Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T05:07:14.898Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Dunn Worry Questionnaire and the Paranoia Worries Questionnaire: new assessments of worry

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 April 2019

Daniel Freeman*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Oxford, UK
Jessica C. Bird
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Oxford, UK
Bao S. Loe
Affiliation:
The Psychometrics Centre, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
David Kingdon
Affiliation:
Academic Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, University of Southampton, Southampton, UK
Helen Startup
Affiliation:
Sussex Partnership NHS Trust, Worthing, UK
David M. Clark
Affiliation:
Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Oxford, UK Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
Anke Ehlers
Affiliation:
Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Oxford, UK Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
Emma Černis
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Oxford, UK
Gail Wingham
Affiliation:
Academic Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, University of Southampton, Southampton, UK
Nicole Evans
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
Rachel Lister
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
Katherine Pugh
Affiliation:
Sussex Partnership NHS Trust, Worthing, UK
Jacinta Cordwell
Affiliation:
Oxford University Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, Oxford, UK
Graham Dunn
Affiliation:
Centre for Biostatistics, Institute of Population Health, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Daniel Freeman, E-mail: daniel.freeman@psych.ox.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

The cognitive process of worry, which keeps negative thoughts in mind and elaborates the content, contributes to the occurrence of many mental health disorders. Our principal aim was to develop a straightforward measure of general problematic worry suitable for research and clinical treatment. Our secondary aim was to develop a measure of problematic worry specifically concerning paranoid fears.

Methods

An item pool concerning worry in the past month was evaluated in 250 non-clinical individuals and 50 patients with psychosis in a worry treatment trial. Exploratory factor analysis and item response theory (IRT) informed the selection of scale items. IRT analyses were repeated with the scales administered to 273 non-clinical individuals, 79 patients with psychosis and 93 patients with social anxiety disorder. Other clinical measures were administered to assess concurrent validity. Test-retest reliability was assessed with 75 participants. Sensitivity to change was assessed with 43 patients with psychosis.

Results

A 10-item general worry scale (Dunn Worry Questionnaire; DWQ) and a five-item paranoia worry scale (Paranoia Worries Questionnaire; PWQ) were developed. All items were highly discriminative (DWQ a = 1.98–5.03; PWQ a = 4.10–10.7), indicating small increases in latent worry lead to a high probability of item endorsement. The DWQ was highly informative across a wide range of the worry distribution, whilst the PWQ had greatest precision at clinical levels of paranoia worry. The scales demonstrated excellent internal reliability, test-retest reliability, concurrent validity and sensitivity to change.

Conclusions

The new measures of general problematic worry and worry about paranoid fears have excellent psychometric properties.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Introduction

Excessive worry is identified as a contributory causal factor in many mental health disorders, including anxiety (Borkovec and Inz, Reference Borkovec and Inz1990), depression (Watkins, Reference Watkins2008), eating disorders (Sternheim et al., Reference Sternheim, Startup, Saeidi, Morgan, Hugo, Russell and Schmidt2012) and persecutory delusions (Freeman, Reference Freeman2016). Our view is that worry brings fearful ideas to mind, keeps them there and elaborates the content. Distress escalates and feared outcomes are judged as more likely to occur. We consider problematic worry to comprise: repeated thinking about problems that cause anxiety about the future; a focus on potential things that could go wrong; problems being catastrophised; a belief of lack of control over the thinking process; and interference in activities and distress. We wished to develop a new measure that assesses current levels of problematic worry across the spectrum of severity in the population. Importantly, we wanted a high level of clarity in item content and ease of use, basing the format on the successful Warwick Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale (Tennant et al., Reference Tennant, Hiller, Fishwick, Platt, Joseph, Weich, Parkinson, Secker and Stewart-Brown2007).

The most commonly used measure of worry, the Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ; Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Miller, Metzger and Borkovec1990), was developed 30 years ago. The questionnaire was derived from a principal component analysis of an item pool completed by 300 psychology students. It helped initiate the psychological study of worry and has been used with thousands of people in research studies and clinical trials, showing good test-retest reliability, internal reliability and convergent and discriminant validity. It is a trait measure of worry, comprising 16 items rated on a 1–5 scale with anchors at each end (‘Not at all typical of me’/‘Very typical of me’). Five items are reverse worded, a procedure that can be problematic due to participant inattention and confusion (Woods, Reference Woods2006; Van Sonderen et al., Reference Van Sonderen, Sanderman and Coyne2013). The reverse items on the PSWQ tend to produce a separate artefact factor (Brown, Reference Brown2003; Yilmaz et al., Reference Yilmaz, Gençöz and Wells2008). Our own experience is that the reverse items on the PSWQ can sometimes confuse people. The PSWQ items focus upon a tendency to worry (e.g. ‘I have been a worrier all my life’, ‘I am always worrying about something’, ‘I never worry about anything’). There are no items on the emotional impact of worry. Not every scale item is easily comprehendible (e.g. ‘If I do not have enough time to do everything, I do not worry about it’). The overall success of the scale has led to adaptations, including shorter versions and state versions (e.g. Yao et al., Reference Yao, Sripada, Klumpp, Abelson, Muzik, Zhao, Rosenblum, Briggs, Kaston and Warren2016).

Our objective was to produce – using a latent trait model approach with item response theory (IRT) (Reise and Henson, Reference Reise and Henson2003) – a new worry scale, straightforward to complete, that focuses on problematic worry. IRT examines the probabilistic relationship between varying levels of a latent trait and the ability of individual items to measure this trait. By aligning items and respondents on the same scale, IRT leads to the development of measurements with greater precision and parameters that are sample independent (Bortolotti et al., Reference Bortolotti, Tezza, de Andrade, Bornia and de Sousa2013). Our aim was to develop a general worry scale that would: assess worry over a defined time period (1 month); be brief; use a scale with anchors for every point; not include reverse items; include the tendency to worry but also levels of control and emotional impact; and would be suitable for use in clinical and non-clinical populations. The scale was designed to be neutral with regards to theoretical accounts of the causes of worry (Davey and Meeten, Reference Davey and Meeten2016). Our secondary objective was to develop a content-specific measure of worry focused upon paranoid concerns (unfounded fear of harm from others). Previously we have shown that: patients with persecutory delusions have levels of worry comparable to individuals with generalised anxiety disorder (GAD) (Freeman and Garety, Reference Freeman and Garety1999); worry predicts the occurrence and persistence of paranoia (Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Stahl, McManus, Meltzer, Brugha, Wiles and Bebbington2012); and treating worry in patients with persecutory delusions significantly lowers the delusions [The Worry Intervention Trial (WIT); Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Dunn, Startup, Pugh, Cordwell, Mander, Cernis, Wingham, Shirvell and Kingdon2015]. In the WIT, we independently assessed levels of general worry and paranoia. For the implementation of such worry treatments for patients with psychosis, it will be beneficial for clinicians to have a brief measure that combines both concepts. We also planned to produce clinical cut-off scores for the questionnaires to facilitate use as screening tools.

Method

Participants

To extract the items for the new measures of general worry and worry concerning paranoia, a derivation sample of 300 participants (250 from the general population and 50 patients with persecutory delusions) completed the full item pool (mean age = 42.8, s.d. = 18.6, female = 167, male = 133, White British = 90%). A second cross-validation sample consisting of 449 participants [273 from the general population, 79 patients with persecutory delusions and 93 patients with social anxiety disorder (SAD)] completed the final versions of both measures (mean age = 33.5, s.d. = 13.8, female = 192, male = 257, White British = 90%).

Participants from the general population were recruited via local radio adverts and the distribution of leaflets in Oxfordshire. Patients with persecutory delusions were participants from the WIT, a randomised controlled trial of a psychological intervention to reduce worry in adults with persecutory delusions in the context of non-affective psychosis (Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Dunn, Startup, Pugh, Cordwell, Mander, Cernis, Wingham, Shirvell and Kingdon2015). They had: a clinical diagnosis of non-affective psychosis (i.e. schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder or delusional disorder); a current and persistent persecutory delusion; and clinically significant levels of worry (44+ on the PSWQ; Startup and Erickson, Reference Startup, Erickson, Davey and Wells2006). Participants with SAD were referred for psychological treatment by their GP or IAPT services to either the London or Oxford Centre for Anxiety Disorders and Trauma. They met criteria for SAD according to the Anxiety and Related Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-IV (Brown et al., Reference Brown, DiNardo and Barlow1996) and SAD was their primary clinical problem.

Assessments

Item pools

An initial item pool of 40 general worry items was devised by the study team based upon consideration of our definition of worry, comments patients had made, clinical experience and existing questionnaires. We aimed to cover items concerning time spent worrying, control over worry and the impact of worry. An initial item pool of 16 paranoia worry items was created from patient comments during the WIT and the team's clinical experience. The time period for all the items was 1 month. Following the response format of the WEMSBS (Tennant et al., Reference Tennant, Hiller, Fishwick, Platt, Joseph, Weich, Parkinson, Secker and Stewart-Brown2007), items were rated on a 0–4 scale (None of the time, Rarely, Some of the time, Often, All of the time).

Penn State Worry Questionnaire

The PSWQ is the most established measure of trait worry and has been used in non-clinical and clinical populations (Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Miller, Metzger and Borkovec1990; Startup and Erickson, Reference Startup, Erickson, Davey and Wells2006). Each of the 16 items is rated on a five-point scale. Higher scores indicate a greater tendency to worry.

Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire

This is a 15-item questionnaire asking how a person typically thinks about negative problems (e.g. ‘The same thoughts keep going through my mind again and again’), with each item assessed on a 0–4 scale (Ehring et al., Reference Ehring, Zetsche, Weidacker, Wahl, Schönfeld and Ehlers2011). Higher scores indicate greater levels of repetitive negative thinking.

Green et al. Paranoid Thoughts Scale Part B

The Green et al. Paranoid Thoughts Scale (GPTS) Part B is a 16-item measure of persecutory thinking (e.g. ‘I was convinced there was a conspiracy against me’) (Green et al., Reference Green, Freeman, Kuipers, Bebbington, Fowler, Dunn and Garety2008). Items are rated on a 1–5 scale. Higher scores indicate greater levels of paranoid thinking.

Beck Anxiety Inventory

The Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) is a self-report 21-item assessment of anxiety. Items are rated on a 0–3 scale (Beck et al., Reference Beck, Epstein, Brown and Steer1988). Higher scores indicate higher levels of anxiety.

Depression Anxiety Stress Scales

The Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS Anxiety) subscale comprises 14 items rated on a 0–3 scale (Lovibond and Lovibond, Reference Lovibond and Lovibond1995). Higher scores indicate higher levels of anxiety.

Generalised Anxiety Disorder Questionnaire-IV

The Generalised Anxiety Disorder Questionnaire-IV (GAD-Q-IV) is a nine-item self-report diagnostic measure for GAD (Newman et al., Reference Newman, Zuellig, Kachin, Constantino, Przeworski, Erickson and Cashman-McGrath2002). A cut-off score of 7.67 is recommended to indicate probable GAD diagnostic status (Moore et al., Reference Moore, Anderson, Barnes, Haigh and Fresco2014). This was used to from a GAD subgroup (n = 50) from the non-clinical participants.

Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale

The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) is a 30-item interviewer-rated instrument developed for the assessment of patients with schizophrenia (Kay, Reference Kay1991). Current symptoms over the last week were rated. Higher scores indicate the greater presence of psychiatric symptoms. Only the general psychopathology scale was considered in this study, as a marker of changes in levels of affect.

Procedure

Participants from the general population completed questionnaires online using Qualtrics and participants from the clinical samples completed paper versions of the questionnaires. The derivation sample (n = 300) completed the full item pools for both measures. The final versions of both measures were then completed by the cross-validation sample (n = 449). To assess concurrent validity in the cross-validation sample, participants from the general population also completed the PSWQ, GAD-Q-IV, Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ), DASS and the GPTS, and the persecutory delusion group completed the PSWQ, BAI, GPTS, PTQ and PANSS. No additional measures were completed by the patients with SAD. To examine test-retest reliability, 75 participants from the cross-validation sample (50 from the general population and 25 with persecutory delusions) also repeated the new measures 1 week later. To assess sensitivity to clinical change, the worry scales were repeated by 43 participants (who had been in the cross-validation sample) with a persecutory delusion during the WIT (Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Dunn, Startup, Pugh, Cordwell, Mander, Cernis, Wingham, Shirvell and Kingdon2015).

Analysis

All analyses were conducted in R, version 3.5 (R Core Team, 2013). Rates of missing data were low (<3%). Participants with missing data on the item pools were excluded (derivation sample n = 8, cross-validation sample n = 4). Demonstrating the appropriateness of factor analysis in the derivation sample, Bartlett's test of Sphericity was significant (χ2 = 22 577, df = 1540, p < 0.001) and the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin test of sampling adequacy was excellent (KMO = 0.98). To assess the factor structure and separability of the items, an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) with the maximum likelihood estimator and oblique rotation was conducted using the ‘Psych’ package (Revelle, Reference Revelle2018). Parallel analysis and examination of the scree plot was used to identify the number of factors to extract from the 56 items. The EFA and subsequent IRT analyses were used to inform the selection of items for the final versions of both scales.

The sample sizes were sufficient for IRT analysis given previous recommendations that a minimum of 250 will provide stable estimates of the item parameters for questionnaire development (Orlando-Edelen and Reeve, Reference Orlando Edelen and Reeve2007). Given the polytomous response options, for each measure a two-parameter graded response model (Samejima, Reference Samejima1969) IRT analysis was conducted using the ‘mirt’ package (Chalmers, Reference Chalmers2012). Items with poor item fit (signed χ2 test of p < 0.01; Orlando and Thissen, Reference Orlando and Thissen2000) or residual correlations above +0.2 (Yen, Reference Yen1993) were excluded.

The IRT analysis produced discrimination and difficulty parameters for each item. The θ values represent the number of standard deviations from the average level (θ = 0) of the latent trait (i.e. general worry or paranoia worry), with higher values representing more severe presentations. Discrimination (a) parameters represent the capacity of each item to discriminate among participants at different levels of severity (i.e. θ). Higher discrimination values indicate the probability of endorsing the item increases rapidly as the level of severity increases. Discrimination values of at least 0.5 are considered acceptable, whilst values above 1 are highly discriminative (Baker and Kim, Reference Baker and Kim2017). For each item, the difficulty parameters (b) represent the 50% probability of responding at the threshold between each option (b 1 = 0–1, b 2 = 1–2, b 3 = 2–3 and b 4 = 3–4). Higher levels of difficulty represent items that measure the severe end of the spectrum.

To validate the psychometric properties of the selected items for both questionnaires, the IRT analyses were repeated in the cross-validation sample. The overall test information (TI) provides a measure of the internal reliability of the scale across the θ distribution. For interpretability, TI at specific values of θ was converted into an equivalent α reliability using the formula ${\rm 1/}\sqrt {{\rm TI}\lpar \theta \rpar } $ (O'Connor, Reference O'Connor2017). The concurrent validity of the two scales was examined by evaluating the pattern of correlations between each scale and additional measures relating to worry, anxiety and paranoia and differences in the total scores between the participant subgroups. For the group comparisons, participants from the general population were split into those scoring above (n = 50) and below the clinical threshold of 7.67 on the GAD-Q-IV.

To assess sensitivity to change in the 43 patients with psychosis from the cross-validation sample, the mean change in scores between the two time points was examined and effect sizes calculated using the formula (M pre-M post)/SD pre. Individual changes on the scales were assessed using the reliable change index (RCI; Jacobson and Trux, Reference Jacobson and Truax1991) in the 12 participants from this sub-group who completed the worry questionnaires directly before and after receiving a worry treatment (Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Dunn, Startup, Pugh, Cordwell, Mander, Cernis, Wingham, Shirvell and Kingdon2015). For the RCI, Cronbach's α was calculated for each measure from the cross-validation sample of patients with persecutory delusions (n = 79).

To examine the ability of each measure to accurately identify clinical levels of general worry and paranoia worry receiver operating characteristic (ROC) analyses were conducted using the ‘pROC’ (Robin et al., Reference Robin, Turck, Hainard, Tiberti, Lisacek, Sanchez and Müller2011) and ‘optimumCutpoints’ (Lopez-Raton et al., Reference Lopez-Raton, Xose Rodriguez-Alvarez, Cadarso Suarez and Gude Sampedro2014) packages. Patients in the WIT were an appropriate discrimination group given they had been identified based on reliably rated clinical levels of worry and desire for an intervention to reduce worry. To remove potential cases of clinical worry from the general population in the general worry analysis, participants from the non-clinical population who scored above cut-off on the GAD-Q-IV formed a GAD subgroup that were excluded from this analysis (n = 51). ROC curves were generated with the area under the curve (AUC) indicating the measure's discriminatory power, with values above 0.70 considered fair, over 0.80 good and over 0.90 excellent (Egan, Reference Egan1975). The optimal clinical cut-off threshold, calculated based on Youden's J statistic (Youden, Reference Youden1950), represents the optimal balance of sensitivity and specificity for the accurate discrimination of cases whilst reducing rates of false positives/negatives. The R code for the IRT and ROC analysis is available in the online Supplementary materials.

Results

Extracting the questionnaires

An initial EFA of all 56 items in the derivation sample identified two distinct factors for general worry and paranoia worry. To obtain a clean factor structure, one paranoia worry item that primarily loaded on the general worry factor and two items (one item from each factor) with cross-loadings over 0.3 were deleted. EFA of the remaining 53 items supported a two-factor structure which explained 73% of the variance. Although the two factors were highly correlated (r = 0.73), the distinct factor structure demonstrated that general worry (39 items) and paranoia worry (14 items) were separable. The two scales were therefore treated as unidimensional measures and IRT analyses were conducted on the general worry and paranoia worry items separately. The factor loadings and item parameters for the initial IRT analyses are shown in online Supplementary materials Table S1.

General worry items

Following the initial removal of items with correlated residuals (18 items), IRT analysis was conducted. All 21 remaining items were highly discriminative with parameters ranging from 1.62 to 4.88 (online Supplementary materials). Preference was given to items with a range of difficulty thresholds to ensure the questionnaire would represent a wide proportion of the distribution of worry. Ten final items were selected to represent the theoretically important aspects of worry: time spent worrying (two items), control over worry (two items), interference of worry (two items) and the emotional consequences of worry (four items).

Paranoia worry items

Following removal of items with correlated residuals (five items), IRT analysis was conducted. All nine remaining items were extremely discriminative with parameters ranging from 4.33 to 10.0 (online Supplementary material 1). All items demonstrated high difficulty parameters indicating that they tended to discriminate at more severe levels of paranoia worry. Five items were selected based on time spent worrying (two items), control over worry (one item), interference of worry (one item) and the emotional consequences of worry (one item). The final scales can be seen in the Appendix.

Cross-validation

All 10 general worry items and all five paranoia worry items selected from the derivation analysis had adequate item fit and residual correlations below 0.2 in the cross-validation sample. The IRT item parameters for both scales from this sample are shown in Table 1. Figure 1 shows the category response curves (CRCs) for each scale, depicting for each item the probability of every response option (0–4) along the distribution of θ. The discrimination parameter (a) is represented by the steepness of the curve, with higher values indicating a greater capacity to discriminate small differences in severity levels.

Fig. 1. Category response curves (CRCs) for the Dunn Worry Questionnaire and Paranoia Worries Questionnaire. The lines represent the probability (y axis) of responding to each Likert scale option (0–4) across the distribution of θ (x axis) for each item.

Table 1. IRT parameters for the final versions of the DWQ and PWQ with the cross-validation sample (n = 449)

Standard errors are shown in parentheses.

a = discrimination, b = difficulty parameters at the category thresholds between 0–1 (b 1), 1–2 (b 2), 2–3 (b 3) and 3–4 (b 4).

Dunn Worry Questionnaire

All 10 items of the Dunn Worry Questionnaire (DWQ) had high levels of discrimination (a = 1.98–5.03), indicating an increase in latent worry leads to a high probability that each item will be endorsed. The most discriminating item was ‘I have been worrying even though I didn't want to’ (a = 5.03), suggesting endorsement of this item was particularly representative of more severe worry. The least discriminating item was ‘Worry has stopped me sleeping’ (a = 1.98), although this was still well above the threshold of 1.0 for a highly discriminative item (Baker and Kim, Reference Baker and Kim2017). The CRCs in Fig. 1 show that all items of the DWQ discriminate well across a wide range of the worry distribution. Examination of the expected score across this distribution shows most people are likely to endorse a number of items on this scale; however, more severe worry is associated with higher levels of item endorsement.

The TI function in Fig. 2 represents the reliability of the test at different points of the θ spectrum. The DWQ had excellent reliability across the worry distribution, providing equivalent α values above 0.95 (TI>20) between 1.5 s.d. below and 1.7 s.d. above the average levels of trait worry (Fig. 2). Precision was high within this range with extremely small standard errors (0.16–0.22). The reliability only dropped below α = 0.85 (TI = 6.67) after the extremes of 2.0 s.d. below and 2.1 s.d. above the average trait worry where standard errors increased. The maximum information obtained was 41.1 (s.e. = 0.16) at a θ level of 0.2, equivalent to a reliability of α = 0.98. The DWQ had excellent test-retest reliability over 1 week with an intra-class correlation of 0.97 (95% CI 0.95–0.98, p < 0.001) between the two time points. These findings demonstrate that the DWQ has an excellent ability to discriminate worry with high reliability and precision for use in both non-clinical and clinical populations.

Fig. 2. Test information (TI) with standard errors (----) and expected score across the θ distribution for DWQ and PWQ.

Paranoia Worries Questionnaire

The item parameters for the Paranoia Worries Questionnaire (PWQ) show the five selected items are extremely discriminative (a = 4.10–10.7). The item with the strongest discriminative power was ‘Worries about someone trying to harm me have been really hard to control’ (a = 10.7), suggesting endorsement of this item was the most indicative of severe paranoia worry. As shown in Fig. 1, all five items tended to discriminate at higher levels of θ. Indeed, the steep expected score function in Fig. 2 shows people at the average level (θ = 0) have a low probability of scoring above zero, whereas item endorsement is strongly indicative of severe paranoia worry.

The TI function in Fig. 2 confirms that overall the PWQ provides an extremely high level of information, but primarily across the higher end of the paranoia worry distribution. The PWQ had excellent reliability with equivalent α values higher than 0.95 within 0.29–2.05 s.d.s above the average levels of paranoia worry. Precision was also high in this range with extremely small standard errors (0.11–0.22). The maximum information obtained was 82.8 (s.e. = 0.11) at a θ level of 0.78, equivalent to a reliability of α = 0.99. Conversely, the PWQ items discriminate less well at the lower end of the spectrum, with little information obtained at θ values below zero. The reliability of the PWQ starts to drop below α = 0.85 after −0.01 s.d.s below and 2.27 s.d.s above the average levels of paranoia worry where standard errors rapidly increase (Fig. 2). Test-retest reliability over 1 week was excellent with an intra-class correlation of 0.96 (95% CI 0.95–0.98, p < 0.001). These findings suggest the PWQ is highly discriminative of severe levels of paranoia worry for use in clinical populations.

Concurrent validity

The concurrent validity of the scales was demonstrated by the correlations with other measures of worry, anxiety and paranoia (Table 2). For both the non-clinical and persecutory delusion samples, the DWQ demonstrated a distinct pattern of strong correlations with related measures of worry (i.e. PSWQ, perseverative thinking, generalised anxiety and anxiety symptoms) and a moderate association with paranoia. Conversely, the PWQ was strongly related to paranoia and moderately correlated with measures of worry and anxiety.

Table 2. Bivariate correlations between the DWQ and PWQ with other measures in the cross-validation samples from the general population (n = 273) and patients with persecutory delusions (n = 79)

Further supporting the construct validity of each scale, there was a significant group effect on the DWQ [F (3,441) = 97, p < 0.001] and PWQ [F (3,441) = 255.5, p < 0.001] scores for the four groups (non-clinical, non-clinical GAD, psychosis, SAD) (see Table 3). As expected, the highest scores on the DWQ were observed in the GAD and persecutory delusion groups (mean difference = 1.4, p = 0.78). Participants in these two groups scored significantly higher (p < 0.001) than the remaining participants from the general population and patients with SAD. As would be expected, patients with SAD also scored significantly higher than those in the general population (p < 0.001). In line with the PWQ as a clinical measure of paranoid worry, patients with persecutory delusions scored significantly higher than all other subgroups (p < 0.001). Participants in the high GAD subgroup scored significantly higher (p < 0.001) than patients with SAD and general population controls, both of which had similarly low scores (p = 0.74).

Table 3. Mean total scores on the DWQ, PWQ, PSWQ and GPTS across all participant subgroups

Standard deviations in parentheses.

Sensitivity to change

Forty-three patients with persecutory delusions (mean age = 41.0, s.d. = 10.6, female = 28%, male = 72%) repeated the questionnaires either 8 (n = 27), 16 (n = 14) or 24 weeks (n = 2) later. Across this whole group (which comprised both randomisation arms of the trial), changes in the DWQ were moderately correlated with changes in the PSWQ (r = 0.50, p < 0.001) and the general psychopathology subscale of the PANSS (r = 0.34, p = 0.028), although overall there was no significant change in worry on either the DWQ (ES = 0.31, p > 0.05) or the PSWQ (ES = 0.07, p > 0.05). Twelve participants completed the assessments directly before and after the worry treatment. In these participants, reductions in worry were observed for the DWQ (ES = 1.2, v = 54, p = 0.03) and the PSWQ (ES = 0.9, v = 74.5, p = 0.003). Using the RCI, 8/12 participants showed a reliable reduction in DWQ worry scores following the intervention (online Supplementary materials). Notably, four of these patients did not show corresponding reliable changes on the PSWQ (only five participants showed a significant reduction on this scale). Only one participant with a significant RCI on the PSWQ did not show a significant change on the DWQ. These findings indicate the DWQ might have greater sensitivity to reductions in worry compared with the PSWQ.

Changes on the PWQ were strongly correlated with changes in GPTS paranoia (r = 0.61, p < 0.001) and DWQ worry scores (r = 0.74, p < 0.001), although there were no significant changes between the two time points in PWQ paranoid worry (ES = 0.29, p > 0.05) or GPTS paranoid thoughts (ES = 0.25, p > 0.05) scores. Notably, the correlation between change in paranoid worry and worry measured by the PSWQ (r = 0.42, p = 0.005) was not as strong as the correlation with the DWQ. In the subgroup of 12 participants who received the worry intervention between the measurements, large changes were observed in both paranoid worry (ES = 1.45, v = 66, p = 0.002) and paranoid thoughts (ES = 1.6, v = 63, p = 0.004). Following the intervention, the PWQ was able to detect reliable reductions in paranoid worry in 9/12 patients, which corresponded with reliable improvements in paranoia as measured by the GPTS.

Clinical cut-off scores

The ROC curves for both questionnaires and the sensitivity and specificity at different thresholds are shown in the online Supplementary materials.

Dunn Worry Questionnaire

The ROC analysis for the DWQ provided an AUC of 0.90 (95% CI 0.86–0.95), demonstrating an excellent level of discriminatory power. This indicates that a person with clinically identified levels of worry is 90% more likely to have a higher score on the DWQ than someone in the general population. This analysis identified the closest threshold to the optimal cut-off point was a score of 21 or above, providing a sensitivity of 0.88 (95% CI 0.80–0.95) and specificity of 0.83 (95% CI 0.78–0.87).

Paranoia Worries Questionnaire

The PWQ had an excellent ability to discriminate non-clinical and clinical levels of paranoia worry with an AUC of 0.95 (95% CI 0.90–0.98). This indicates a person with a clinically diagnosed persecutory delusion is 95% more likely to have a higher PWQ score than someone in the general population. The ROC analysis identified the closest threshold to the optimal cut-off point was a score of 5 or above, providing a sensitivity of 0.91 (95% CI 0.84–0.96) and specificity of 0.89 (95% CI 0.85–0.92). This same threshold was identified when the analysis was repeated with the SAD group as controls (AUC = 0.96, 95% CI 0.93–0.99), where a score of 5 provided a sensitivity of 0.91 (95% CI 0.84–0.97) and a specificity of 0.91 (95% CI 0.84–0.96). This demonstrates that even within clinical populations, a PWQ score of 5 or above is highly indicative of severe paranoia worry in the context of persecutory delusions.

Discussion

It is increasingly recognised that mental health conditions arise from multiple interacting factors that cross diagnostic boundaries. Worry is a plausible contributory factor to many mental health conditions, as shown in a recent analysis of epidemiological survey data using a dynamic Bayesian network approach (Kuipers et al., Reference Kuipers, Moffa, Kuipers, Freeman and Bebbington2019). Our clinical and research experience is that the assessment of worry can be improved. Therefore, we developed a new scale of general problematic worry, combining classical test theory with latent trait models, that has a clear time period, is straightforward to complete and includes the impact of the thinking style. The IRT analysis shows that the DWQ reliably assesses the range of worry severity across the non-clinical and clinical population and can discriminate between different levels of this spectrum. Internal reliability and test-retest reliability were extremely high. Sensitivity to change was established and convergent validity was shown with existing assessments of worry, perseverative negative thinking and GADs. As would be expected, individuals from the general population meeting cut-offs for GAD and patients with persecutory delusions scored more highly on the DWQ than patients with SAD, who had higher scores than non-clinical controls. A psychometrically strong, comprehensible measure of problematic worry has been produced.

In a clear illustration of the trans-diagnostic importance of worry, we have shown in the WIT that treating worry in patients with psychosis leads to a reduction in persecutory delusions (Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Dunn, Startup, Pugh, Cordwell, Mander, Cernis, Wingham, Shirvell and Kingdon2015). The best treatment approaches regularly monitor the key outcome. We therefore also developed a brief measure of problematic worry focused on paranoid content, the PWQ. In contrast to the DWQ, reliable across non-clinical and clinical levels of worry, the PWQ is most reliable for those at the clinical end of the spectrum. The items are primarily discriminative of severe levels of paranoia worry, which makes it ideal for the intended use in treatment with patients with psychosis. To score on the measure requires both paranoid fears and worry. The scale has extremely high internal reliability at severe levels of paranoia worry and excellent test-retest reliability. It is associated with scores on assessments for paranoia in particular but also negative repetitive thinking. In the context of the treatment trial, the PWQ showed sensitivity to clinical change.

There are limitations in the development of the questionnaires. The questionnaires were not tested with patients with GAD, which is considered the archetypal disorder of worry. It would be valuable for the questionnaires to be tested with this patient group. However, individuals with persecutory delusions, who were tested in this study, typically have levels of worry comparable to patients with GAD (Freeman and Garety, Reference Freeman and Garety1999). Both the non-clinical individuals screening positive for GAD and the patients with persecutory delusions scored highly on the new general worry measures. We think it highly unlikely that a different pattern would be found for patients with GAD. The development of the assessments would have benefited from greater input from patients. Patients only gave feedback on the ease of completion of the initial item pool and subsequent scales. In the years since the development of the measures we have developed much more rigorous patient involvement procedures. Further, the participant groups, clinical and non-clinical, were unlikely to have been fully representative of the populations from which they were drawn. The true potential of the questionnaires will only be known with use.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719000588.

Author ORCIDs

Daniel Freeman, 0000-0002-2541-2197.

Acknowledgements

Graham Dunn passed away in January 2019. Graham was a wonderful methodologist, statistician, mentor and friend – he will be missed enormously.

Financial support

This study was supported by the WIT project (09/160/06) which was funded by a grant from the Efficacy and Mechanism Evaluation (EME) Programme, and is funded by the UK Medical Research Council (MRC) and managed by the UK NHS National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) on behalf of the MRC-NIHR partnership. DF is supported by a NIHR Research Professorship. AE and DMC are funded by the Wellcome Trust (200796) and NIHR Senior Investigator Awards.

Conflict of interest

None.

Appendix

1. The Dunn Worry Questionnaire

Please circle the numbers that best describe your experience in the past month.

2. The Paranoia Worries Questionnaire

The following items concern worries you may have about others trying to upset or harm you.

Please circle the numbers that best describe your experience in the past month.

References

Baker, FB and Kim, SH (2017) The Basics of Item Response Theory Using R. Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, AT, Epstein, N, Brown, G and Steer, R (1988) An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 56, 893897.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borkovec, TD and Inz, J (1990) The nature of worry in generalized anxiety disorder: a predominance of thought activity. Behaviour Research and Therapy 28, 153158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bortolotti, S, Tezza, R, de Andrade, D, Bornia, A and de Sousa, A (2013) Relevance and advantages of using the item response theory. Quality and Quantity 47, 23412360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, T (2003) Confirmatory factor analysis of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire: multiple factors or method effects? Behaviour Research and Therapy 41, 14111426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, TA, DiNardo, P and Barlow, DH (1996) Anxiety and Related Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-IV. Albany, NY: Graywind Publications.Google Scholar
Chalmers, RP (2012) Mirt: a multidimensional item response theory package for the r environment. Journal of Statistical Software 48, 129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davey, GC and Meeten, F (2016) The perseverative worry bout: a review of cognitive, affective and motivational factors that contribute to worry perseveration. Biological Psychology 121, 233243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egan, JP (1975) Signal Detection Theory and ROC Analysis. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ehring, T, Zetsche, U, Weidacker, K, Wahl, K, Schönfeld, S and Ehlers, A (2011) The Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ): validation of a content-independent measure of repetitive negative thinking. Journal of Behaviour Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 42, 225232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D (2016) Persecutory delusions: a cognitive perspective on understanding and treatment. The Lancet Psychiatry 3, 685692.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D and Garety, PA (1999) Worry, worry processes and dimensions of delusions. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy 27, 4762.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, D, Stahl, D, McManus, S, Meltzer, H, Brugha, T, Wiles, N and Bebbington, P (2012) Insomnia, worry, anxiety and depression as predictors of the occurrence and persistence of paranoid thinking. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 47, 11951203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D, Dunn, G, Startup, H, Pugh, K, Cordwell, J, Mander, H, Cernis, E, Wingham, G, Shirvell, K and Kingdon, D (2015) Effects of cognitive behaviour therapy for worry on persecutory delusions in patients with psychosis (WIT): a parallel, single-blind, randomised controlled trial with a mediation analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry 2, 305313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, C, Freeman, D, Kuipers, E, Bebbington, P, Fowler, D, Dunn, G, Garety, PA (2008) Measuring ideas of persecution and reference: the Green et al. Paranoid Thought Scales (G-PTS). Psychological Medicine 38, 101111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobson, N and Truax, P (1991) Clinical significance: a statistical approach to defining meaningful change in psychotherapy research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 59, 1219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kay, SR (1991) Positive and Negative Syndromes in Schizophrenia. New York: Brunner.Google Scholar
Kuipers, J, Moffa, G, Kuipers, E, Freeman, D and Bebbington, P (2019) Links between psychotic and neurotic symptoms in the general population: an analysis of longitudinal British National Survey data using Directed Acyclic Graphs. Psychological Medicine 49, 388395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lopez-Raton, M, Xose Rodriguez-Alvarez, M, Cadarso Suarez, C and Gude Sampedro, F (2014) Optimalcutpoints: an R package for selecting optimal cutpoints in diagnostic tests. Journal of Statistical Software 61, 136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lovibond, P and Lovibond, SH (1995) The structure of negative emotional states. Behaviour Research and Therapy 33, 335343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, TJ, Miller, ML, Metzger, RL and Borkovec, TD (1990) Development and validation of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy 28, 487495.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, M, Anderson, N, Barnes, J, Haigh, E and Fresco, D (2014) Using the GAD-Q-IV to identify generalized anxiety disorder in psychiatric treatment seeking and primary care medical samples. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 28, 2530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newman, M, Zuellig, A, Kachin, K, Constantino, M, Przeworski, A, Erickson, T and Cashman-McGrath, L (2002) Preliminary reliability and validity of the Generalized Anxiety Disorder Questionnaire–IV: a revised self-report diagnostic measure of generalized anxiety disorder. Behavior Therapy 33, 215233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Connor, BP (2017) An illustration of the effects of fluctuations in test information on measurement error, the attenuation of effect sizes, and diagnostic reliability. Psychological Assessment 30, 9911003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orlando Edelen, M and Reeve, BB (2007) Applying item response theory (IRT) modeling to questionnaire development, evaluation, and refinement. Quality of Life Research 16, 518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orlando, M and Thissen, D (2000) Likelihood-based item fit indices for dichotomous item response theory models. Applied Psychological Measurement 24, 5064.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team (2013) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available at http://www.R-project.org/2013.Google Scholar
Reise, S and Henson, J (2003) A discussion of modern versus traditional psychometrics as applied to personality assessment scales. Journal of Personality Assessment 81, 93103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Revelle, W (2018) Psych: Procedures for Personality and Psychological Research. Illinois, USA: Northwestern University. Available at https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psychVersion=1.8.4.Google Scholar
Robin, X, Turck, N, Hainard, A, Tiberti, N, Lisacek, F, Sanchez, JC and Müller, M (2011) pROC: an open-source package for R and S+ to analyze and compare ROC curves. BMC Bioinformatics 12, 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Samejima, F (1969) Estimation of latent ability using a response pattern of graded scores. Psychometrika 34(Suppl 1), 197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Startup, HM and Erickson, TM (2006) The Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ). In Davey, GCL and Wells, A (eds), Worry and its Psychological Disorders. Chichester: Wiley, pp. 101120.Google Scholar
Sternheim, L, Startup, H, Saeidi, S, Morgan, J, Hugo, P, Russell, A and Schmidt, U (2012) Understanding catastrophic worry in eating disorders. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 43, 10951103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tennant, R, Hiller, L, Fishwick, R, Platt, S, Joseph, S, Weich, S, Parkinson, J, Secker, S and Stewart-Brown, S (2007) The Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale (WEMWBS): development and UK validation. Health & Quality of Life Outcomes 5, 63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Sonderen, E, Sanderman, R and Coyne, J (2013) Ineffectiveness of reverse wording of questionnaire items. PLoS ONE 8, e68967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watkins, E (2008) Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin 134, 163206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woods, CM (2006) Careless responding to reverse-worded items: implications for confirmatory factor analysis. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment 28, 189194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yao, B, Sripada, R, Klumpp, H, Abelson, J, Muzik, M, Zhao, Z, Rosenblum, K, Briggs, H, Kaston, M and Warren, R (2016) Penn State Worry Questionnaire – 10: a new tool for measurement-based care. Psychiatry Research 239, 6267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yen, WM (1993) Scaling performance assessments: strategies for managing local item dependence. Journal of Educational Measurement 30, 187213.Google Scholar
Yilmaz, A, Gençöz, T and Wells, A (2008) Psychometric characteristics of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire and Metacognitions Questionnaire-30 and metacognitive predictors of worry and obsessive–compulsive symptoms in a Turkish sample. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy 15, 424439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Youden, WJ (1950) Index for rating diagnostic tests. Cancer 3, 3235.3.0.CO;2-3>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Category response curves (CRCs) for the Dunn Worry Questionnaire and Paranoia Worries Questionnaire. The lines represent the probability (y axis) of responding to each Likert scale option (0–4) across the distribution of θ (x axis) for each item.

Figure 1

Table 1. IRT parameters for the final versions of the DWQ and PWQ with the cross-validation sample (n = 449)

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Test information (TI) with standard errors (----) and expected score across the θ distribution for DWQ and PWQ.

Figure 3

Table 2. Bivariate correlations between the DWQ and PWQ with other measures in the cross-validation samples from the general population (n = 273) and patients with persecutory delusions (n = 79)

Figure 4

Table 3. Mean total scores on the DWQ, PWQ, PSWQ and GPTS across all participant subgroups

Supplementary material: File

Freeman et al. supplementary material

Freeman et al. supplementary material 1

Download Freeman et al. supplementary material(File)
File 70 KB