidence that eating patterns and food preferences are acquired during these years and are shaped by early experiences with foods and flavours( Reference Birch and Fisher 8 , Reference Birch 9 ). Energy balance-related behaviours, psychological traits and physiological processes can be optimally influenced at this age( Reference Manios, Grammatikaki and Androutsos 10 ) since children’s high plasticity and rapid transitions occur concurrently with adults’ primary control over children’s environment and interactions( Reference Anzman, Rollins and Birch 11 ).

The Commission on Ending Childhood Obesity recommended that interventions to prevent childhood overweight should provide guidance to parents and caregivers on children’s healthy body size and age-appropriate nutrition, to decrease portion size, limit the consumption of energy-dense and nutrient-poor foods, and encourage the intake of a wide variety of fruits and vegetables( 12 ). School- and family-based interventions can lead to positive changes in pre-school children’s diet and slightly improve weight outcomes( Reference Brown, Kelly and Summerbell 13 ). Programmes that include parents as key targets and agents of change, combining parental involvement with behavioural interventions and not focusing only on nutritional education, are more likely to be successful in providing effective and long-lasting changes in children’s eating patterns( Reference Bluford, Sherry and Scanlon 14 Reference Hingle, O’Connor and Dave 17 ). The involvement of kindergartens can optimize the long-term effectiveness of interventions for children’s healthy food intake( Reference Hendrie, Lease and Bowen 18 , 19 ) and allow easier access to children and parents from mixed socio-economic backgrounds( Reference Manios, Grammatikaki and Androutsos 10 ).

Parental cognitive dimensions have been related to parents’ motivation to implement strategies to regulate their child’s food intake. Traditionally, interventions developed to target healthy eating behaviour have included, as a main parental cognitive outcome, nutrition and/or diet knowledge( Reference Skouteris, McCabe and Swinburn 20 , Reference Peters, Sinn and Campbell 21 ). However, targeting other cognitive dimensions may contribute to parental change. Parent report of higher self-efficacy to promote children’s healthy diet has been associated with young children’s higher consumption of vegetables and fruits and lower intake of sugary foods( Reference Campbell, Hesketh and Silverii 22 ). On the other hand, early parental identification of the child’s physical signs of overweight( Reference Warschburger and Kröller 23 ) and recognition of their child’s weight as a health problem( Reference Rhee, DeLago and Arscott-Mills 24 ) seem to be related to parents’ readiness to improve the family’s diet quality and effectively manage the child’s eating. However, how these dimensions change over time or in response to parental education programmes remains unclear. Only a few intervention studies that involved parents of pre-school children measured parental self-efficacy at follow-up and change in parental perception of their child’s weight has not been reported as an outcome measure( Reference Hingle, O’Connor and Dave 17 , Reference Skouteris, McCabe and Swinburn 20 , Reference Morris, Skouteris and Edwards 25 ).

The purpose of the present study was to conduct a pilot test of the effectiveness of the ‘Red Apple’ programme, a brief parental school-based intervention to promote healthy eating behaviours in their young children, and to assess the impact of the programme on behavioural measures (child’s healthy and unhealthy eating habits, parental feeding practices) and parental cognitive dimensions previously associated with parents’ motivation to engage in positive changes related to the child’s eating patterns (i.e. parents’ perception of the child’s weight, self-efficacy and nutritional knowledge). We hypothesized that the ‘Red Apple’ programme, compared with a single parental nutrition education session and a control condition with no intervention, would result in: (i) a significant increase in the child’s healthy food intake and a decrease in the child’s unhealthy food intake; (ii) improved parental self-efficacy, nutritional knowledge and feeding practices; and (iii) a more accurate parental perception of the child’s weight. It was also expected that these changes would continue, at least partially, during follow-up.

Methods

Study design

The present study used a quasi-experimental, longitudinal design with three conditions (complete intervention (CIG), minimal intervention (MIG), control (CG)) and four repeated measures (at baseline, immediately after the intervention, 6 months and 1 year after the intervention).

Participants

Detailed information about the participants in the study can be found in Fig. 1. Of the initial 743 parents invited, 353 returned the evaluation protocol. Four parents were considered ineligible for analysis. After this process, 138 parents were assigned to the CIG, 120 were assigned to the MIG and ninety-one were assigned to the CG. At the end of the study, thirty-eight parents in the CIG, twenty-six parents in the MIG and fifty-four parents in the CG had completed all the evaluation components.

Fig. 1 Flow of the participants through the pilot study of the ‘Red Apple’ programme, a parental school-based intervention to promote healthy eating behaviours in young children (aged 3–6 years), Lisbon, Portugal, October 2011–June 2014

Table 1 presents sociodemographic and clinical variables for the three groups at baseline. Most questionnaires were completed by mothers (100 % for CIG, 92·3 % for MIG, 96·3 % for CG). In general, children lived with both parents and most were 4 or 5 years old (73·7 % for CIG, 73·1 % for MIG, 92·6 % for CG). There was a similar percentage of boys and girls in the sample.

Table 1 Sociodemographic and clinical variables for the three group conditions at baseline in the pilot study of the ‘Red Apple’ programme, a parental school-based intervention to promote healthy eating behaviours in young children (aged 3–6 years), Lisbon, Portugal, October 2011–June 2014

CIG, complete intervention; MIG, minimal intervention; CG, control.

Instruments

Sociodemographic and clinical information

Background information about children’s age, gender, gestational age at birth and chronic health conditions, parent’s age, kinship with the child, family composition and educational level was collected using a general questionnaire.

Parent’s perception about the child’s current weight (correct/incorrect)

Parents were asked to subjectively rate their child’s current weight, considering the child’s age and height (‘underweight’, ‘average weight’, ‘overweight’). The Centers for Disease Control and Prevention’s standards were used to calculate BMI-for-age percentiles (boys and girls) and nutritional status, because, at the time of the study, these charts were still being used by Portuguese physicians (WHO charts only started to be used at consultations with children born after March 2014). Children above the 85th percentile were classified as overweight, those below the 5th percentile as underweight and the remaining children as healthy weight. Parents’ perception of children’s weight was then compared with children’s nutritional status and categorized as correct or incorrect( Reference Maynard, Galuska and Blanck 26 ).

Parent’s self-efficacy

A short questionnaire with four items was developed to evaluate parents’ perception of their efficacy in promoting their children’s healthy food intake (e.g. ‘To what extent am I sure that I can get my child to have a healthy diet?’), controlling the intake of unhealthy foods (e.g. ‘To what extent am I sure I am able to control the behaviour of my child, so that he/she does not eat too much candy?’). The scale was developed according to the objectives of the intervention and general guidelines for the self-efficacy scale construction( Reference Bandura 27 ) and was submitted to a validity and reliability analysis. For each item, parents answered on a 5-point Likert scale; the responses were summed to calculate the total score, with higher values indicating higher self-efficacy (score ranged between 4 and 20). This scale showed acceptable internal consistency (α=0·74; mean inter-item correlation=0·35) and good test–retest (15 d interval) reliability (r s=0·78, P>0·01).

Parent’s nutritional knowledge questionnaire

The questionnaire on parent’s nutritional knowledge( Reference Vereecken and Maes 28 ) contains ten items describing beliefs about pre-school children’s diet and eating behaviours that were chosen originally by researchers and health professionals working with parents of young children in dietary consultations. Parents answered on a 5-point Likert scale (‘right’, ‘I think it is right’, ‘I do not know’, ‘I think it is wrong’, ‘wrong’). Correct responses were scored as +2 if the parent was sure about the answer and +1 if s/he was unsure; incorrect answers were scored as −2 or −1 depending also on the parent being sure or not about the answer, and ‘I do not know’ as 0, with a higher total score indicating better nutritional knowledge. The Portuguese version was subject to preliminary analysis, based on difficulty and discrimination indices calculation, and eight items were considered for the total score (score ranged between −16 and 16). Test–retest (15 d interval) reliability was good (r s=0·88, P<0·01).

Children’s Eating Habits Questionnaire

The Children’s Eating Habits Questionnaire (CEHQ) was developed to assess the quality of the children’s dietary habits, based on national and international dietary guidelines for young children. Although recognizing the limitations of this kind of measure, the option for an FFQ aimed to better adapt the assessment to the specific objectives of the intervention, to cover a larger time frame and to target the usual eating patterns of the child. This instrument was developed in collaboration with a group of dietitians and a validity and reliability analysis was conducted to confirm its sensibility to change( Reference Willis, George and Hunt 29 ). Parents’ report of children’s frequency of consumption of several foods was evaluated on a 4-point Likert scale (‘never’, ‘1–2 times a week’, ‘3–6 times a week’, ‘every day’). After an exploratory factor analysis, the items were grouped into two dimensions: healthy foods (soup, fruits, vegetables and fish) and unhealthy foods (desserts, candies, fast foods, sodas and foods with added salt/sugar). Higher mean values on the total score of the scales corresponded to more frequent intake of those foods (score ranged between 1 and 4 for both scales). Details about the conception of the instrument and psychometric features are described elsewhere( Reference Gomes, Barros and Pereira 30 ).

Caregiver’s feeding styles questionnaire

The nineteen-item caregiver’s feeding styles questionnaire( Reference Hughes, Power and Fisher 31 ) evaluates parental feeding styles through a 5-point Likert scale, in which parents indicate the frequency of use of specific strategies. For the Portuguese version, a sequence of exploratory factor analyses was run, which resulted in the extraction of two factors: coercive (parent-centred strategies used to modify the child’s eating behaviours through disapproval, pressure to eat, physical punishment and contingency management; ten items) and inductive (child-centred strategies based on reasoning, positive reinforcement and child’s involvement in decisions about food; seven items). The answers for each subscale were summed, with higher values indicating a more frequent use of each group of strategies (total score ranged between 10 and 50 for coercive strategies, between 7 and 35 for inductive strategies). The scales achieved good and acceptable internal consistency (coercive strategies α=0·87; inductive strategies α=0·69; mean inter-item correlation >0·2 for both scales) and good test–retest (15 d interval) reliability (r s=0·84, P<0·01 and r s=0·78, P<0·01 for coercive and inductive strategies, respectively).

Procedure

Recruitment and data collection were performed between October 2011 and June 2014. Fourteen public and state-funded kindergartens near Lisbon were invited and agreed to participate in the study. Schools were randomly allocated to one of the three conditions, so that in each school all participants were in the same group, to control for possible dissemination of information. Acknowledging earlier reports of high rates of attrition in childhood obesity prevention programmes, approximately twice the number of participants in the CG were attributed to the CIG. Recruitment occurred simultaneously for the three conditions at four times during the year, to account for the possible influences of seasons and festivities in the children’s eating behaviours. Parents were invited to participate through a leaflet sent home; those who agreed received the informed consent and the evaluation protocol through the kindergarten teacher, with instructions for completion and to return the questionnaires in sealed envelopes. After parents’ enrolment, children’s weight and height measurements were assessed in the school by a dietitian with experience in anthropometric evaluations in infancy. Children were individually taken to a separate room, where they were weighed and measured in light clothing and without shoes before lunch, with a SECA® model 220 column scale with stadiometer.

Complete intervention group: ‘Red Apple’ programme

The ‘Red Apple’ programme was inspired by cognitive and social-cognitive theories, and intervention goals were selected considering the dimensions commonly considered as determinants of behavioural intentions and behaviour itself, namely threat appraisal (severity, vulnerability), self-efficacy and behavioural control (internal control factors as skills and information; external control factors as barriers and opportunities)( Reference Rogers 32 , Reference Ajzen 33 ). Thus, we developed an intervention aimed at modifying the specific parental cognitive determinants known to be related to the child’s eating behaviour and/or the parent’s motivation to undertake changes in diet (perception about the child’s weight, self-efficacy and nutritional knowledge)( Reference Peters, Sinn and Campbell 21 , Reference Moore, Harris and Bradlyn 34 , Reference Hildebrand and Betts 35 ), to influence parental attitudes and practices according to the individual characteristics of their child and thus modify children’s dietary intake.

The ‘Red Apple’ programme included four sessions, each focusing on one major theme related to the young child’s growth process, nutritional guidelines and parental strategies to promote children’s healthy eating behaviours (Table 2). The approach was behaviourally focused, promoting the participation of the parents and supported by everyday examples. At the end of each session, a theme-related homework assignment was proposed, suggesting adult–child activities between sessions to apply the strategies discussed in the session and inviting caregivers to reflect on the benefits of applying the suggested changes. A week after each session, parents received a newsletter, reinforcing the key messages of the previous session. The intervention was delivered to fourteen groups, each one with six to eight parents. Sessions were conducted in the schools in the presence of the class teacher and were scheduled to better fit parents’ preferences, which were previously assessed in the baseline protocol. Each session lasted 90 min and occurred every two weeks. The first author was responsible for the delivery of the intervention, with the participation of a dietitian in the second session to direct the nutritional education component. Only those parents who participated in the first two sessions and in at least one of the sessions dedicated to parental feeding strategies were considered to have completed the programme and were retained for statistical analysis.

Table 2 ‘Red Apple’ intervention: structure and content of sessions

Minimal intervention and control groups

Parents assigned to the MIG attended only a nutritional counselling session with the dietitian (similar to the second session in the CIG) and received the homework assignment and a newsletter related to the theme. We chose different dosages for the two intervention condition groups because our aim was to assess the benefits of a comprehensive intervention targeting cognitive parental variables beyond nutritional knowledge, compared with a single healthy eating sensitization session, most often provided in Portuguese kindergartens. Parents included in the CG did not receive any intervention and completed the evaluation protocol.

Process evaluation

Fidelity to intervention delivery was evaluated through recording of the activities performed by the facilitator during the sessions and the homework assignments returned by parents and kindergarten teachers along the programme. Parental satisfaction regarding the ‘Red Apple’ programme was evaluated separately through questions about utility and interest of the sessions, home activities, class activities and newsletters.

Data analysis

Initial statistical analyses were performed using the statistical software package IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, version 23.0. The χ 2 test and ANOVA were used to compare samples in the three conditions on nominal and ordinal variables (parental perception of children’s weight, demographic and clinical variables) and metric variables (children’s eating habits), respectively, after screening and before intervention. Similarly, differences between those who completed the intervention and those who dropped out were calculated using χ 2 tests and independent-samples t tests (or independent-samples Mann–Whitney U tests as a non-parametric alternative). Mixed within-between ANOVA and Cochran’s Q tests (repeated measures) were conducted to compare the mean differences between conditions at each assessment point and between the four assessment moments in each condition.

Results

Retention and dropout

The retention rate for completion of the ‘Red Apple’ programme was 31·9 %. Several reasons were reported for dropping out: incompatibility between programme sessions and work schedule (35·3 %), illness (23·5 %) and lack of childcare (11·8 %). For most variables, no significant differences were found between parents who completed the ‘Red Apple’ programme and those who dropped out. However, the parents who completed the intervention were younger ( $\chi _{{(4)}}^{2} $ =9·611, P=0·048), had more years of schooling ( $\chi _{{(4)}}^{2} $ =12·101, P=0·017) and had children in the programme who were more likely to be preterm ( $\chi _{{(4)}}^{2} $ =16·423, P=0·003).

For the MIG, the retention rate was higher (49·2 %). Parents who completed the nutritional session had a higher education level ( $\chi _{{(2)}}^{2} $ =11·849, P=0·003), and their children consumed fewer unhealthy foods (U=1179·5, P=0·005) and had a higher BMI percentile (U=1355, P=0·049) than those who were enrolled but did not participate.

Sample characteristics and group equivalence at baseline

Table 3 reports the results of the variables at baseline for the parents who remained in the study and performed the four evaluation assessments. A considerable percentage of parents (26·3 % of CIG, 42·3 % of MIG, 40·7 % of CG) failed to correctly recognize their child’s weight status, with most underestimating the child’s weight (90·0 % for CIG, 100·0 % for MIG, 95·5 % for CG). The proportion of children with excessive weight was high (26·3 % for CIG, 34·6 % for MIG, 33·3 % for CG).

Table 3 Parent and child variables at baseline, post-intervention, 6-month and 1-year assessments for the three group conditions in the pilot study of the ‘Red Apple’ programme, a parental school-based intervention to promote healthy eating behaviours in young children (aged 3–6 years), Lisbon, Portugal, October 2011–June 2014

The results regarding multiple comparisons are presented only for the variables that achieved significant results (P<0·05) in repeated-measures statistical tests. The ordination of mean/percentage values is achieved using lower-case letters for comparisons between different assessment points in each group condition and upper-case letters for comparisons among group conditions at the same assessment point (a>b>c; A>B). Mean/percentage values that share the same lower-case or upper-case ordination letter did not differ significantly in the multiple comparisons.

At baseline, no significant differences were found among the three groups for the variables above (correct perception of child’s weight: $\chi _{{(2)}}^{2} $ =2·499, P=0·287; child’s weight: $\chi _{{(4)}}^{2} $ =2·443, P=0·655; parents’ self-efficacy: F (2,115)=0·139, P=0·870; nutritional knowledge: F (2,115)=0·139, P=0·870; inductive strategies: F (2,115)=0·208, P=0·813; coercive strategies: F (2,115)=0·006, P=0·994; healthy food intake: F (2,115)=0·884, P=0·416; unhealthy food intake: F (2,113)=0·022, P=0·978).

Similarly, the demographic and clinical characteristics (see Table 1) of the samples at baseline did not differ significantly among the groups (parent’s age: $\chi _{{(2)}}^{2} $ =2·121, P=0·346; parent’s educational level: $\chi _{{(4)}}^{2} $ =2·852, P=0·583; household situation: $\chi _{{(4)}}^{2} $ =4·565, P=0·335; child’s age: $\chi _{{(8)}}^{2} $ =12·743, P=0·121; child’s sex: $\chi _{{(2)}}^{2} $ =0·262, P=0·877; child’s gestational age at birth: $\chi _{{(2)}}^{2} $ =3·193, P=0·203; child’s health chronic problems: $\chi _{{(2)}}^{2} $ =0·306, P=0·858).

Intervention effects

Based on the results of the repeated-measures ANOVA for children’s intake of healthy food, Mauchly’s test indicated that the assumption of sphericity had been violated ( $\chi _{{(5)}}^{2} $ =24·573, P<0·001); therefore, degrees of freedom were corrected using Greenhouse–Geisser estimates of sphericity (ε=0·87). Significant effects were found for the condition×time interaction (within-subjects effects, F (5,214;297·184)=2·370, P=0·037, $\eta _{{\rm p}}^{2} $ =0·040) and condition (between-subjects effects, F (2;114)=5·722, P=0·004, $\eta _{{\rm p}}^{2} $ =0·091). Further post hoc analysis revealed that there were significant differences between baseline and post-intervention assessments for the CIG (P=0·004), suggesting a significant increase in the frequency of children’s healthy food intake after the ‘Red Apple’ intervention. For the MIG, a significant decrease was found in the frequency of children’s healthy food intake at the 6-month assessment compared with the post-intervention assessment (P=0·046). The CIG differed significantly from the other groups in each follow-up assessment, with higher values for children’s healthy food intake (post-intervention: CIG v. MIG, P=0·028 and CIG v. CG, P=0·000; 6-month assessment: CIG v. MIG, P=0·014 and CIG v. CG, P=0·026; 1-year assessment: CIG v. MIG, P=0·028 and CIG v. MIG, P=0·017).

The same statistical procedure showed a statistically significant effect of the condition×time interaction on parental self-efficacy (within-subjects effects, F (6,342)=2·877, P=0·010, $\eta _{{\rm p}}^{2} $ =0·048). In the CG, the baseline assessment differed significantly from the post-intervention (P=0·027) and 1-year assessments (P=0·010), indicating that there was a significant decrease in parental self-efficacy at both follow-up evaluation points for this group compared with that at the baseline assessment.

No significant results regarding the effect of the condition×time interaction were found for the other metric variables examined (unhealthy eating habits: F (5,605;313,906)=0·757, P=0·596, $\eta _{{\rm p}}^{2} $ =0·013; coercive strategies: F (5,492;302,064)=0·247, P=0·952, $\eta _{{\rm p}}^{2} $ =0·004; inductive strategies: F (6;333)=1·184, P=0·314, $\eta _{{\rm p}}^{2} $ =0·021; nutritional knowledge: F (6;339)=0·583, P=0·744, $\eta _{{\rm p}}^{2} $ =0·010). The proportion of parents in each group with a correct perception of their children’s weight did not differ over time (CIG: $\chi _{{(3)}}^{2} $ =1·723, P=0·632; MIG: $\chi _{{(3)}}^{2} $ =2·118, P=0·548; CG: $\chi _{{(3)}}^{2} $ =1·723, P=0·632).

Process evaluation

At least sixteen of the eighteen activities that were designed to be accomplished throughout the ‘Red Apple’ sessions were delivered for all the groups. Homework assignments were completed by at least 70 % of parents and 85 % of teachers in all sessions. More than 90 % of parents reported that each component of the programme was useful/very useful for changing their child’s eating behaviours and interesting/very interesting in terms of knowledge transmitted and tasks proposed.

Discussion

The main objective of the present study was to conduct a pilot study to evaluate the effectiveness of a brief parental school-based intervention to promote young children’s healthy eating behaviours, particularly its impact on specific behavioural (children’s healthy and unhealthy eating habits, parental feeding practices) and cognitive variables (parents’ perception of the child’s weight, self-efficacy and nutritional knowledge).

The ‘Red Apple’ intervention (CIG) was the only condition that led to an improvement in children’s healthy food intake after the intervention, although this difference became non-significant in the follow-up assessments. Nevertheless, the CIG maintained significant positive differences at those assessment points compared with the MIG and the CG. These results are especially important since the mean scores for this variable were already relatively high at baseline (on average, children consumed healthy foods more than 3–6 times per week). In contrast, the impact of the programme on children’s unhealthy food intake was not noteworthy. Similar results were found after the HENRY (Health Exercise Nutrition for the Really Young) parent course( Reference Willis, George and Hunt 29 ) where a significant improvement in fruit and vegetable intake was achieved for both children and parents, although the reduction in sugar-sweetened foods and beverages was only found for parents. One possible explanation is that the children’s consumption of unhealthy foods was already low before the intervention (the mean intake of unhealthy food was less than 1–2 times per week). A Portuguese study that assessed dietary habits of 4-year-old children showed that 65 % of the sample consumed cakes and sweets at least once daily, and 52 % had sodas and soft drinks daily( Reference Lopes, Oliveira and Afonso 36 ). Although the measures used were not completely equivalent, it is possible that our sample had better values for this food group from before the intervention, thus creating a floor effect. However, these results might also indicate that it is easier to promote healthy eating behaviours than to decrease unhealthy eating behaviours. Forthcoming interventions may need to direct more attention to parents’ beliefs and behaviours related to the control of unhealthy foods.

Although children’s eating is measured differently in the literature (e.g. through the use of different instruments and timings of assessment), thus making outcome comparisons between studies difficult( Reference Hingle, O’Connor and Dave 17 ), we may conclude that, in general, a positive and long-lasting impact of parental interventions on children’s eating patterns is not common( Reference Nixon, Moore and Douthwaite 15 , Reference Hingle, O’Connor and Dave 17 , Reference Peters, Sinn and Campbell 21 ), especially with brief interventions( Reference Peters, Sinn and Campbell 21 ). Although the results obtained are modest, the changes in the CIG suggest preliminary evidence for the effectiveness of the strategies used. For instance, features of the ‘Red Apple’ intervention, such as suggesting specific practices according to the developmental competencies of pre-school children (e.g. the most adequate ways to talk with young children about food, recognition of the child’s ability to self-regulate the food intake through satiety cues), are relatively new in the way they are presented to parents. Future studies involving children with high-risk eating habits, as well as more sessions in the programme, are needed to confirm the effectiveness of this approach.

Regarding parental feeding strategies, the programme was not able to promote positive changes. This finding can be explained by the low dosage of the intervention, since only two sessions were dedicated to this issue. However, several tasks were proposed for parents to practice at home. In general, parents accomplished those tasks according to their specific problems (e.g. introduction of new and healthy foods for children with a less diversified diet, the ‘belly training’ for children who consumed higher portions of food), thus showing that these tasks were acceptable and pertinent. Earlier studies showed mixed results regarding changes in specific parental feeding practices after an intervention. In two programmes that also involved a brief educational intervention( Reference Horodynski and Stommel 37 , Reference Shelton, LeGros and Norton 38 ), no significant differences were found. In contrast, a decrease in restrictive feeding practices was achieved after Harvey-Berino and Rourke’s intervention, which included sixteen one-hour weekly home visits( Reference Harvey-Berino and Rourke 39 ), as well as a study by West et al. which included a 12-week intervention group plus phone call sessions( Reference West, Sanders and Cleghorn 40 ). Both interventions were longer, more intensive and used individual coaching and feedback.

Nevertheless, the ‘Red Apple’ intervention was successful in the maintenance of parents’ self-efficacy to regulate their children’s eating behaviours towards a healthy diet. In fact, we found a significant decrease in parental self-efficacy for the CG but not for the intervention groups. As children grow, their eating behaviour becomes more difficult to control and they are more assertive in imposing their food likes and dislikes. Campbell et al.’s early work suggests that mothers’ self-efficacy regarding the limitation of non-core foods/drinks may decline during the first years of a child’s life( Reference Campbell, Hesketh and Silverii 22 ). As such, it is possible that the intervention might have had a protective effect against this parental self-efficacy decline. Earlier studies found a significant increase in parents’ confidence in eliciting their children’s compliance with a healthy diet, to encourage good behaviour towards food and to set limits after the intervention, but the absence of a control group did not allow for further detailed comparisons( Reference Willis, George and Hunt 29 , Reference Harvey and Coleman 41 ). Another study, which involved a Group Lifestyle Triple P intervention, found an increase in self-efficacy at 1-year follow-up, but the intervention was much longer and directed exclusively towards overweight children( Reference West, Sanders and Cleghorn 40 ). Two other studies did not find improvements in parents’ self-efficacy( Reference Horodynski and Stommel 37 , Reference Harvey-Berino and Rourke 39 ).

An unexpected result was the absence of significant findings for nutritional knowledge in the intervention groups. Earlier intervention studies showed an increase in parental nutritional knowledge( Reference Horodynski and Stommel 37 , Reference Haire-Joshu, Elliott and Caito 42 ). Parents in the CIG, despite not improving their nutritional knowledge, were able to make the changes necessary for their children to increase their healthy eating behaviours. It is possible that these parents already had a minimum level of knowledge necessary to change their behaviours.

Additionally, the results on parental perceptions of children’s weight did not confirm our initial hypothesis. Previous literature has emphasized that nutrition education programmes that include screening of children’s BMI and feedback to parents can contribute to earlier awareness of children’s weight patterns and active engagement in the adoption of healthier eating habits( Reference Moore, Harris and Bradlyn 34 , Reference Park, Falconer and Saxena 43 Reference Jouret, Ahluwalia and Dupuy 45 ). Furthermore, in our programme, we tried to overcome some barriers to the correct identification of children’s weight status by parents, such as the understanding of the child’s growth charts and percentiles( Reference Ben-Joseph, Dowshen and Izenberg 46 ). However, it is possible that the time spent in this intervention or the group approach used was insufficient for parents to acquire the ability to observe the initial signs of weight problems and adjust their perceptions of the children’s development. Further studies might consider the additional participation of a physician or a nurse in the programme to address the information about how to calculate the children’s BMI and percentiles and to assess the risks to the children’s health associated with their nutritional status. An early study found that parental recall of physicians’ warnings about children’s overweight are highly correlated with parental concerns about their children’s weight, increasing up to six times their probability of being concerned( Reference Gomes, Barros and Pereira 30 ).

It is important to notice that although the ‘Red Apple’ programme was developed and designed for a universal and group application, the general objectives of the sessions may not fully match the needs of all participants. For some parents, it may be necessary to enhance the perception of risk regarding overweight or unhealthy eating habits; for others, it might be more important to provide strategies to regulate the child’s defiant behaviour during mealtimes (asking for more food, refusing vegetables). This might be especially relevant in clinical samples, where parents and children could have specific issues to address during the intervention( Reference Foster, Farragher and Parker 47 , Reference Hoelscher, Kirk and Ritchie 48 ). As such, the way the programme is built also allows its extension, combining group sessions with an individual component, or introducing booster sessions to reinforce changes. Moreover, the ‘Red Apple’ programme, although not currently applied, is manualized and available to be disseminated in kindergartens.

Limitations

The difficulty in maintaining reasonable rates of adherence throughout the programme was a major problem in the present study, although this issue was also frequent in other studies involving preventive programmes and extended follow-up assessments( Reference Skelton and Beech 49 ). Considering the parents’ reasons for non-attendance, it could be important that future studies offer additional solutions to overcome these barriers (e.g. offering childcare during the session, sessions in the morning or in the evening). The use of technologies to deliver some programme components and offer individual feedback( Reference Turner, Spruijt‐Metz and Wen 50 , Reference Tate, Spruijt-Metz and O’Reilly 51 ) might result in lower attrition. In our sample, parents with lower education levels dropped out more often in both intervention groups. Although these parents may be more focused on other priorities, additional efforts must be made to understand families’ expectations about these programmes and to identify and overcome the barriers to change.

It is also possible that the small sample retained until the end of the study might have limited the statistical power of the analyses conducted. Additionally, in our sample, most of the parents had a college education, which could be less representative of the Portuguese population, and thus limit the generalizability of the results. Also, the results may not be replicable with children with unhealthier eating behaviours or with parents less motivated to participate. Further investigations with larger and/or clinical samples are needed to confirm the results obtained.

Given that the results were assessed only through parental report, it is important to consider the potential bias of reporting positive outcome data( Reference Nixon, Moore and Douthwaite 15 ), especially in the intervention conditions, in which parental involvement and contact with the research team were higher (e.g. social desirability and expectations), although this positive bias in the intervention groups was not observed across all dimensions.

Conclusions

In summary, the ‘Red Apple’ programme proved to be effective in the improvement of children’s healthy food intake compared with a single nutrition educational session and a control condition. Participation in an intervention condition might also prevent the decline of parental self-efficacy in promoting healthier food intake as their children grow older. The absence of significant results regarding other parental variables targeted in the ‘Red Apple’ intervention, in addition to problems with attrition, highlights the need to optimize the strategies adopted (e.g. regarding session contents and methodologies, the dosage of the intervention) to augment the success of the programme.

Acknowledgements

Acknowledgements: The authors sincerely thank for their kind collaboration all the school boards and, especially, the parents, children and kindergarten teachers who were enrolled in the study. Financial support: This work was supported by the Faculty of Psychology, University of Lisbon (grant number BD/FPUL-052010). The funder had no role in the design, analysis or writing of this article. Conflict of interest: The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest. Authorship: All authors participated in the literature research, conception and design of the study. A.I.G. performed the data collection. A.I.G., A.I.P. and M.S.R. carried out the statistical analysis, and together all authors interpreted and discussed the results. A.I.G. prepared the manuscript and all authors read, edited and approved the final version submitted for publication. Ethics of human subject participation: This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and all procedures involving human subjects were approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Psychology, University of Lisbon, and by the boards of all schools involved. Written informed consent was obtained from all parents. After receiving a brief explanation of the purposes of the study and the tasks involved, all children gave their verbal consent in the presence of the kindergarten teacher, which was formally recorded.

References

1. Rietmeijer-Mentink, M, Paulis, W, van Middelkoop, M et al. (2013) Difference between parental perception and actual weight status of children: a systematic review. Matern Child Nutr 9, 322.Google Scholar
2. Ng, M, Fleming, T, Robinson, M et al. (2014) Global, regional, and national prevalence of overweight and obesity in children and adults during 1980–2013: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2013. Lancet 384, 766781.Google Scholar
3. Aparício, G, Cunha, M, Duarte, J et al. (2013) Nutritional status in preschool children: current trends of mother’s body perception and concerns. Aten Primaria 45, Espec Cong 1, 194200.Google Scholar
4. Viveiro, C, Brito, S & Moleiro, P (2016) Pediatric overweight and obesity: the Portuguese reality. Rev Port Saude Publica 34, 3037.Google Scholar
5. Wofford, LG (2008) Systematic review of childhood obesity prevention. J Pediatr Nurs 23, 519.Google Scholar
6. Dietz, W (1997) Periods of risk in childhood for the development of adult obesity: what do we need to learn? J Nutr 127, issue 9, 1884S1886S.Google Scholar
7. Campbell, M, Williams, J, Hampton, A et al. (2006) Maternal concern and perceptions of overweight in Australian preschool-aged children. Med J Aust 184, 274277.Google Scholar
8. Birch, LL & Fisher, J (1998) Development of eating behaviors among children and adolescents. Pediatrics 101, 539549.Google Scholar
9. Birch, LL (1998) Development of food acceptance patterns in the first years of life. Proc Nutr Soc 57, 617624.Google Scholar
10. Manios, Y, Grammatikaki, E, Androutsos, O et al. (2012) A systematic approach for the development of a kindergarten-based intervention for the prevention of obesity in preschool age children: the ToyBox-study. Obes Rev 13, Suppl. 1, 312.Google Scholar
11. Anzman, S, Rollins, B & Birch, L (2010) Parental influence on children’s early eating environments and obesity risk: implications for prevention. Int J Obes (Lond) 34, 11161124.Google Scholar
12. World Health Organization (2016) Report of the Commission on Ending Childhood Obesity. Geneva: WHO Press.Google Scholar
13. Brown, T, Kelly, S & Summerbell, C (2007) Prevention of obesity: a review of interventions. Obes Rev 8, Suppl. 1, 127130.Google Scholar
14. Bluford, D, Sherry, B & Scanlon, K (2007) Interventions to prevent or treat obesity in preschool children: a review of evaluated programs. Obesity (Silver Spring) 15, 13561372.Google Scholar
15. Nixon, C, Moore, H, Douthwaite, W et al. (2012) Identifying effective behavioural models and behaviour change strategies underpinning preschool and school-based obesity prevention interventions aimed at 4–6-year-olds: a systematic review. Obes Rev 13, Suppl. 1, 106117.Google Scholar
16. Hesketh, K & Campbell, K (2010) Interventions to prevent obesity in 0–5 year olds: an updated systematic review of the literature. Obesity (Silver Spring) 18, Suppl. 1, S27S35.Google Scholar
17. Hingle, M, O’Connor, T, Dave, J et al. (2010) Parental involvement in interventions to improve child dietary intake: a systematic review. Prev Med 51, 103111.Google Scholar
18. Hendrie, GA, Lease, HJ, Bowen, J et al. (2017) Strategies to increase children’s vegetable intake in home and community settings: a systematic review of literature. Matern Child Nutr 13, 122.Google Scholar
19. World Health Organization (2016) Consideration of the Evidence on Childhood Obesity for the Commission on Ending Childhood Obesity: Report of the Ad hoc Working Group on Science and Evidence for Ending Childhood Obesity. Geneva: WHO Press.Google Scholar
20. Skouteris, H, McCabe, M, Swinburn, B et al. (2011) Parental influence and obesity prevention in pre-schoolers: a systematic review of interventions. Obes Rev 12, 315332.Google Scholar
21. Peters, J, Sinn, N, Campbell, K et al. (2012) Parental influences on the diets of 2–5-year-old children: systematic review of interventions. Early Child Dev Care 182, 837857.Google Scholar
22. Campbell, K, Hesketh, K, Silverii, A et al. (2010) Maternal self-efficacy regarding children’s eating and sedentary behaviours in the early years: associations with children’s food intake and sedentary behaviours. Int J Pediat Obes 5, 501508.Google Scholar
23. Warschburger, P & Kröller, K (2012) Childhood overweight and obesity: maternal perceptions of the time for engaging in child weight management. BMC Public Health 12, 295.Google Scholar
24. Rhee, K, DeLago, C, Arscott-Mills, T et al. (2005) Factors associated with parental readiness to make changes for overweight children. Pediatrics 116, e94e101.Google Scholar
25. Morris, H, Skouteris, H, Edwards, S et al. (2015) Obesity prevention interventions in early childhood education and care settings with parental involvement: a systematic review. Early Child Dev Care 185, 12831313.Google Scholar
26. Maynard, M, Galuska, D, Blanck, H et al. (2003) Maternal perceptions of weight status of children. Pediatrics 111, 12261231.Google Scholar
27. Bandura, A (2006) Guide for constructing self-efficacy scales. In Self-efficacy Beliefs of Adolescents, vol. 5, pp. 307337 [F Pajares and T Urdan, editors]. Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
28. Vereecken, C & Maes, L (2010) Young children’s dietary habits and associations with the mothers’ nutritional knowledge and attitudes. Appetite 54, 4451.Google Scholar
29. Willis, TA, George, J, Hunt, C et al. (2014) Combating child obesity: impact of HENRY on parenting and family lifestyle. Pediatr Obes 9, 339350.Google Scholar
30. Gomes, AI, Barros, L & Pereira, AI (2017) Predictors of parental concerns about child weight in parents of healthy-weight and overweight 2–6 year olds. Appetite 108, 491497.Google Scholar
31. Hughes, S, Power, T, Fisher, JO et al. (2005) Revisiting a neglected construct: parenting styles in a child-feeding context. Appetite 44, 8392.Google Scholar
32. Rogers, RW (1983) Cognitive and physiological processes in fear appeals and attitude change: a revised theory of protection motivation. In Social Psychology: A Source Book, pp. 153176 [JR Cacioppo and RE Petty, editors]. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
33. Ajzen, I (1985) From intention to actions: a theory of planned behavior. In Action-Control: From Cognition to Behavior, pp. 1139 [J Kuhl and J Beckman, editors]. Heidelberg: Springer.Google Scholar
34. Moore, L, Harris, C & Bradlyn, A (2012) Exploring the relationship between parental concern and the management of childhood obesity. Matern Child Health J 16, 902908.Google Scholar
35. Hildebrand, DA & Betts, NM (2009) Assessment of stage of change, decisional balance, self-efficacy, and use of processes of change of low-income parents for increasing servings of fruits and vegetables to preschool-aged children. J Nutr Educ Behav 41, 110119.Google Scholar
36. Lopes, C, Oliveira, A, Afonso, L et al. (2014) Consumo alimentar e nutricional de crianças em idade pré-escolar: Resultados da coorte Geração 21 (Food and Nutritional Intake of Preschool Children: Results from the 21 Generation Cohort). Porto: Instituto de Saúde Pública da Universidade do Porto.Google Scholar
37. Horodynski, MA & Stommel, M (2005) Nutrition education aimed at toddlers: an intervention study. Pediatr Nurs 31, 364372.Google Scholar
38. Shelton, D, LeGros, K, Norton, L et al. (2007) Randomised controlled trial: a parent-based group education programme for overweight children. J Paediatr Child Health 43, 799805.Google Scholar
39. Harvey-Berino, J & Rourke, J (2003) Obesity prevention in preschool Native-American children: a pilot study using home visiting. Obes Res 11, 606611.Google Scholar
40. West, F, Sanders, MR, Cleghorn, GJ et al. (2010) Randomised clinical trial of a family-based lifestyle intervention for childhood obesity involving parents as the exclusive agents of change. Behav Res Ther 48, 11701179.Google Scholar
41. Harvey, H & Coleman, G (2008) Raising healthy eaters: a parenting and nutrition curriculum. J Nutr Educ Behav 40, 5253.Google Scholar
42. Haire-Joshu, D, Elliott, MB, Caito, NM et al. (2008) High 5 for Kids: the impact of a home visiting program on fruit and vegetable intake of parents and their preschool children. Prev Med 47, 7782.Google Scholar
43. Park, M, Falconer, C, Saxena, S et al. (2013) Perceptions of health risk among parents of overweight children: a cross-sectional study within a cohort. Prev Med 57, 5559.Google Scholar
44. Crawford, D, Timperio, A, Telford, A et al. (2006) Parental concerns about childhood obesity and the strategies employed to prevent unhealthy weight gain in children. Public Health Nutr 9, 889895.Google Scholar
45. Jouret, B, Ahluwalia, N, Dupuy, M et al. (2009) Prevention of overweight in preschool children: results of kindergarten-based interventions. Int J Obes (Lond) 33, 10751083.Google Scholar
46. Ben-Joseph, E, Dowshen, S & Izenberg, N (2009) Do parents understand growth charts? A national, internet-based survey. Pediatrics 124, 11001109.Google Scholar
47. Foster, BA, Farragher, J, Parker, P et al. (2015) Treatment interventions for early childhood obesity: a systematic review. Acad Pediatr 15, 353361.Google Scholar
48. Hoelscher, DM, Kirk, S, Ritchie, L et al. (2013) Position of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics: interventions for the prevention and treatment of pediatric overweight and obesity. J Acad Nutr Diet 113, 13751394.Google Scholar
49. Skelton, J & Beech, B (2011) Attrition in paediatric weight management: a review of the literature and new directions. Obes Rev 12, e273e281.Google Scholar
50. Turner, T, Spruijt‐Metz, D, Wen, CKF et al. (2015) Prevention and treatment of pediatric obesity using mobile and wireless technologies: a systematic review. Pediatr Obes 10, 403409.Google Scholar
51. Tate, EB, Spruijt-Metz, D, O’Reilly, G et al. (2013) mHealth approaches to child obesity prevention: successes, unique challenges, and next directions. Transl Behav Med 3, 406415.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Flow of the participants through the pilot study of the ‘Red Apple’ programme, a parental school-based intervention to promote healthy eating behaviours in young children (aged 3–6 years), Lisbon, Portugal, October 2011–June 2014

Figure 1

Table 1 Sociodemographic and clinical variables for the three group conditions at baseline in the pilot study of the ‘Red Apple’ programme, a parental school-based intervention to promote healthy eating behaviours in young children (aged 3–6 years), Lisbon, Portugal, October 2011–June 2014

Figure 2

Table 2 ‘Red Apple’ intervention: structure and content of sessions

Figure 3

Table 3 Parent and child variables at baseline, post-intervention, 6-month and 1-year assessments for the three group conditions in the pilot study of the ‘Red Apple’ programme, a parental school-based intervention to promote healthy eating behaviours in young children (aged 3–6 years), Lisbon, Portugal, October 2011–June 2014