Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T04:59:12.475Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Future directions in prenatal stress research: Challenges and opportunities related to advancing our understanding of prenatal developmental origins of risk for psychopathology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 August 2018

Colleen Doyle*
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota
Dante Cicchetti
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Colleen Doyle at doyle291@umn.edu and Dante Cicchetti at cicchett@umn.edu.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Special Issue Editorial
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

Throughout history, scholars have explored the idea that experiences early in life have a profound and persisting influence on health and well-being (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti2017). Over the past century, this idea has inspired the emergence of a vigorous field of research dedicated to understanding how the prenatal environment shapes long-term developmental outcomes. The foundation of this literature can be found in vanguard studies on fetal physiology dating back to the 1930s and 1940s (Barcroft, Reference Barcroft1946; Gottlieb Reference Gottlieb1976, Reference Gottlieb and Mussen1983; Sontag & Richards, Reference Sontag and Richards1938). Today, the convergence of diverse disciplines continues to fuel an expansion of research that has given rise to a conceptual paradigm called the developmental origins of health and disease (DOHaD; Gluckman & Hanson, Reference Gluckman and Hanson2006; Wadhwa, Buss, Entrigner, & Swanson, 2010). Sometimes known as “Barker's hypothesis,” the “thrifty phenotype hypothesis,” or the “fetal programming hypothesis,” the central hypothesis guiding this approach is that the in utero environment shapes fetal development and subsequently sets probabilistic parameters for both adaptive and maladaptive outcomes. Historically, the emphasis of DOHaD research has been on physical health outcomes, such as cardiovascular disease, obesity, and type 2 diabetes. More recent work examining neurobehavioral and clinical outcomes has provided compelling evidence of long-term educational, behavioral, and psychological sequelae of prenatal stress. Of note, the influence of developmental principles on the field of DOHaD research is increasingly visible, for example, in a growing interest in plasticity and the probabilistic nature of development (in contrast to “programmed” outcomes; Cicchetti & Curtis, Reference Cicchetti, Curtis, Cicchetti and Cohen2006), in the recognition of the bidirectional relationship between a pregnant woman and a fetus (e.g., DiPietro, Reference DiPietro2010), as well as in the recognition of pregnancy as a significant period of organization and development for a fetus and a woman, and in a mounting interest in the association between prenatal stress and adaptive outcomes and individual differences (and not only maladaptive outcomes) and what factors and processes may buffer stress effects and promote resilience.

Despite the recent maturation and rapid growth of the DOHaD field of research, precisely how prenatal stress may alter developmental trajectories is not well understood. The goal of this Special Issue is to examine the wealth of research investigating prenatal stress and its effects on offspring development. Our hope is that the research featured in this issue will increase our understanding of the putative mechanisms and pathways by which variation in the prenatal environment may have a persisting influence on developmental organization and outcomes in infancy, childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. Our aims for this Special Issue are threefold. The first two aims relate to twin themes of theory building and theory appraisal within this area of work. In this issue, investigators outline theoretical frameworks that have guided recent prenatal stress research, and examine the accumulating evidence that support, amend, or extend these frameworks. These reviews reveal an increasing emphasis on moderating and mediating processes that may influence or explain the association between prenatal factors and offspring risk for psychopathology.

The third theme of the issue is the consideration of challenges (and opportunities) related to advancing our understanding of the prenatal origins of risk for psychopathology. Among the challenges touched on by authors in this issue are three challenges we briefly review here: developing a systematic conceptual framework for defining and studying prenatal stress; parsing the heterogeneity of the construct of prenatal stress in the service of identifying latent risk phenotypes; and leveraging experimental and quasi-experimental designs and novel statistical approaches to conduct informative tests of the fetal programming hypothesis.

The first major challenge to the field is related to the current lack of a strong systematic conceptual framework, or nomological net (Cronbach & Meehl, Reference Cronbach and Meehl1955), for studying the construct of prenatal stress. This effort begins with the need for definitional and conceptual consensus and clarity, as the delineation of prenatal stress effects depends on the definition of the construct itself (e.g., see Barnett, Manly, & Cicchetti, Reference Barnett, Manly, Cicchetti, Cicchetti and Toth1993; Cicchetti & Barnett, Reference Cicchetti, Barnett, Grove and Cicchetti1991). Currently, within the literature prenatal stress is a complex umbrella term that encompasses many types of maternal factors that can influence the in utero environment, including physiological, psychological, and behavioral responses to perceived threat or challenge. These factors can include variables related to biological functioning (i.e., hormonal and immune), cognitive and affective states associated with “distress” or negative mood (i.e., frustration, anxiety, and depression), and characteristic ways of experiencing, managing, or responding to stressful events (i.e., personality traits, cognitive appraisal processes, coping strategies, and health behaviors). Frequently, in human studies of prenatal stress, “stress” is conceptualized as distress (Kemeny, Reference Kemeny2003). This is problematic for several reasons. First, not all stressful states are negative; stressful events that are mild, brief, and controllable function as normative developmental experiences, and can be experienced as positive and exert a positive influence on development. Second, the perception or interpretation of whether something is stressful depends on characteristics of the individual, which investigators often do not attempt to measure or account for, such as personality traits, outlook on life, and views of the world as safe or unsafe. Of note, DiPietro, Costigan, and Sipsma (Reference DiPietro, Costigan and Sipsma2008) have shown that correlations among self-report measures of perceived stress and symptoms of depression and anxiety are high during pregnancy and remain stable through the first 2 years after delivery. These results suggest that measures of prenatal distress may gauge important moderating factors and mediating processes such as personality traits and social support. Third, although anxiety, depression, and chronic psychological stress have been associated with the dysregulation of biological stress response systems, primarily the hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal axis, stress hormones do not function as “emotion juice” (Gunnar, Reference Gunnar2016; Hennessy & Levine, Reference Hennessy and Levine1979). Instead, mood dysregulation likely has unique biological mechanisms, such as serotonergic activity, that may go overlooked within a “stress” approach (Beijers, Buitelaar, & de Weerth, Reference Beijers, Buitelaar and de Weerth2014). Due to the lack of definitional and conceptual consensus on prenatal stress, it is unclear if studies examining the effects of the maternal biological stress response in humans, or translating prenatal stress findings from animal models, are investigating the same phenomena as studies examining psychological distress. The field's current reliance on a conceptual model of prenatal stress that “lumps” potential mediating and/or moderating processes in with stressors is conceptually unclear and empirically problematic (Grant et al., Reference Grant, Compas, Stuhlmacher, Thurm, McMahon and Halpert2003). Future research that defines and measures prenatal stress factors and related moderating and mediating variables, and provides data to integrate, identify, and quantify the higher order interrelationships among and between them, has the potential to profoundly advance our knowledge of how the construct of prenatal stress operates to shape developmental outcomes.

The difficulty of defining the construct of prenatal stress is closely related to its heterogeneous nature. Due to this variability, maternal exposure risk groups are latent and unobservable, and this represents a second challenge/opportunity facing the field. Contributing to the complexity of prenatal stress is variability across the typical-to-atypical continuum of (dys)regulation in mood and stress regulation systems in the context of pregnancy, as well as heterogeneity within and across symptoms of psychiatric disorders. One approach to this challenge of characterizing the complexity of the construct of prenatal stress is the research domain criteria (RDoC) initiative (Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn and Wang2010). RDoC seeks quantitative approaches that are agnostic to traditional psychiatric nosology to improve characterization of the genotypic and phenotypic heterogeneity observed within and across DSM disorders. To this end, RDoC has formalized research strategies for parsing the heterogeneity inherent to the etiology, phenotypic presentation, and treatment response of major psychiatric disorders using data-driven subtyping. Identifying and defining different (and more homogeneous) maternal subtypes can advance a deeper understanding of prenatal stress and its effects by facilitating the discovery of maternal risk phenotypes and multiple etiological pathways whereby prenatal stress can lead to various effects on offspring development. These data are needed to enable the design of effective and timely preventative interventions for pregnant women and their children. Examples of subtyping are rapidly accumulating in different literatures (e.g., Bebko et al., Reference Bebko, Bertocci, Fournier, Hinze, Bonar, Almeida and Gill2014; Ivleva et al., Reference Ivleva, Clementz, Dutcher, Arnold, Jeon-Slaughter, Aslan and Pearlson2017) with a significant body of evidence emerging from the B-SNIP study that has identified distinct neurobiological “biotypes” that cross clinical diagnostic boundaries (e.g., Karalunas et al., Reference Karalunas, Fair, Musser, Aykes, Iyer and Nigg2014). These studies leverage novel, data-driven statistical techniques such as machine learning or factor mixture modeling (e.g., see Lubke & Muthén, Reference Lubke and Muthén2005; Orru, Pettersson-Yeo, Marquand, Sartori, & Mechelli, Reference Orru, Pettersson-Yeo, Marquand, Sartori and Mechelli2012) that effectively reduce data dimensions and identify meaningful phenotypes. The growing subtyping literature reinforces that greater measurement precision is required to identify more homogenous subgroups. Although most DOHaD studies to date have examined a single prenatal stress exposure, prenatal stress risk factors typically do not occur in isolation (Stroud, McCallum, & Salisbury, Reference Sroud, McCallum and Salisbury2018 [this issue]), thus, to fully understand how these distinct variables relate to one another in the prediction of psychopathology, it is important to define and measure each of these variables separately. Expanding measurement to include a wider range of risk factors, moderators, and mediators will be key to identifying risk groups and understanding the complex pathways and mechanisms by which the prenatal and perinatal environment influences neurobehavioral trajectories. Finally, existing subtyping studies reveal that parsing the complexity of prenatal stress via data-driven subtyping approaches will require the recruitment of larger samples to increase statistical power. Already, prenatal stress investigators have called for the field to leverage the benefits of a “big data” approach (de Weerth, Reference deWeerth2018 [this issue]). Studies like the recent NIH research project, PregSource, will begin to help fill this gap (Kuehn, Reference Kuehn2017).

A third challenge facing the field is related to strengthening causal inference. Although epidemiological and correlational research with human subjects supports the “fetal origins hypothesis,” confounding variables prevent these studies from providing definitive proof (Thapar & Rutter, Reference Thapar and Rutter2009). Strong causal evidence for prenatal stress effects on offspring development comes primarily from animal experiments; however, animal models have both benefits and limitations (Lickliter, Reference Lickliter2018 [this issue]). Currently, prenatal stress research is increasingly moving from a long phase of documenting correlations between offspring outcomes and variables that might be putative causes, to studies that use experimental or quasi-experimental designs to conduct informative tests of causal hypotheses. In this issue, investigators draw attention to the benefits of incorporating novel and genetically informed designs and statistical approaches, as well as translational synthesis across human and animal studies, which are needed to improve causal inference and conduct a “risky test” (Popper, Reference Popper1959). There is growing recognition that assessing and controlling for inherited confounders, postnatal risk, and even preconception risk is required to disaggregate the influence of unique prenatal environmental exposures from other factors. In addition, the importance of randomized controlled trial designs as an experimental test of the fetal programming hypothesis is noted (Davis, Hankin, Swales, & Hoffman, Reference Davis, Hankin, Swales and Hoffman2018 [this issue]; Goodman, Cullum, Dimidjian, River, & Kim, Reference Goodman, Cullum, Dimidjian, River and Kim2018 [this issue]).

The prenatal period is a time of rapid and dynamic development, during which the brain develops from a simple three cell-layer structure to a complex network of neurons and glia designed to support the increasingly complex behaviors that are necessary for successful navigation of the ex utero environment (Ciaranello et al., Reference Ciaranello, Aimi, Dean, Morilak, Porteus, Cicchetti, Cicchetti and Cohen1995; Stiles, Reference Stiles2008). This time of enhanced development, plasticity, and potential vulnerability represents an unmatched opportunity for adaptation to the environment, whether that environment involves exposure to harsh conditions or not. In recent decades, research from a wide range of disciplines has shown prenatal development provides a foundation for learning and health throughout the life span. The rapidly growing body of work examining prenatal stress and its effects and neurobehavioral outcomes indicates this topic is of increasing interest to researchers, clinicians, and policy makers (e.g., Glover, Reference Glover2011; Green et al., Reference Green, Damus, Simpson, Iams, Reece and Hobel2005; Huizink, Mulder, & Buitelaar, Reference Huizink, Mulder and Buitelaar2004; O'Donnell & Meaney, Reference O'Donnell and Meaney2016; Schetter & Tanner, Reference Schetter and Tanner2012). Understanding the prenatal developmental origins of risk for psychopathology has immense preventative and therapeutic value. We believe that this Special Issue will focus much-needed attention on theoretical frameworks and evidence that increases our understanding of how prenatal stress alters the in utero environment, shapes fetal development, and influences long-term outcomes. We also believe the research featured here will direct attention to many practical concerns involved in advancing this area of work and ultimately accelerating the translation of this science to practice. It has been said that “science is built up of facts, as a house is built of stones; but an accumulation of facts is no more a science than a heap of stones is a house” (Poincaré, Reference Poincare and Halsted1913). To begin to usher in a gestalt-like shift in our understanding of prenatal stress effects, we believe that investigators in this area must work toward elaborating a causal taxonomy of the construct of prenatal stress, parsing the complexity of prenatal stress to identify patterns of prenatal stress factors that may characterize maternal risk group profiles, and adopting novel designs and statistical approaches to test hypothesized causal pathways. We hope that the articles included in this Special Issue will stimulate innovative research initiatives directed toward an increased understanding of how prenatal stress may eventuate in psychopathology, and the exportation of this knowledge to inform strategic prevention efforts that may support optimal outcomes for women and their children.

Footnotes

Our work on this Special Issue was supported by grants from the National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship (to C.D.) and from the Emerald Foundation and the Klaus J. Jacobs Research Prize (to D.C.).

References

Barcroft, J. (1946). Researches on pre-natal life (Vol. 1). Oxford: Blackwell Scientific.Google Scholar
Barnett, D., Manly, J. T., & Cicchetti, D. (1993). Defining child maltreatment: The interface between policy and research. In Cicchetti, D. & Toth, S. L. (Eds.), Child abuse, child development, and social policy (pp. 774). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Bebko, G., Bertocci, M. A., Fournier, J. C., Hinze, A. K., Bonar, L., Almeida, J. R., … Gill, M. K. (2014). Parsing dimensional vs diagnostic category–related patterns of reward circuitry function in behaviorally and emotionally dysregulated youth in the longitudinal assessment of manic symptoms study. JAMA Psychiatry, 71, 7180. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.2870Google Scholar
Beijers, R., Buitelaar, J. K., & de Weerth, C. (2014). Mechanisms underlying the effects of prenatal psychosocial stress on child outcomes: Beyond the HPA axis. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 23, 943956. doi:10.1007/s00787-014-0566-3Google Scholar
Ciaranello, R., Aimi, J., Dean, R. S., Morilak, D., Porteus, M. H., & Cicchetti, D. (1995). Fundamentals of molecular neurobiology. In Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Theory and method (Vol. 1, pp. 109160). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (Ed.) (2017). [Special Issue]. Biological and behavioral effects of early adversity on multiple levels of development. Development and Psychopathology, 29, 15171986.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Barnett, D. (1991). Toward the development of a scientific nosology of child maltreatment. In Grove, W. & Cicchetti, D. (Eds.), Thinking clearly about psychology: Essays in honor of Paul E. Meehl: Personality and psychopathology (Vol. 2, pp. 346377). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Curtis, W. J. (2006). The developing brain and neural plasticity: Implications for normality, psychopathology, and resilience. In Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Developmental neuroscience (Vol. 2, 2nd ed., pp. 164). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Cronbach, L. J., & Meehl, P. E. (1955). Construct validity in psychological tests. Psychological Bulletin, 52, 281.doi:10.1037/h0040957Google Scholar
Davis, E. P., Hankin, B. L., Swales, D. A., & Hoffman, M. C. (2018). An experimental test of the fetal programming hypothesis: Can we reduce child ontogenetic vulnerability to psychopathology by decreasing maternal depression? Development and Psychopathology, 30, 787–806.Google Scholar
deWeerth, C. (2018). Prenatal stress and the development of psychopathology: Lifestyle behaviors as a fundamental part of the puzzle. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 1129–1144.Google Scholar
DiPietro, J. A. (2010). Psychological and psychophysiological considerations regarding the maternal–fetal relationship. Infant and Child Development, 19, 2738. doi:10.1002/icd.651Google Scholar
DiPietro, J. A., Costigan, K. A., & Sipsma, H. L. (2008). Continuity in self-report measures of maternal anxiety, stress, and depressive symptoms from pregnancy through two years postpartum. Journal of Psychosomatic Obstetrics & Gynecology, 29, 115124. doi:10.1080/01674820701701546Google Scholar
Glover, V. (2011). Annual research review: Prenatal stress and the origins of psychopathology: An evolutionary perspective. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 356367. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02371.xGoogle Scholar
Gluckman, P. D., & Hanson, M. A. (Eds.) (2006). Developmental origins of health and disease. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Goodman, S. H., Cullum, K. A., Dimidjian, S., River, L. M., & Kim, C. Y. (2018). Opening windows of opportunities: Evidence for interventions to prevent or treat depression in pregnant women being associated with changes in offspring's developmental trajectories of psychopathology risk. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 1179–1196.Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1976). Conceptions of prenatal development. Psychological Review, 83, 215234. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.83.3.215Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1983). The psychobiological approach to developmental issues. In Mussen, P. (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 126). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Grant, K. E., Compas, B. E., Stuhlmacher, A. F., Thurm, A. E., McMahon, S. D., & Halpert, J. A. (2003). Stressors and child and adolescent psychopathology: Moving from markers to mechanisms of risk. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 447. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.447Google Scholar
Green, N. S., Damus, K., Simpson, J. L., Iams, J., Reece, E. A., Hobel, C. J., … March of Dimes Scientific Advisory Committee on Prematurity. (2005). Research agenda for preterm birth: Recommendations from the March of Dimes. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 193, 626635. doi:10.1016/j.ajog.2005.02.106Google Scholar
Gunnar, M. R. (2016). Early life stress: What is the human chapter of the mammalian story? Child Development Perspectives, 10, 178183. doi:10.1111/cdep.12182Google Scholar
Hennessy, J. W., & Levine, S. (1979). Stress, arousal, and the pituitary-adrenal system: A psychoendocrine hypothesis. Progress in Psychobiology and Physiological Psychology, 8, 133178.Google Scholar
Huizink, A. C., Mulder, E. J., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2004). Prenatal stress and risk for psychopathology: Specific effects or induction of general susceptibility? Psychological Bulletin, 130, 115. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.1.115Google Scholar
Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., … Wang, P. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): Toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 748751. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379Google Scholar
Ivleva, E. I., Clementz, B. A., Dutcher, A. M., Arnold, S. J., Jeon-Slaughter, H., Aslan, S., … Pearlson, G. D. (2017). Brain structure biomarkers in the psychosis biotypes: Findings from the bipolar-schizophrenia network for intermediate phenotypes. Biological Psychiatry, 82, 2639. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.08.030Google Scholar
Karalunas, S. L., Fair, D., Musser, E. D., Aykes, K., Iyer, S. P., & Nigg, J. T. (2014). Subtyping attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder using temperament dimensions: Toward biologically based nosologic criteria. JAMA Psychiatry, 71, 10151024. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.763Google Scholar
Kemeny, M. E. (2003). The psychobiology of stress. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 124129. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.01246Google Scholar
Kuehn, B. (2017). Crowdsourcing pregnancy data. Journal of the American Medical Association, 318, 24182418. doi:10.1001/jama.2017.19923Google Scholar
Lickliter, R. (2018). The influence of prenatal experience on behavioral and social development: The benefits and limitations of an animal model. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 871–880.Google Scholar
Lubke, G. H., & Muthén, B. (2005). Investigating population heterogeneity with factor mixture models. Psychological Methods, 10, 21. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.10.1.21Google Scholar
O'Donnell, K. J., & Meaney, M. J. (2016). Fetal origins of mental health: The developmental origins of health and disease hypothesis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 174, 319328. doi:10.1176/appi.apj.2016.16020138Google Scholar
Orru, G., Pettersson-Yeo, W., Marquand, A. F., Sartori, G., & Mechelli, A. (2012). Using support vector machine to identify imaging biomarkers of neurological and psychiatric disease: A critical review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 11401152. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.01.004Google Scholar
Poincare, H. 1913. The foundations of science: Science and hypothesis, the value of science, science and method. Trans. Halsted, G. E.. New York: Science Press.Google Scholar
Popper, K. R. (1959). The logic of scientific discovery. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Schetter, C. D., & Tanner, L. (2012). Anxiety, depression and stress in pregnancy: Implications for mothers, children, research, and practice. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 25, 141148. doi:10.1097/YCO.0b013e3283503680Google Scholar
Sontag, L. W., & Richards, T. W. (1938). Studies in fetal behavior: I. Fetal heart rate as a behavioral indicator. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 3.Google Scholar
Sroud, L. R, McCallum, M., & Salisbury, A., (2018). Impact of maternal prenatal smoking on fetal to infant neurobehavioral development. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 10871105.Google Scholar
Stiles, J. (2008). The fundamentals of brain development: Integrating nature and nurture. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Thapar, A., & Rutter, M. (2009). Do prenatal risk factors cause psychiatric disorder? Be wary of causal claims. British Journal of Psychiatry, 195, 100101. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.109.062828Google Scholar