Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T06:58:32.726Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

‘It's not over when it's over’: persistent neurobiological abnormalities in recovered depressed patients

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 August 2007

Z. Bhagwagar
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Yale University, Newhaven, USA
P.J. Cowen*
Affiliation:
University Department of Psychiatry, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, UK
*
*Address for correspondence: Professor Philip J. Cowen, Neurosciences Building, Warneford Hospital, Oxford OX3 7JX, UK. (Email: phil.cowen@psych.ox.ac.uk)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The tendency of major depression to recur is a leading problem in clinical management and is responsible for much of the illness burden. Until recently, biological studies of depression have focused on the mechanisms involved in acute illness but there are now many data to suggest that neurobiological abnormalities persist when depressed patients are clinically recovered and withdrawn from medication. These abnormalities encompass a number of neurochemical and neuropsychological mechanisms that could be relevant to recurrence, including changes in the availability of serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine, 5-HT) receptor subtypes, decreases in cortical γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA), increases in cortisol secretion and negative biases in the processing of emotional information. Studies of groups at high risk of depression before illness onset will help to clarify which biological abnormalities precede the development of depression and which are the product of recurrent illness. Ultimately this work should lead to a better understanding of the neurobiology of vulnerability to depression and more innovative approaches to primary and secondary prevention.

Type
Editorial Review
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2007

Introduction

One of the more remarkable features of clinical depression is its tendency to remit, either spontaneously or during the course of treatment. For William Styron, the ‘men and women who have recovered from the disease … bear witness to what is probably its only saving grace: it is conquerable’ (Styron, Reference Styron2004). However, the tendency to recovery is accompanied by an equally great tendency to recurrence; for example, at least 80% of recovered depressed patients will experience further major depression, and over a 25-year follow-up, patients will suffer, on average, about five further episodes (Mueller et al. Reference Mueller, Leon, Keller, Solomon, Endicott, Corywell, Warshaw and Maser1999; Angst, Reference Angst, Gelder, Lopez-Ibor and Andreasen2000). The long-term outcome of those hospitalized for depression is particularly poor (Lee & Murray, Reference Lee and Murray1988).

These epidemiological data suggest that even when depressed patients are clinically recovered, they possess risk factors that place them at increased liability of future episodes. Several clinical predictors of depressive recurrence have been identified, including the number of previous episodes, age of illness onset and persisting subsyndromal symptomatology (Angst, Reference Angst, Gelder, Lopez-Ibor and Andreasen2000). It also appears that with increasing number of depressive episodes, the role of life events in triggering illness in individuals decreases; this has been suggested to represent a kind of ‘kindling’ of susceptibility (Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Thorton and Gardner2001). The neurobiological basis of vulnerability to recurrent depression has been less studied but there is growing evidence that recovered depressed patients manifest several abnormalities in mood-related biological mechanisms that could be relevant to increased risk of relapse.

Traditionally, biological studies of conditions such as depression have focused on abnormalities associated with the acute episode. A difficulty with this approach is that any abnormalities detected could represent epiphenomena of the abnormal clinical state or its treatment rather than mechanisms relevant to pathophysiology. There is therefore a good argument for developing a complementary strategy in which biological studies are carried out in people with a history of recurrent depression who are currently recovered and medication free. Neurobiological abnormalities identified in these circumstances could be markers of trait vulnerability to recurrent illness and arguably valuable as illness endophenotypes (Hasler et al. Reference Hasler, Drevets, Manji and Charney2004; Flint & Munafò, Reference Flint and Munafò2007). Whatever the merits of the latter proposition, a better understanding of trait neurobiological abnormalities might lead to improved ways of identifying those at high risk of recurrence and to novel methods of prevention. In this review we outline current developments in this area, focusing specifically on studies where recovered patients have been free of medication. We concentrate on unmedicated recovered subjects because, not surprisingly, antidepressant drugs can influence many of the neurobiological mechanisms relevant to vulnerability. It should also be noted that in most cases it is unclear whether neurobiological abnormalities might have been present before the development of illness or might instead be a consequence of the illness and its treatment (the so-called ‘scar’ effect).

Monoamine neurotransmission

Serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine, 5-HT)

Acutely depressed patients show changes in many aspects of 5-HT neurotransmission (Cowen, Reference Cowen, Griez, Faravelli, Nutt and Zohar2005). The efficacy of 5-HT-potentiating agents such as selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitors (SSRIs) in the treatment of depression suggests that low 5-HT activity might be associated with the state of depression itself; however, changes in 5-HT mechanisms are also present in recovered unmedicated depressed patients. For example, both acutely depressed (Drevets et al. Reference Drevets, Frank, Price, Kupfer, Holt, Greer, Huang, Gautier and Mathis1999; Sargent et al. Reference Sargent, Kjaer, Bench, Rabiner, Messa, Meyer, Gunn, Grasby and Cowen2000) and recovered depressed (Bhagwagar et al. Reference Bhagwagar, Rabiner, Sargent, Grasby and Cowen2004a) patients demonstrate rather generalized decreases in brain 5-HT1A receptor availability measured by positron emission tomography (PET).

This finding is of interest because the phenotype of the 5-HT1A receptor knockout mouse is characterized by increased anxiety (Ramboz et al. Reference Ramboz, Oosting, Amara, Kung, Blier, Mendelsohn, Mann and Brunner1998). Perhaps, therefore, low 5-HT1A receptor availability in depressed patients could be a developmental phenomenon exposing affected individuals to an increased risk of emotional disorders. Indeed, a recent multimodal imaging study by Fisher et al. (Reference Fisher, Meltzer, Ziolko, Price and Harari2006) in healthy subjects demonstrated that increased reactivity of the amygdala to negative facial expressions correlated with lower 5-HT1A receptor binding in the raphe nuclei. Thus, low 5-HT1A receptor availability could predispose to emotional disorders by facilitating increased processing of negative emotional stimuli in limbic regions (Vuillemier, Reference Vuillemier2005).

We have also found that recovered depressed patients have increased cortical 5-HT2A receptor binding measured by PET in conjunction with [11C]MDL 100907 (Bhagwagar et al. Reference Bhagwagar, Hinz, Taylor, Fancy, Cowen and Grasby2006). Because the status of 5-HT2A receptor binding in acute depression is controversial (Meyer et al. Reference Meyer, McMain, Kennedy, Korman, Brown, DaSilva and Wilson2003; Mintun et al. Reference Mintun, Sheline, Moerlein, Vlassenko, Huang and Snyder2004), this finding requires replication. However, 5-HT1A and 5-HT2A receptors are often co-localized on cortical neurones, where they appear to have opposing physiological roles (Piquet & Galvan, Reference Piquet and Galvan1994; Martin-Ruiz et al. Reference Martin-Ruiz, Puig, Celada, Shapiro, Roth, Mengod and Artigas2001). Thus, our data would be consistent with a persistent dysregulation of the ascending 5-HT input to cortex and limbic regions, presumably including the circuitry involved in the representation and regulation of emotion.

One of the most striking 5-HT-linked abnormalities in unmedicated recovered depressed patients is the liability to re-experience depressive symptomatology when undergoing acute tryptophan depletion (ATD), a dietary manipulation that decreases brain 5-HT function by limiting the availability of tryptophan for brain 5-HT synthesis (Smith et al. Reference Smith, Fairburn and Cowen1997; Ruhé et al. Reference Ruhé, Mason and Schene2007). This effect is not seen in people who have no history of depression and no personal risk factors (Ruhé et al. Reference Ruhé, Mason and Schene2007). In addition, while ATD may produce some subjective lowering of mood on visual analogue scales in people with a strong family history of depression, this does not reach the level of clinical symptomatology (Ruhé et al. Reference Ruhé, Mason and Schene2007). This indicates that depressive reactions to ATD are probably associated with a personal history of major depression. As a result of ATD, the brain apparently acquires an organizational state in which negative emotions and their associations are much more readily accessed.

The unusual psychological effects of ATD in recovered depressed patients could be attributable to underlying deficits in 5-HT neurotransmission, such that the effect of ATD to impair 5-HT function in recovered depressives is greater than in non-depressed controls. This would imply that a sufficient lowering of 5-HT neurotransmission would cause depression even in non-vulnerable individuals. Perhaps a more likely explanation might be the presence of persistent abnormalities in the cortical and limbic neural circuitry with which 5-HT pathways interact. PET imaging studies suggest that ATD-induced depressive relapse is associated with altered activity in orbitofrontal cortex, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex and thalamus (Smith et al. Reference Smith, Morris, Friston, Cowen and Dolan1999; Neumeister et al. Reference Neumeister, Nugent, Waldeck, Geraci, Schwarz, Bonne, Bain, Luckenbaugh, Herscovitch, Charney and Drevets2004); the integrated activity of these brain regions is known to underpin the processing of emotional information (Phillips et al. Reference Phillips, Drevets, Rauch and Lane2003). It is possible that underlying deficits in this circuitry in recovered depressed patients are ‘unmasked’ by ATD. The fact that the effects of ATD appear to be present only in people who have experienced depression suggests that depression itself can cause neurobiological changes that increase psychological vulnerability to low 5-HT states.

Could the striking effects of ATD in recovered depressed patients be a consequence of previous antidepressant treatment, particularly with SSRIs? It is difficult to exclude this possibility completely, but in our own ATD study of recovered depressed patients we also saw symptomatic relapse in patients who had not been treated with antidepressants (Smith et al. Reference Smith, Fairburn and Cowen1997), suggesting that this is not the explanation.

Catecholamines

Treatment of recovered depressed patients with the catecholamine synthesis inhibitor α-methyl-para-tyrosine (AMPT) also produces acute depressive relapse (Berman et al. Reference Berman, Narasimhan, Miller, Anand, Cappiello, Oren, Heninger and Charney1999). However, dietary depletion of tyrosine, the amino acid precursor of noradrenaline and dopamine, does not (McTavish et al. Reference McTavish, Mannie, Harmer and Cowen2005). While AMPT diminishes both noradrenaline and dopamine synthesis, the effects of tyrosine depletion appear limited to dopamine activity, perhaps because of the greater utilization of tyrosine by dopaminergic neurones (McTavish et al. Reference McTavish, Cowen and Sharp1996). This suggests that the effect of AMPT to produce depressive relapse is caused by lowered noradrenaline activity. Importantly, a PET study of AMPT in recovered depressed patients implicated similar brain circuitry to that involved in ATD-induced depressive relapse, that is, orbitofrontal cortex, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and thalamus (Bremner et al. Reference Bremner, Vythilingam, Ng, Vermetten, Nazeer, Oren, Berman and Charney2003). Thus, if this circuitry is indeed potentially dysfunctional in recovered depressed patients, it appears to be susceptible to the effects of impaired noradrenaline neurotransmission as well as lowered 5-HT function.

Gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA)

As noted earlier, ascending 5-HT pathways make synaptic connection with both GABA interneurones and glutamatergic pyramidal neurones in both cortical and limbic regions (Taylor et al. Reference Taylor, Bhagwagar, Cowen and Sharp2003). Studies using proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) have shown lowered GABA levels in occipital cortex in acutely depressed patients, particularly those with a melancholic syndrome (Sanacora et al. Reference Sanacora, Gueorguiva, Epperson, Wu, Appel and Rothman2004). Both SSRI treatment (Sanacora et al. Reference Sanacora, Mason, Rothman and Krystal2002) and electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) (Sanacora et al. Reference Sanacora, Mason, Rothman, Hyder, Ciarcia and Ostroff2003) increase GABA levels in depressed patients; however, this effect is not seen with cognitive behaviour therapy despite clinical improvement (Sanacora et al. Reference Sanacora, Fenton, Fasula, Rothman, Levin, Krystal and Mason2006).

The latter finding suggests that the effect of SSRIs and ECT to increase GABA levels in depressed patients might be an effect of treatment rather than a consequence of clinical recovery. Consistent with this we found lowered GABA levels in occipital cortex in recovered, unmedicated depressed patients (Bhagwagar et al. Reference Bhagwagar, Wyleszinska, Jezzard, Evans, Ashworth, Sule, Matthews and Cowen2007), which suggests that diminished GABA availability might be a trait maker of vulnerability to depression. It is unclear whether this abnormality is a consequence of recurrent depression or might also be present in high-risk groups; however, the non-invasive nature of MRS should make it feasible to study those at high risk of depression before the onset of clinical illness. It is possible that changes in GABA levels might be caused by the glial cell deficits that have been reported in neuropathological studies of patients with recurrent mood disorders (Harrison, Reference Harrison2002; Sanacora et al. Reference Sanacora, Gueorguiva, Epperson, Wu, Appel and Rothman2004).

Cortisol hypersecretion and sleep

Increased secretion of cortisol is a common accompaniment of major depression and is usually regarded as a state marker of illness (Holsboer, Reference Holsboer2000). However, some aspects of hypothalamo–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis function may remain abnormal in remitted patients, particularly those at high risk of recurrence (Zobel et al. Reference Zobel, Nickel, Sonntag, Uhr, Holsboer and Sing2001). We have used the waking increase in salivary cortisol as a measure of HPA axis activity (Pruessner et al. Reference Pruessner, Wolf, Hellhammer, Buske-Kirschbaum, von-Auer, Jobst and Kirschbaum1997). Our studies have shown that waking salivary cortisol is increased both in acute depression (Bhagwagar et al. Reference Bhagwagar, Hafizi and Cowen2005) and in recovered depressed patients (Bhagwagar et al. Reference Bhagwagar, Hafizi and Cowen2003). Furthermore, we have found a similar increase in waking salivary cortisol in young people who have not been depressed themselves but who have a depressed parent (Mannie et al. Reference Mannie, Harmer and Cowen2007). The increase in cortisol secretion in the latter group could not be explained by symptomatic status, childhood adversity or recent life events.

Taken together, the waking salivary cortisol data show that recovered depressed patients continue to demonstrate abnormal HPA axis activity. Moreover, judging from the increase in cortisol secretion in children of depressed parents, this particular HPA axis abnormality probably precedes the onset of clinical illness. Persistently increased cortisol secretion could represent a risk factor for subsequent depression; for example, a recent study reported that increased cortisol levels at age 13 independently predicted depressive symptomatology 3 years later (Halligan et al. Reference Halligan, Herbert, Goodyer and Murray2007). These findings support studies from other high-risk groups of adolescents and adults showing that elevated cortisol secretion is a risk factor for subsequent major depression (Goodyer et al. Reference Goodyer, Herbert, Tamplin and Altham2000; Harris et al. Reference Harris, Borsanyi, Messari, Stanford, Cleary, Shiers, Brown and Herbert2000).

Increased cortisol secretion is often suggested to be a risk factor for neuronal atrophy and has been linked to decreased hippocampal volume in patients with recurrent depression (Campbell & MacQueen, Reference Campbell and MacQueen2004). Although the role of cortisol in this effect is not yet clearly established, there does seem to be an association between recurrent depression and hippocampal atrophy (Campbell & MacQueen, Reference Campbell and MacQueen2006). This could be another example of the way in which depression itself may result in neurobiological changes that complicate the course of the illness. However, it has not yet been demonstrated clearly that hippocampal volume is decreased in fully recovered depressed patients (Campbell & MacQueen, Reference Campbell and MacQueen2006). It is, however, certainly possible that persistently increased cortisol secretion could underlie some of the medical complications associated with chronic depression, including cardiovascular disease and obesity (Sherwood et al. Reference Sherwood, Varghese and McEwen2004).

There is also a considerable body of work on the HPA axis in depression using the dexamethasone–corticotrophin releasing hormone (DEX/CRH) test. Acutely depressed patients show increased cortisol release in the DEX/CRH test, which is attenuated following antidepressant treatment and clinical improvement (see Holsboer, Reference Holsboer2000). However, remitted patients who continue to demonstrate increased cortisol release are at higher risk of subsequent relapse (Zobel et al. Reference Zobel, Nickel, Sonntag, Uhr, Holsboer and Sing2001). This raises the possibility that abnormal responses to the DEX/CRH test might represent a marker of vulnerability to depression. The ‘Munich vulnerability study in affective disorders’ involves a longitudinal investigation of individuals at high familial risk of depression and initially it was reported that cortisol responses to DEX/CRH were abnormally increased in this group (Holsboer et al. Reference Holsboer, Lauer, Schreiber and Krieg1995); however, current findings indicate that the DEX/CRH test is not abnormal in people who develop depression prior to the onset of illness (Ising et al. Reference Ising, Lauer, Holsboer and Modell2005). These latter observations are difficult to reconcile at present.

The Munich study has also investigated changes in the sleep polysomnogram (electroencephalogram, EEG) in patients at risk of depression. Patients with acute depression have fairly reliable abnormalities in rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, including shortened latency to REM sleep and increased density of REM sleep (increased frequency of eye movements during REM sleep periods) (see Hasler et al. Reference Hasler, Drevets, Manji and Charney2004). Previous studies have suggested that recovered depressed patients continue to manifest a short latency to REM sleep and that this abnormality co-segregates in first-degree relatives (Giles et al. Reference Giles, Jarrett, Rush and Roffwarg1993, Reference Giles, Kupfer, Rush and Roffwarg1998). The Munich study did not find abnormal REM latency in people at increased familial risk of depression, but both high-risk individuals and those who eventually developed depression had increased REM density (Modell et al. Reference Modell, Ising, Holsboer and Lauer2005). Thus, the regulation of REM sleep remains a possible endophenotypic marker of vulnerability to depression (Hasler et al. Reference Hasler, Drevets, Manji and Charney2004)

Discussion

Recovered depressed patients who are off medication and essentially asymptomatic continue to manifest abnormalities in many of the neurobiological mechanisms that are impaired in acute depression. This means that it is not yet clear what neurobiological changes might be responsible for the acute experience of depression itself. The best current candidates in this respect probably remain the monoamine neurotransmitters because lowering of 5-HT and noradrenaline function can cause acute depression in people with a previous history of illness (see above). However, depletion studies in healthy subjects indicate that lowered monoamine function is not sufficient to cause depression and the variable response of depressed patients to monoamine potentiating drugs suggests that diminished monoamine function is probably not a necessary cause of depression either.

Current data suggest that recovered depressed patients have abnormalities in aspects of 5-HT neurotransmission and in the regulation of GABA activity. Ascending 5-HT pathways make important synaptic connection with GABA interneurones in cortical and limbic regions and GABA neurones in turn influence the activity of 5-HT cell bodies (Taylor et al. Reference Taylor, Bhagwagar, Cowen and Sharp2003). What might be the neuropsychological consequences of dysfunction in these neuronal networks?

GABA and 5-HT neurones are among the neuronal elements involved in the processing of emotional information. In the amygdala, for example, lowered GABA and 5-HT function would be expected to lead to increased excitability (Stutzman & LeDoux, Reference Stutzmann and LeDoux1999) and there is evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies that the amygdala is hyper-responsive to negative facial expressions both in acute depression (Sheline et al. Reference Sheline, Barch, Donnelly, Ollinger, Snyder and Mintun2001; Fu et al. Reference Fu, Williams, Cleare, Brammer, Walsh, Kim, Andrew, Williams, Reed, Mitterschiffthaler, Suckling and Bullmore2004) and in recovered unmedicated depressed patients (Neumeister et al. Reference Neumeister, Drevets, Belfer, Luckenbaugh, Henry, Bonne, Herscovitch, Goldman and Charney2006). Increased activity of the amygdala might be relevant to vulnerability to depression because it could facilitate automatic processing of negative emotional stimuli (Vuillemier, Reference Vuillemier2005). Consistent with this, we found that recovered depressed patients showed increased ability to detect fearful facial expressions and that this increased sensitivity was attenuated by a single intravenous dose of the SSRI citalopram (Bhagwagar et al. Reference Bhagwagar, Cowen, Goodwin and Harmer2004b). This is consistent with the effect of intravenous citalopram to lower amygdala reactivity to negative emotional stimuli in healthy controls (Del-Ben et al. Reference Del-Ben, Deakin, McKie, Delvai, Williams, Elliott, Dolan and Anderson2005). Attenuating the sensitivity of the amygdala to negative stimuli is a plausible mechanism of action for SSRIs in the prevention of depression in those at risk (Drevets, Reference Drevets2003) and, as noted above, persistent overactivity of the neural responses of the amygdala to negative stimuli might also be characteristic of recovered depressed patients (Neumeister et al. Reference Neumeister, Drevets, Belfer, Luckenbaugh, Henry, Bonne, Herscovitch, Goldman and Charney2006).

Taken together, the data suggest that many of the neurobiological abnormalities found in recovered depressed patients are associated with changes in the processing of emotional information and that in people at risk of recurrent depression the brain is in a state in which negative information is preferentially processed. Much appraisal of emotional information, for example that carried out by the amygdala, occurs at a non-conscious (automatic) level (Vuillemier, Reference Vuillemier2005). Thus, it is possible that excessive negative biases in emotional appraisal could be present in patients who do not subjectively experience depressive symptomatology. However, because of selective attention to negative stimuli, this processing style would be likely to put people at risk of more intense and prolonged negative emotions, particularly in adverse circumstances. This could lead in turn to the emergence and maintenance of depressed mood (Johnson-Laird et al. Reference Johnson-Laird, Mancini and Gangemi2006).

In this respect it is worth noting that recovered depressed patients undergoing ATD can briefly re-experience the totality of the depressive syndrome including, for example, physical symptoms as well as associated negative cognitions about the self and the world (Smith et al. Reference Smith, Fairburn and Cowen1997). This suggests that, in patients who have suffered several episodes of depression, relatively modest psychological or biological triggers can be sufficient to recruit many depressive phenomena into conscious awareness. Recurrent depression may therefore lead to the formation of an associative network in which sadness and anxiety can readily bring to mind depressive memories, beliefs and experiences.

This formulation gives importance to the neurobiological effects of the depressive experience itself in increasing vulnerability to future episodes of illness. From this viewpoint it will be important to establish which of the biological abnormalities we have described are present before the onset of illness and which apparently occur after the first few depressive episodes. It may be that, in the future, more therapeutic attention needs to be given to the biological consequences of acute depressive episodes, perhaps through the development of treatments with neuroprotective effects or those that might regulate the formation and recall of emotional memories (Lee et al. Reference Lee, Milton and Everitt2006).

Another issue for future studies is how far any of the abnormalities we have described here might be useful predictors of future prognosis. This work is difficult because it requires prospective follow-up studies. However, there is preliminary evidence that susceptibility to the mood-lowering effects of ATD may be of prognostic significance in patients with recurrent depression (Ruhé et al. Reference Ruhé, Mason and Schene2007). In addition, it appears that elevated cortisol secretion may have a role in predicting depression in high-risk individuals (Goodyer et al. Reference Goodyer, Herbert, Tamplin and Altham2000; Harris et al. Reference Harris, Borsanyi, Messari, Stanford, Cleary, Shiers, Brown and Herbert2000). Neurobiological markers, such as elevated cortisol, could help to guide early intervention studies in young people before the onset of recurrent illness. While the use of antidepressants in this group would not be appropriate, it is possible that, at this stage, other forms of intervention, for example psychological or nutritional therapies, might have a preventative role. This kind of approach might enable us eventually to decrease the long-term personal and societal burden of depression, which continues to grow despite the availability of reasonably effective symptomatic treatments.

Acknowledgements

Our studies were supported by the Medical Research Council.

Declaration of Interest

P.J.C. has served as a paid member of advisory boards for Eli Lilly, Lundbeck, Servier and Wyeth. Z.B. is a member of speakers' panels for Bristol–Myers Squibb, Astra Zeneca and Janssen and has received research support from Bristol–Myers Squibb.

References

Angst, J. (2000). Course and prognosis of mood disorders. In New Oxford Textbook of Psychiatry (ed. Gelder, M. G., Lopez-Ibor, J. J. and Andreasen, N. C.), pp. 719724. Oxford University Press: New York.Google Scholar
Berman, R. M., Narasimhan, M., Miller, H. L., Anand, A., Cappiello, A., Oren, D. A., Heninger, G. R. & Charney, D. S. (1999). Transient depressive relapse induced by catecholamine depletion: potential phenotypic vulnerability marker? Archives of General Psychiatry 56, 395403.Google Scholar
Bhagwagar, Z., Cowen, P. J., Goodwin, G. & Harmer, C. J. (2004 b). Normalization of enhanced fear recognition by acute SSRI treatment in subjects with a previous history of depression. American Journal of Psychiatry 161, 166168.Google Scholar
Bhagwagar, Z., Hafizi, S. & Cowen, P. J. (2003). Increase in concentration of waking salivary cortisol in recovered patients with depression. American Journal of Psychiatry 160, 18901891.Google Scholar
Bhagwagar, Z., Hafizi, S. & Cowen, P. J. (2005). Increased salivary cortisol after waking in depression. Psychopharmacology 182, 5457.Google Scholar
Bhagwagar, Z., Hinz, R., Taylor, M., Fancy, S., Cowen, P. J. & Grasby, P. M. (2006). Increased 5-HT2A receptor binding in euthymic, medication-free patients recovered from depression: a positron emission study with [11C]MDL 100,907. American Journal of Psychiatry 163, 15801587.Google Scholar
Bhagwagar, Z., Rabiner, E. A., Sargent, P. A., Grasby, P. M. & Cowen, P. J. (2004 a). Persistent reduction in brain serotonin1A receptor binding in recovered depressed men measured by positron emission tomography with [11C]WAY-100635. Molecular Psychiatry 9, 386392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bhagwagar, Z., Wyleszinska, M., Jezzard, P., Evans, J., Ashworth, F., Sule, A., Matthews, P. M. & Cowen, P. J. (2007). Reduction in occipital cortex γ-aminobutyric acid concentrations in medication-free recovered unipolar depressed and bipolar subjects. Biological Psychiatry 61, 808812.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bremner, J. D., Vythilingam, M., Ng, C. K., Vermetten, E., Nazeer, A., Oren, A., Berman, R. M. & Charney, D. S. (2003). Regional brain metabolic correlates of α-methylparatyrosine-induced depressive symptoms: implications for the neural circuitry of depression. Journal of the American Medical Association 289, 31253134.Google Scholar
Campbell, S. & MacQueen, G. (2004). The role of the hippocampus in the pathophysiology of major depression. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience 29, 417426.Google Scholar
Campbell, S. & MacQueen, G. (2006). An update on regional brain volume differences associated with mood disorders. Current Opinion in Psychiatry 19, 2533.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowen, P. J. (2005). Neurobiology of depression. In Mood Disorders: Clinical Management and Research Issues (ed. Griez, E. J., Faravelli, C., Nutt, D. J. and Zohar, J.), pp. 191210. Wiley: Oxford.Google Scholar
Del-Ben, C. M., Deakin, J. F. W., McKie, S., Delvai, N. S., Williams, S. R., Elliott, R., Dolan, M. & Anderson, I. M. (2005). The effect of citalopram pretreatment on neuronal responses to neuropsychological tasks in normal volunteers: an fMRI study. Neuropsychopharmacology 30, 17231734.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drevets, W. C. (2003). Neuroimaging abnormalities in the amygdala in mood disorder. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 985, 420444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drevets, W. C., Frank, E., Price, J. C., Kupfer, D. J., Holt, D., Greer, P. J., Huang, Y., Gautier, C. & Mathis, C. (1999). PET imaging of serotonin 1A receptor binding in depression. Biological Psychiatry 46, 13571387.Google Scholar
Fisher, P. M., Meltzer, C. C., Ziolko, S. K., Price, J. C. & Harari, A. R. (2006). Capacity for 5-HT1A-mediated autoregulation predicts amygdala reactivity. Nature Neuroscience 9, 13621363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flint, J. & Munafò, M. R. (2007). The endophenotype concept in psychiatric genetics. Psychological Medicine 37, 163180.Google Scholar
Fu, C. H. Y., Williams, S. C. R., Cleare, A. J., Brammer, M. J., Walsh, M. D., Kim, J., Andrew, C. M., Williams, P. M., Reed, L. J., Mitterschiffthaler, M. T., Suckling, J. & Bullmore, E. T. (2004). Attenuation of the neural response to sad faces in major depression by antidepressant treatment: a prospective, event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Archives of General Psychiatry 61, 877889.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giles, D. E., Jarrett, R. B., Rush, A. J. & Roffwarg, H. P. (1993). Prospective assessment of electroencephalographic sleep in remitted major depression. Psychiatry Research 46, 269284.Google Scholar
Giles, D. E., Kupfer, D. J., Rush, A. J. & Roffwarg, H. P. (1998). Controlled comparison of electrophysiological sleep in families of probands with unipolar depression. American Journal of Psychiatry 155, 192199.Google Scholar
Goodyer, I. M., Herbert, J., Tamplin, A. & Altham, P. M. (2000). Recent life events, cortisol, dehydroepiandrosterone and the onset of major depression in high-risk adolescents. British Journal of Psychiatry 177, 499504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halligan, S. L., Herbert, J., Goodyer, I. & Murray, L. (2007). Disturbances in morning cortisol secretion in association with maternal postnatal depression predict subsequent depressive symptomatology in adolescents. Biological Psychiatry 62, 4046.Google Scholar
Harris, T. O., Borsanyi, S., Messari, S., Stanford, K., Cleary, S. E., Shiers, H. M., Brown, G. W. & Herbert, J. (2000). Morning cortisol as a risk factor for subsequent major depressive disorder in adult women. British Journal of Psychiatry 177, 505510.Google Scholar
Harrison, P. J. (2002). The neuropathology of primary mood disorder. Brain 125, 14281449.Google Scholar
Hasler, G., Drevets, W. C., Manji, H. K. & Charney, D. S. (2004). Discovering endophenotypes for major depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 29, 17651781.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holsboer, F. (2000). The corticosteroid receptor hypothesis of depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 23, 477501.Google Scholar
Holsboer, F., Lauer, C. J., Schreiber, W. & Krieg, J.-C. (1995). Altered hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical regulation in healthy subjects at high familial risk for affective disorders. Neuroendocrinology 62, 340347.Google Scholar
Ising, M., Lauer, C. J., Holsboer, F. & Modell, S. (2005). The Munich vulnerability study on affective disorders: premorbid neuroendocrine profile of affected high-risk probands. Journal of Psychiatric Research 39, 2128.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N., Mancini, F. & Gangemi, A. (2006). A hyper-emotion theory of psychological illness. Psychological Reviews 113, 822841.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Thorton, L. M. & Gardner, C. O. (2001). Genetic risk, number of previous depressive episodes, and stressful life events in predicting onset of major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry 158, 582586.Google Scholar
Lee, A. S. & Murray, R. M. (1988). The long-term outcome of Maudsley depressives. British Journal of Psychiatry 153, 741751.Google Scholar
Lee, J. L., Milton, A. L. & Everitt, B. J. (2006). Reconsolidation and extinction of conditioned fear: inhibition and potentiation. Journal of Neuroscience 26, 1005110056.Google Scholar
Mannie, Z. N., Harmer, C. J. & Cowen, P. J. (2007). Increased waking salivary cortisol in young people at increased familial risk of depression. American Journal of Psychiatry 164, 617621.Google Scholar
Martin-Ruiz, R., Puig, M. V., Celada, P., Shapiro, D. A., Roth, B. L., Mengod, G. & Artigas, F. (2001). Control of serotonergic function in medial prefrontal cortex by serotonin-2A receptors through a glutamate-dependent mechanism. Journal of Neuroscience 21, 98569866.Google Scholar
McTavish, S. F. B., Cowen, P. J. & Sharp, T. (1996). Effect of tyrosine-free amino acid mixture on regional brain catecholamine synthesis and release. Psychopharmacology 141, 182188.Google Scholar
McTavish, S. F. B., Mannie, Z. N., Harmer, C. J. & Cowen, P. J. (2005). Lack of effect of tyrosine depletion on mood in recovered depressed women. Neuropsychopharmacology 30, 786911.Google Scholar
Meyer, J., McMain, S., Kennedy, S. H., Korman, L., Brown, G. M., DaSilva, J. N. & Wilson, A. A. (2003). Dysfunctional attitudes and 5-HT receptors during depression and self harm. American Journal of Psychiatry 160, 6090.Google Scholar
Mintun, M. A., Sheline, Y. I., Moerlein, S. M., Vlassenko, A. G., Huang, Y. & Snyder, A. Z. (2004). Decreased hippocampal 5-HT receptor binding in major depressive disorder: in vivo measurement with [F]altansarin positron emission tomography. Biological Psychiatry 55, 217224.Google Scholar
Modell, S., Ising, M., Holsboer, F. & Lauer, C. J. (2005). The Munich vulnerability study on affective disorders: premorbid polysomnographic profile of affected high-risk probands. Biological Psychiatry 58, 694699.Google Scholar
Mueller, T. I., Leon, A. C., Keller, M. B., Solomon, D. A., Endicott, J., Corywell, W., Warshaw, M. & Maser, J. D. (1999). Recurrence after recovery from major depressive disorder during 15 years of observational follow-up. American Journal of Psychiatry 156, 10001006.Google Scholar
Neumeister, A., Drevets, W. C., Belfer, I., Luckenbaugh, D. A., Henry, S., Bonne, O., Herscovitch, P., Goldman, D. & Charney, D. S. (2006). Effects of α2C-adrenoreceptor gene polymorphism on neural responses to facial expressions in depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 31, 17501756.Google Scholar
Neumeister, A., Nugent, A. C., Waldeck, T., Geraci, M., Schwarz, M., Bonne, O., Bain, E. E., Luckenbaugh, D. A., Herscovitch, P., Charney, D. S. & Drevets, W. C. (2004). Neural and behavioral responses to tryptophan depletion in unmedicated patients with remitted major depressive disorder and controls. Archives of General Psychiatry 61, 765773.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips, M. L., Drevets, W. C., Rauch, S. L. & Lane, R. (2003). Neurobiology of emotion perception I: The neural basis of normal emotion perception. Biological Psychiatry 54, 504514.Google Scholar
Piquet, P. & Galvan, M. (1994). Transient and long-lasting actions of 5-HT on rat dentate gyrus neurones in vitro. Journal of Physiology 481, 629639.Google Scholar
Pruessner, J. C., Wolf, O. T., Hellhammer, D. H., Buske-Kirschbaum, A., von-Auer, K., Jobst, S. & Kirschbaum, C. (1997). Free cortisol levels after awakening: a reliable biological marker for the assessment of adrenocorticol activity. Life Sciences 61, 25392549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramboz, S., Oosting, R., Amara, D. A., Kung, H. F., Blier, P., Mendelsohn, M., Mann, J. J. & Brunner, D. (1998). Serotonin receptor 1A knockout: an animal model of anxiety-related disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 95, 1447614481.Google Scholar
Ruhé, H. G., Mason, N. S. & Schene, A. H. (2007). Mood is indirectly related to serotonin, norepinephrine and dopamine levels in humans: a meta-analysis of monoamine depletion studies. Molecular Psychiatry 12, 331359.Google Scholar
Sanacora, G., Fenton, L. R., Fasula, M. K., Rothman, D. L., Levin, Y., Krystal, J. H. & Mason, G. F. (2006). Cortical γ-aminobutyric acid concentrations in depressed patients receiving cognitive behavioral therapy. Biological Psychiatry 59, 284286.Google Scholar
Sanacora, G., Gueorguiva, R., Epperson, C. N., Wu, Y. T., Appel, M. & Rothman, D. L. (2004). Subtype-specific alterations of γ-aminobutyric acid and glutamate in patients with major depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 61, 705713.Google Scholar
Sanacora, G., Mason, G. F., Rothman, D. L., Hyder, F., Ciarcia, J. J. & Ostroff, R. B. (2003). Increased cortical GABA concentrations in depressed patients receiving ECT. American Journal of Psychiatry 160, 577579.Google Scholar
Sanacora, G., Mason, G. F., Rothman, D. L. & Krystal, J. H. (2002). Increased occipital cortex GABA concentrations in depressed patients after therapy with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors. American Journal of Psychiatry 159, 663665.Google Scholar
Sargent, P. A., Kjaer, K. A., Bench, C. J., Rabiner, E. A., Messa, C., Meyer, J., Gunn, R. N., Grasby, P. M. & Cowen, P. J. (2000). Brain serotonin (1A) receptor binding measured by positron emission tomography with [11C]WAY-100635: effects of depression and antidepressant treatment. Archives of General Psychiatry 57, 174180.Google Scholar
Sheline, Y. I., Barch, D. M., Donnelly, J. M., Ollinger, J. M., Snyder, A. Z. & Mintun, M. A. (2001). Increased amygdala response to masked emotional faces in depressed subjects resolves with antidepressant treatment: an fMRI study. Biological Psychiatry 50, 651658.Google Scholar
Sherwood, B., Varghese, F. P. & McEwen, B. S. (2004). Association of depression with medical illness: does cortisol play a role? Biological Psychiatry 55, 19.Google Scholar
Smith, K. A., Fairburn, C. G. & Cowen, P. J. (1997). Relapse of depression after rapid depletion of tryptophan. Lancet 349, 915919.Google Scholar
Smith, K. A., Morris, J. S., Friston, K. J., Cowen, P. J. & Dolan, R. J. (1999). Brain mechanisms associated with depressive relapse and associated cognitive impairment following acute tryptophan depletion. British Journal of Psychiatry 174, 525529.Google Scholar
Stutzmann, G. E. & LeDoux, J. E. (1999). Gabaergic antagonists block the inhibitory effects of serotonin in the lateral amygdala: a mechanism for modulation of sensory inputs related to fear conditioning. Journal of Neuroscience RC8, 14.Google Scholar
Styron, W. (2004). Darkness Visible. Vintage Classics: London.Google Scholar
Taylor, M., Bhagwagar, Z., Cowen, P. J. & Sharp, T. (2003). GABA and mood disorders. Psychological Medicine 33, 387393.Google Scholar
Vuillemier, P. (2005). How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9, 585594.Google Scholar
Zobel, A. W., Nickel, T., Sonntag, A., Uhr, M., Holsboer, F. I. & Sing, M. (2001). Cortisol response in the combined dexamethasone/CRH test as predictor of relapse in patients with remitted depression: a prospective study. Journal of Psychiatric Research 35, 8394.Google Scholar